1. Introduction
The study of mindfulness and mental health has flourished in psychological science over the recent decade [
1,
2,
3,
4]. According to [
5], mindfulness refers to the arising awareness of paying attention to the present moment, without judgment, and on purpose. In mindfulness-to-meaning theory, [
1] postulates that mindfulness is associated with psychological well-being and meaning in life through a spiral of processes, including reduced habitual and autopilot reactions to stress, greater cognitive reappraisal, and greater savoring of positive experiences. Recent studies have also suggested that trait mindfulness and mindfulness-based practices are associated with fewer symptoms of depression and anxiety [
6,
7,
8,
9,
10]. Thus, the relations between mindfulness, psychological distress, and mental well-being have been well-established in the literature.
1.1. Mindfulness and Depressive Symptoms
While ample evidence suggests that mindfulness is beneficial to psychological health [
6,
7,
8,
9,
10], a handful of studies have also shown that psychological health, as reflected by fewer depressive symptoms, is predictive of mindfulness [
11,
12,
13]. Nevertheless, these findings appear to be inconsistent, especially on the bidirectional relation between mindfulness and depressive symptoms. In terms of significant findings, a study showed that depression severity was related to a lower level of mindfulness, and that mindfulness mediated the longitudinal relation between the severity of depression and problematic smartphone use [
11]. However, another study indicated that mindfulness and depressive symptoms did not predict one another from baseline to 3-month follow-up, but did show predictions between one another from 3-month to 6-month follow-up [
13]. In a third study [
12], depressive symptoms consistently predicted mindfulness facets including nonjudging and nonreactivity to inner experience from baseline to 6-month follow-up and from 6-month to 12-month follow-up. Though depressive symptoms also predicted describing from baseline to 6-month follow-up, they did not predict the rest of the facets including observing and acting with awareness over time. As for the predictability of mindfulness on depressive symptoms, a significant effect was found for acting with awareness, but not other mindfulness facets, on depressive symptoms from 6-month to 12-month follow-up (see also [
14] for a meta-analytic review). As such, while depressive symptoms appear to be crucial for the worsening of some mindfulness facets, mindfulness facets are not always predictive of worsened depression in cross-lagged models. Given the mixed findings in the literature, the present study aims to disentangle the longitudinal relation between mindfulness and depressive symptoms among emerging adults.
1.2. Mindfulness and Savoring Positive Experiences
Consistent with mindfulness-to-meaning theory, recent findings have demonstrated that mindfulness is associated with a greater savoring of positive experiences [
15,
16,
17]. Savoring refers to people’s propensity to be appreciative of the past (reminiscing), the present (savoring the moment), and the future (anticipation) [
18,
19]. Indeed, mindfulness is central to savoring, particularly to savoring the moment, as an awareness of the present is essential for the appreciation of it [
20]. Not surprisingly, behavioral interventions have integrated components of mindfulness, savoring, and cognitive reappraisal techniques to reduce autopilot and foster behavior change [
21,
22,
23,
24,
25].
While previous studies have demonstrated a direct association between mindfulness and savoring positive experiences, most of them are cross-sectional [
3,
26]. The studies that are longitudinal, however, did not examine the link between mindfulness and specific types of savoring, such as anticipation, savoring the moment, and reminiscing [
17,
27,
28]. In terms of directionality of effects, even though mindfulness and mindful decentering foster savoring [
20,
28], savoring the present moment may serve as a reward for people to be more mindful of the here and now. As such, this study aims to unravel the longitudinal relation between mindfulness and different types of savoring, including anticipation, savoring the moment, and reminiscing.
1.3. Savoring Positive Experiences and Depressive Symptoms
The association between savoring and psychological distress has also been well-established in the literature [
29,
30,
31], with a majority of the findings showing the prediction of savoring on lower distress, including depressive symptoms [
3,
32,
33]. For instance, in a sample of older adults, a savoring intervention was found to reduce depression and negative affect, and increase life satisfaction, subjective happiness, resilience, and positive affect [
34]. Similarly, another study involving older adults showed that people who completed a savoring intervention with high fidelity had a greater reduction of depressive symptoms and greater improvements in happiness and resilience [
35]. In a cross-sectional study involving undergraduate students, savoring, mindfulness, and self-compassion all mediated the relation between social support and depressive symptoms [
3]. Interestingly, when the types of savoring were distinguished, only savoring the moment, but not anticipation or reminiscing, mediated the cross-sectional relation between positive and negative affectivity and depressive symptoms among undergraduate students [
36].
