1. Introduction
The distribution of attention and mental representation of space seem asymmetrical, as demonstrated by biases displayed by healthy subjects in attentional tasks using a wide range of stimuli.
The most known attentional bias is pseudoneglect, initially described as a leftward bias in the bisection of physical lines [
1,
2,
3]. Indeed, pseudoneglect is observed in different domains. In number comparison tasks, healthy subjects overestimate the difference between a middle number and an outer number positioned on its left side [
3,
4]. Healthy participants show a leftward bias even when they are shown three-letter strings and are asked to estimate which of the two flankers (e.g., C and P) has a greater alphabetical distance from the inner letter (H) [
5,
6]
It remains to be seen whether there may be attentional biases also for linguistic domains without explicit left-to-right representation. Studies investigating the processing of letters that needed to be organised alphabetically have yielded conflicting results. In letter line bisection tasks, some studies documented a bias towards the left hemispace [
7], whilst others documented a bias towards the right hemispace [
8].
Mohr and Leonard [
9] investigated the impact of semantic information on letter line bisection in healthy subjects. The authors used letter lines with embedded words that were either emotional (e.g., eucsoiaad
killfp) or neutral (e.g., heaiineb
mainul). A stronger rightward bisection bias for letter lines containing emotional words was documented. The authors suggested that the semantic information activated more strongly in the left than the right hemisphere, resulting in a rightward shift of attention. Turriziani and colleagues [
10] explored whether semantic judgments could be modulated by the location in space where a stimulus conveying semantic information was presented. Healthy subjects were presented with three pictures of items in the same semantic category arranged horizontally (one middle and two outer pictures). They were asked to indicate the spatial position in which the semantic distance between the outer and middle pictures was smaller. The results showed an overestimation of the semantic distance of items on the right side of space. These findings suggest the existence of an attentional and mental representational bias in semantic judgements, similar to those reported for space, numbers and alphabet lines. In addition, rTMS over the left parietal cortex selectively reduced this rightward bias. This suggests that spatial manipulation of semantic material could result in the activation of specialised attentional resources located in the left hemisphere.
The present study aimed to explore whether the attentional biases previously reported in semantic judgement tasks are specific to semantic representation or can be generalised to other language components, such as phonology. We investigated whether the spatial location in which phonological strings are presented could be a factor that modulates the performance of healthy participants in phonological judgements tasks.
2. Experiment 1
This experiment investigated the relationship between the phonological distance among words and the space in which these words were presented.
2.1. Subjects
Sixteen right-handed native Italian speakers (6M, 10F; mean age: 25 ± 2.9 years) with normal or corrected to normal vision and naïve to the purpose of the study were enrolled. All subjects gave written informed consent for participation in the study that was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Palermo (approval n. 25/2020). The experiments were done by the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.
2.2. Materials and Procedure
280 four- or five-letter words and 40 five-letter pseudowords were selected. These 320 words were combined to obtain 120 different triplets. Each triplet comprised a middle and two outer words. Triplets were presented for 550 ms on a 19-inch computer monitor. The middle word was presented in the centre of the monitor. The two outer words were presented with 5° of eccentricity to the left and right of the middle word (
Figure 1a). The intertrial interval was 2500 ms. Participants were seated 45 cm from the monitor and were asked to focus on a central fixation cross that preceded the item presentation. The phonological distance between the middle and the two outer words could be the same or smaller on the right or left side of space.
There were three experimental conditions: Same, Different and Very Different conditions. In the Same condition, triplets were composed of three words that differed only for the first letter (e.g., mela, vela, gela) and with an identical phonological distance between the middle and the two outer words. In the Different conditions (e.g., pollo, collo, cesta), one outer word of the triplet was phonologically close to the middle one. In contrast, the other outer word was similar to the middle word only for the first letter (e.g., collo, cesta). Finally, in Very Different conditions, the triplets comprised two words and one pseudoword. Therefore, the phonological distance between the middle word and one of the two outer words was much smaller than that between the middle word and the pseudoword (e.g., gatto, matto, fupro).
There were 40 triplets in each of the three experimental conditions.
2.3. Procedure
All subjects received training in the testing procedure until they felt confident to start the experiment. Subjects were asked to indicate the side of space in which the phonological distance between the middle word and an outer word was smaller (“Where is the word phonologically closest to the middle word?”). Participants were told to choose the “same” response if neither of the two outer words appeared more phonologically related to the middle item. Participants responded by pressing one of three buttons with the right middle, index or ring finger for “same,” “left”, or “right” responses, respectively. The side of space in which the target words appeared within each triplet was randomised.
2.4. Results
Accuracy (mean number of errors) and reaction times for correct responses (RTs: interval of time between the onset of stimuli and the participant’s response) were analysed.
We performed a 3x2 ANOVA on the mean number of errors, with the variables Condition (Different, Same, Very Different) and Space (left, right) as within-subjects factors. As shown in
Figure 1b, there was a significant main effect of Condition (F
2,30 = 13.67, p < 0.001). This reflects the fact that the average number of errors in the Very Different condition was significantly different from both the Same (F
1,15 = 35.23, p < 0.001) and Different (F
1,15 = 16.99, p < 0.001) conditions. The error rates in the Same and Different conditions were comparable (F
2,18 = 3.7, p > 0.5). The main effect of Space was not significant (F
1,15 = 0.001, p > 0.5). The interaction of Condition x Space was significant (F
2,30 = 11.4, p < 0.005). Planned comparisons revealed a rightward bias in the Different conditions. Specifically, in trials where the phonological distance was smaller between the middle and the outer word positioned in the right space, participants tended to produce erroneous ‘‘left’’ or ‘‘same’’ responses (F
1,15 = 11.19, p < 0.005). In the Same condition, participants erroneously judged the phonological distance between the middle and the outer word positioned in the left space to be smaller F
1,15 = 4.67, p < 0.05). There was no significant difference between leftward and rightward biases in the Very Different condition (F
1,15 = 2.3, p < 0.5).