In terms of the opposite directionality of effects, a handful of studies have also suggested that people’s health, including fewer depressive symptoms and better perceived health, were predictive of savoring [
28,
37,
38]. However, the findings were somewhat mixed. For example, in a sample of parents of 8- to 12-year-old children, parental depressive symptoms longitudinally predicted lower relational savoring, i.e., parents’ ability to attend to, re-experience, recall, and process instances of positive connection with the child. Relational savoring was further associated with greater children’s physiological regulation [
39]. As such, parental depression has direct implications for parents’ relational savoring as well as child adjustment. In another study involving community-dwelling older adults, perceived health at baseline predicted anticipation and reminiscing, but not savoring the moment, 2.5 years later [
37]. Hence, the predictive effects of health on savoring varied, depending on the type of savoring. To further illuminate the directionality of effects between savoring and depressive symptoms, the present study tested the longitudinal relations between different types of savoring and depressive symptoms among emerging adults.
1.4. The Present Study
Situated in the developmental period of emerging adulthood, the present study examined cross-lagged relations between mindfulness, savoring, and depressive symptoms in a Chinese sample from Hong Kong. Emerging adulthood is a transition period in between adolescents and adulthood, characterized by identity explorations, instability, and possibilities [
40,
41]. Of note, people at this developmental period are prone to mental health problems including depression (e.g., [
41,
42]). As a result, identifying mental health correlates at this period is tremendously important. Based on the literature, we hypothesized that mindfulness would predict greater savoring, including reminiscing, savoring the moment, and anticipation, as well as fewer depressive symptoms. Next, we also hypothesized that savoring would predict fewer depressive symptoms. Finally, we hypothesized that depressive symptoms would predict lower levels of mindfulness and savoring. However, we did not set an a priori hypothesis for the effects of savoring on mindfulness, given the lack of evidence in the literature.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
One hundred and eighty Chinese university students at 18-27 years old (M =21.12, SD = 2.02; 89.44% female) were recruited via online platforms and mass emailing. The retention rate was 92.78% (nT3 = 167) from Time 1 (T1) to Time 2 (T2), with each time spanning three months apart. The present study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of The Education University of Hong Kong. Informed consent was sought prior to the administration of the study. Upon completion of all time points, participants received HK$200.00 supermarket coupons (~US$25.64) as a compensation.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Mindfulness
The 10-item Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R; [
43]) was used to assess mindfulness. Sample items included “It is easy for me to concentrate on what I am doing.” and “I am able to accept the thoughts and feelings I have.” Participants rated on a 4-point scale from 1 (
rarely/not at all) to 4 (
almost always). The items were averaged to form a composite score of mindfulness, with higher scores indicating a greater level of mindfulness. The CAMS-R was validated previously in a Chinese community sample [
44]. Cronbach’s alpha = .86 at T1 and .89 at T2.
2.2.2. Savoring
The 24-item Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI; [
45]) was used to measure savoring positive experiences on a 5-point scale from 1 (
strongly disagree) to 5 (
strongly agree). The SBI has three subscales including Anticipation, Savoring the Moment, and Reminiscing. The 8-item Anticipation subscale included sample items such as, “I feel a joy of anticipation when I think about upcoming good things” and “For me, anticipating what upcoming good events will be like is basically a waste of time.” The item scores were averaged to form a composite score of anticipation, with higher scores indicating greater anticipation. The 8-item Savoring the Moment subscale included sample items such as, “I know how to make the most of good time.” and “I feel fully able to appreciate good things that happen to me.” The item scores were averaged to form a composite score of savoring the moment, with higher scores indicating greater present focus. The 8-item Reminiscing subscale included sample items such as, “I can make myself feel good by remembering pleasant events from my past.” and “I like to store memories of fun times that I go through so that I can recall them later.” The item scores were averaged to form a composite score of reminiscing, with higher scores indicating greater reminiscing. The measures had been used with Chinese samples with adequate reliability [
46]. Cronbach’s alpha = .77 at T1 and .81 at T2 for Anticipation, .81 at T1 and .83 at T2 for Savoring the Moment, and .83 at T1 and .80 for Reminiscing.
2.2.3. Depressive Symptoms
The 10-item Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9; [
47]) was used to measure severity of depression over the past two weeks on a 4-point scale from 0 (
not at all) to 3 (
nearly every day). Sample items included, “Feeling down, depressed, or hopeless” and “Poor appetite or overeating” The item scores were summed to form a composite score of depressive symptoms, with higher scores indicating higher severity. The measure has previously been validated in a Chinese community sample ([
48]). The measure demonstrated adequate internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha = .89 and .91 at T2.
2.3. Data Analysis
Correlations, means, and standard deviations were examined in the preliminary analyses. A cross-lagged panel model using MPLUS, Version 8 [
49] was conducted to investigate the changes in mindfulness, anticipation, savoring the moment, reminiscing, and depressive symptoms respectively. The changes in variables were modeled by the stability coefficients between time-adjacent measures, e.g., T1 mindfulness was used to predict Time 2 (T2) mindfulness. Cross-lagged associations was modeled by predictions between the variables, e.g., T1 mindfulness was used to predict T2 depressive symptoms. Residual variance was allowed to covary to account for shared measurement variance. Sex, age, and monthly family income were included as covariates for all variables under study. Maximum likelihood method was used to investigate the model fit to the observed matrices of variance and covariance. Full information maximum likelihood estimation was used to handle missing data.
3. Results
Table 1 shows the zero-order correlations, means, and standard deviations.
In testing the cross-lagged analysis, the path model fit adequately to the data, χ
2(8) = 8.74, p = .36, CFI = 1.00, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .02 (see
Figure 1).
The core findings indicated that T1 savoring the moment positively predicted T2 mindfulness (β = .25, p = .02). T1 depressive symptoms negatively predicted T2 mindfulness (β = -.19, p = .01) and T2 savoring the moment (β = -.17, p = .02). Autoregressive control variables of mindfulness, anticipation, savoring the moment, reminiscing, and depressive symptoms were significant, ps < .001 (see
Table 2 for details).
4. Discussion
The present study has broadened the literature by disentangling the effects between mindfulness, savoring, and depressive symptoms among emerging adults. Importantly, the longitudinal findings lent support to the significance of savoring the moment in enhancing mindfulness. They also indicated that depressive symptoms were crucial for diminishing mindfulness and savoring the moment. Contrary to previous research indicating that mindfulness and savoring are both beneficial to psychological functioning [
1,
22,
23,
24], the cross-lagged analysis revealed, instead, an opposite directionality of effect, after controlling for age, gender, and family income. Consequently, the findings brough forth unique implications for preventions and interventions in emerging adulthood.
Somewhat surprisingly, mindfulness did not predict different types of savoring. The null findings contradicted previous research showing a positive effect of mindfulness on savoring in general [
17,
50]. Conversely, savoring the present moment, but not anticipation or reminiscing, was predictive of greater mindfulness. While this finding is new to the literature, it is consistent with our speculation that savoring the present moment can serve as a reward for people to be more mindful of the here and now. To add specificity to the relation between mindfulness and savoring, or the lack thereof, researchers should recruit a larger sample to conduct cross-lagged analyses between different types of savoring and mindfulness facets, including nonjudging and nonreactivity to inner experience, describing, observing, and acting with awareness.
Unique to the present findings is that depressive symptoms were predictive of mindfulness and savoring the moment, over and above autoregressive effects, age, gender, and family income. These findings partially agreed with previous research showing that depressive symptoms were predictive of some, but not all, facets of mindfulness among adolescents [
12]. Future research is, again, necessary to examine the longitudinal associations between depression and mindfulness facets across developmental periods. As for savoring, the present findings contradicted another study involving community-dwelling older adults, which showed that perceived health at baseline predicted anticipation and reminiscing, but not savoring the moment, 2.5 years later [
37]. Given that the present research examined depressive symptoms as a predictor (instead of general health) during emerging adulthood (instead of older adulthood) at a 3-month time lag (instead of a 2.5-year lag), the discrepancies between the findings might have been due to a range of variables. Hence, further studies are necessary to identify the moderators between health and savoring.
In contrast to previous studies [
2,
3,
4,
36], neither mindfulness nor savoring predicted depressive symptoms in the cross-lagged model. The null findings were surprising, as they contradicted intervention studies showing the benefits of mindfulness [
51,
52,
53] and savoring [
34,
35] in reducing depressive symptoms. Given that depressive symptoms are stable over a three-month period [
54], the null findings might have been due to the stability of depression over a short period of time. Hence, future studies are necessary to replicate the present study by increasing the lag between time points.
4.1. Limitations and Future Directions
The present findings must be interpreted in light of several limitations that point to future research directions. First of all, the present study included self-report measures to assess the variables of interest. To reduce self-report bias, future research should employ a multi-method, multi-reporter approach. Second, a majority of the participants were female (89.44%). As such, researchers should collect a more gender-balanced sample to further investigate gender effect. Third, the time lag was short between assessment points at three months apart. To examine the long-term effects among these variables, researchers should consider increasing the lag to 6 or 12 months. They should also collect additional data for multiple time points to examine the trajectories of mindfulness, savoring, and depressive symptoms.
5. Implications for Interventions and Conclusion
Ample evidence to date has suggested that interventions involving mindfulness and savoring are beneficial to psychological functioning [
1,
22,
23,
24,
32,
34,
51,
52]. Through cross-lagged analysis, the present findings showed that a lower level of depression can also foster greater mindfulness and savoring the moment among emerging adults. The major implication is that upon reductions of depression severity, people are likely to also experience a greater awareness in paying attention to and appreciating the present moment, without judgment, and on purpose. Given that the study involved a Chinese sample residing in Hong Kong, the findings are particularly relevant to emerging adults in the Chinese context. Taken together, the present study informs clinicians and practitioners the importance of lowering the severity of depression, such that people can have a greater capacity for present-moment awareness and appreciation.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung; methodology, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung; formal analysis, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung and Melody C. Y. Ng; investigation, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung and Melody C. Y. Ng; writing—original draft preparation, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung and Melody C. Y. Ng; writing—review and editing, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung; supervision, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung; funding acquisition, Rebecca Y. M. Cheung All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was funded by The Education University of Hong Kong [RG60/2014-2015R and #04190].
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of The Education University of Hong Kong (Approval #: 2015-2016-0352) prior to its implementation. All procedures were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to ethical constraints.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the participants for taking the time to participate in this study.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- Garland, E.L.; Farb, N.A.; R Goldin, P.; Fredrickson, B.L. Mindfulness broadens awareness and builds eudaimonic meaning: A process model of mindful positive emotion regulation. Psychological Inquiry 2015, 26, 293–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parmentier, F.B.; García-Toro, M.; García-Campayo, J.; Yañez A, Ñ.; Andrés, P.; Gili-Planas, M. Mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety in the general population: The mediating roles of worry, rumination, reappraisal and suppression. Frontiers in psychology 2019, 10, 506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, J.M.; Weiss, A.; Shook, N.J. Mindfulness, self-compassion, and savoring: Factors that explain the relation between perceived social support and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences 2020, 152, 109568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung RY, M.; Ng MC, Y. Mindfulness and symptoms of depression and anxiety: The underlying roles of awareness, acceptance, impulse control, and emotion regulation. Mindfulness 2019, 10, 1124–1135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kabat-Zinn, J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 2003, 10, 144–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barcaccia, B.; Hartstone, J.M.; Pallini, S.; Petrocchi, N.; Saliani, A.M.; Medvedev, O.N. Mindfulness, social safeness and self-reassurance as protective factors and self-criticism and revenge as risk factors for depression and anxiety symptoms in youth. Mindfulness 2022, 13, 674–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barnes, S.M.; Lynn, S.J. Mindfulness skills and depressive symptoms: A longitudinal study. Imagination, Cognition and Personality 2010, 30, 77–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung RY, M.; Chan SK, C.; Chui, H.; Chan, W.M.; Ngai SY, S. Enhancing parental self-care: Initial efficacy of a 21-day online self-help mindfulness-based intervention for parents. Mindfulness 2022, 13, 2812–2826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parsons, D.; Gardner, P.; Parry, S.; Smart, S. Mindfulness-based approaches for managing stress, anxiety and depression for health students in tertiary education: a scoping review. Mindfulness 2022, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sharma, P.K.; Kumra, R. Relationship between mindfulness, depression, anxiety and stress: Mediating role of self-efficacy. Personality and Individual Differences 2022, 186, 111363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elhai, J.D.; Levine, J.C.; O’Brien, K.D.; Armour, C. Distress tolerance and mindfulness mediate relations between depression and anxiety sensitivity with problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior 2018, 84, 477–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gómez-Odriozola, J.; Calvete, E. Longitudinal bidirectional associations between dispositional mindfulness, maladaptive schemas, and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Mindfulness 2020, 11, 1943–1955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jury, T.K.; Jose, P.E. Does rumination function as a longitudinal mediator between mindfulness and depression? Mindfulness 2019, 10, 1091–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prieto-Fidalgo, Á.; Gómez-Odriozola, J.; Royuela-Colomer, E.; Orue, I.; Fernández-González, L.; Oñate, L.; Calvete, E.; et al. Predictive Associations of Dispositional Mindfulness Facets with Anxiety and Depression: a Meta-analytic Structural Equation Modeling Approach. Mindfulness 2022, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryant, F.B.; Smith, J.L. Appreciating life in the midst of adversity: Savoring in relation to mindfulness, reappraisal, and meaning. Psychological Inquiry 2015, 26, 315–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ritchie, T.D.; Bryant, F.B. Positive state mindfulness: A multidimensional model of mindfulness in relation to positive experience. International Journal of Wellbeing 2012, 2, 150–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung RY, M.; Ng MC, Y. Dispositional mindful awareness and savoring positive experiences: A prospective test of cognitive reappraisal as a mediator. Personality and Individual Differences 2020, 163, 110050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryant, F.B. A four-factor model of perceived control: Avoiding, coping, obtaining, and savoring. Journal of personality 1989, 57, 773–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryant, F.B.; Veroff, J. Savoring: A new model of positive experience.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Bryant, F.B. Current progress and future directions for theory and research on savoring. Frontiers in Psychology 2021, 12, 771698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garland, E.L.; Froeliger, B.; Howard, M.O. Mindfulness training targets neurocognitive mechanisms of addiction at the attention-appraisal-emotion interface. Frontiers in psychiatry 2014, 4, 173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garland, E.L.; Atchley, R.M.; Hanley, A.W.; Zubieta, J.K.; Froeliger, B. Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement remediates hedonic dysregulation in opioid users: Neural and affective evidence of target engagement. Science advances 2019, 5, eaax1569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hunt, C.; Lerman, S.F.; Smith, K.; Keaser, M.L.; Bingham, C.; Zeidan, F.; Finan, P.H.; et al. Brief Training in Savoring Meditation Enhances Non-dual Awareness in Rheumatoid Arthritis Patients. The Journal of Pain 2022, 23, 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, W.; Garland, E.L.; McGovern, P.; O'brien, J.E.; Tronnier, C.; Howard, M.O. Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for internet gaming disorder in US adults: A stage I randomized controlled trial. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 2017, 31, 393–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thomas, E.A.; Mijangos, J.L.; Hansen, P.A.; White, S.; Walker, D.; Reimers, C.; Garland, E.L.; et al. Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement restructures reward processing and promotes interoceptive awareness in overweight Cancer survivors: mechanistic results from a stage 1 randomized controlled trial. Integrative Cancer Therapies 2019, 18, 1534735419855138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cheung RY, M.; Lau EN, S. Is mindfulness linked to life satisfaction? Testing savoring positive experiences and gratitude as mediators. Frontiers in psychology 2021, 12, 591103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Götmann, A.; Bechtoldt, M.N. Coping with COVID-19–Longitudinal analysis of coping strategies and the role of trait mindfulness in mental well-being. Personality and Individual Differences 2021, 175, 110695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sgherza, T.R.; DeMarree, K.G.; Naragon-Gainey, K. Testing the Mindfulness-to-Meaning Theory in Daily Life. Mindfulness 2022, 13, 2324–2336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cheung, R.Y.; Leung, M.C.; Chiu, H.T.; Kwan, J.L.; Yee, L.T.; Hou, W.K. Family functioning and psychological outcomes in emerging adulthood: Savoring positive experiences as a mediating mechanism. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 2019, 36, 2693–2713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chiu, H.T.; Yee LT, S.; Kwan JL, Y.; Cheung RY, M.; Hou, W.K. Interactive association between negative emotion regulation and savoring is linked to anxiety symptoms among college students. Journal of American College Health 2020, 68, 494–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Irvin, K.M.; Bell, D.J.; Steinley, D.; Bartholow, B.D. The thrill of victory: Savoring positive affect, psychophysiological reward processing, and symptoms of depression. Emotion 2020, 22, 1281–1293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hurley, D.B.; Kwon, P. Results of a study to increase savoring the moment: Differential impact on positive and negative outcomes. Journal of Happiness Studies 2012, 13, 579–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, J.L.; Hollinger-Smith, L. Savoring, resilience, and psychological well-being in older adults. Aging & mental health 2015, 19, 192–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salces-Cubero, I.M.; Ramírez-Fernández, E.; Ortega-Martínez, A.R. Strengths in older adults: Differential effect of savoring, gratitude and optimism on well-being. Aging & Mental Health 2019, 23, 1017–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, J.L.; Hanni, A.A. Effects of a savoring intervention on resilience and well-being of older adults. Journal of Applied Gerontology 2019, 38, 137–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahrilas, I.J.; Smith, J.L.; Silton, R.L.; Bryant, F.B. Savoring the moment: A link between affectivity and depression. International Journal of Wellbeing 2020, 10, 19–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geiger, P.J.; Morey, J.N.; Segerstrom, S.C. Beliefs about savoring in older adulthood: Aging and perceived health affect temporal components of perceived savoring ability. Personality and Individual Differences 2017, 105, 164–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Palmer, C.A.; Oosterhoff, B.; Gentzler, A.L. Don’t worry, be happy: associations between worry and positive emotion regulation. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy 2021, 14, 417–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Borelli, J.L.; Hong, K.; Kazmierski, K.F.; Smiley, P.A.; Sohn, L.; Guo, Y. Parents’ depressive symptoms and reflective functioning predict parents’ proficiency in relational savoring and children’s physiological regulation. Development and Psychopathology 2022, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arnett, J.J. Emerging adulthood: What is it, and what is it good for? Child development perspectives 2007, 1, 68–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riggs, S.A.; Han, G. Predictors of anxiety and depression in emerging adulthood. Journal of adult development 2009, 16, 39–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuwabara, S.A.; Van Voorhees, B.W.; Gollan, J.K.; Alexander, G.C. A qualitative exploration of depression in emerging adulthood: Disorder, development, and social context. General hospital psychiatry 2007, 29, 317–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Feldman, G.; Hayes, A.; Kumar, S.; Greeson, J.; Laurenceau, J.P. Mindfulness and emotion regulation: The development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 2007, 29, 177–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, H.L.; Lo, L.Y.; Lin, M.; Thompson, N. Revalidation of the cognitive and affective mindfulness scale—Revised (CAMS-R) with its newly developed Chinese version (Ch-CAMS-R). Journal of pacific rim psychology 2016, 10, e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryant, F. Savoring Beliefs Inventory (SBI): A scale for measuring beliefs about savouring. Journal of mental health 2003, 12, 175–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, C.W.; Chen, S.L.; Wang, R.Y. Savouring and perceived job performance in positive psychology: Moderating role of positive affectivity. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 2011, 14, 165–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroenke, K.; Spitzer, R.L.; Williams, J.B. The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of general internal medicine 2001, 16, 606–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xiong, N.; Fritzsche, K.; Wei, J.; Hong, X.; Leonhart, R.; Zhao, X.; Fischer, F.; et al. Validation of patient health questionnaire (PHQ) for major depression in Chinese outpatients with multiple somatic symptoms: a multicenter cross-sectional study. Journal of affective disorders 2015, 174, 636–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th ed.; Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Garland, E.L.; Thielking, P.; Thomas, E.A.; Coombs, M.; White, S.; Lombardi, J.; Beck, A. Linking dispositional mindfulness and positive psychological processes in cancer survivorship: a multivariate path analytic test of the mindfulness-to-meaning theory. Psycho-oncology 2017, 26, 686–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuyken, W.; Byford, S.; Taylor, R.S.; Watkins, E.; Holden, E.; White, K.; Teasdale, J.D.; et al. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent relapse in recurrent depression. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology 2008, 76, 966–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shahar, B.; Britton, W.B.; Sbarra, D.A.; Figueredo, A.J.; Bootzin, R.R. Mechanisms of change in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: Preliminary evidence from a randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy 2010, 3, 402–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Teasdale, J.D.; Segal, Z.V.; Williams JM, G.; Ridgeway, V.A.; Soulsby, J.M.; Lau, M.A. Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology 2000, 68, 615–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wong, J.L.; Whitaker, D.J. The stability and prediction of depressive mood states in college students. Journal of Clinical Psychology 1994, 50, 715–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).