The ANOVA performed on the RTs did not reveal differences between the Same and Different conditions with target outer words positioned in the left or right space (F2,30 = .85, p > 0.5). In the Very Different condition, there were no significant differences between triplets with target outer words in the left and right space (F2,30 = .29, p > 0.5).
In sum, phonological judgements were influenced by the spatial location of the stimuli. For example, when comparing the phonological distance between pairs of stimuli, subjects tended to overestimate the distance between a middle word and an outer word positioned at its right.
3. Experiment 2
This experiment examined whether the direction of attentional bias demonstrated in Experiment 1 is also observed in tasks involving phonological judgements on words without semantic representation, such as pronounceable pseudowords.
3.1. Subjects
Twenty-five right-handed subjects (7 M, 19 F; mean age: 24 ± 3.1 years) participated in this experiment. None of them participated in Experiment 1.
3.2. Materials and Procedure
The experimental procedure was identical to that of Experiment 1, except for the stimuli used.
280 four- or five-letter pseudowords and 40 consonant strings on five letters were selected. These 320 stimuli were combined to obtain 120 different triplets. Each triplet was constituted by a middle and two outer pseudowords. The two outer pseudowords were presented with 5° of eccentricity to the left and right of the middle pseudoword (
Figure 2a).
There were three experimental conditions: Same, Different and Very Different conditions. In the Same condition, triplets were composed of three pseudowords with an identical phonological distance between the middle and the two outer pseudowords (e.g., dali, fali, rali). In the Different condition, the triplet was composed of three pseudowords, and the phonological distance between the middle and one of the two outer pseudowords was smaller (e.g., tresa, tarto and marto). The nearer phonological distance was when the middle and one outer pseudoword differed only for the first letter (e.g., tarto, marto). Likewise, the other outer word could be similar to the middle word only for the first letter (e.g., tresa, tarto). In the Very Different condition, the triplets were composed of two pseudowords and one consonant string, and the phonological distance between the middle and one of the two outer pseudowords was smaller than the phonological distance with the other outer pseudoword (e.g., gpnt, pito and nito).
3.3. Results
We performed a 3x2 ANOVA on the mean number of errors, with the variables Condition (Different, Same, Very Different) and Space (left, right) as within-subjects factors. As shown in
Figure 2b, there was a significant main effect of Condition (F
2,48 = 3.00, p = 0.05), with the lowest error rates observed for the Very Different condition. Furthermore, the average number of errors on the Very Different condition was significantly different from both the Same (F
1,24 = 4.89, p < 0.05) and Different (F
1,24 = 5.58, p < 0.05) conditions. However, the error rates in the Same and Different conditions were comparable (F
1,24 = .04, p > 0.5).
The main effect of Space was not significant (F1,24 =.00, p > 0.5). Also, the interaction of Condition x Space was not significant (F2,24 = 2.14, p > 0.5).
The ANOVA performed on the RT data did not reveal differences between the Same and Different conditions with target outer words positioned in the left or right space (F2,48 = .63, p > 0.5). In the Very Different condition, there were no significant differences between triplets with target outer words in the left and right space (F2,48 = .18, p > 0.5).
This experiment’s results show that the stimuli’s spatial location did not influence phonological judgments with pseudowords.
4. Discussion
The main results of the present study show that when asked to compare the phonological distance between words, subjects consistently overestimate phonological distances on the right side of space. Semantic information in phonological strings is crucial in determining this rightward bias. Indeed, healthy subjects show a rightward bias in a bisection task involving phonological distances when stimuli are real words (Experiment 1) but not when stimuli are pseudowords (Experiment 2).
The finding that the spatial location of verbal stimuli modulates the performance on a phonological task is in line with the results of other studies, reporting that spatial manipulation of semantic information induces a rightward bias in a semantic judgement task [
10]. These findings suggest that real language, conveying semantic, phonological and syntactic information, may be internally mapped onto spatial representations.
Interestingly, the type of stimuli processed influences the interaction between space and phonology. For example, a significant rightward bias in the phonological distance judgment task was present only when real words were presented. On the other hand, the mere presentation of verbal material, for example, pseudowords, did not induce any spatial attentional bias. Therefore, the semantic component of language interacts with spatial attention, either when processed explicitly (as in Turriziani et al. [
10]) or implicitly (as in the present study).
Evidence from neuropsychological studies is also suggesting a link between space and language. Coslett reported that in some aphasics, the direction in which they orient their attention influences their use of language [
11]. Chatterjee and colleagues [
12] described an agrammatic patient whose production and comprehension of sentences were influenced by spatial factors. Rinaldi and Pizzamiglio [
13] reported that patients with left spatial neglect made significantly more errors when asked to compare two spoken sentences if the emphatic stress was placed at the beginning of the sentence. Overall, these results seem to support Coslett’s ‘‘Spatial Registration Hypothesis’’ [
11], suggesting that each perceived stimulus is automatically marked with reference to its coordinates in egocentric space, even if spatial information does not seem relevant to the task.
Regarding the neural correlates of the interaction between spatial and linguistic information, we suggest that spatial manipulation of phonological material results in the activation of specialised attentional resources in the left hemisphere. This aligns with the findings Turriziani and colleagues [
10] reported. The authors documented that the left parietal cortex could be the neural correlate that underpins the bias in attention and mental representation of semantic information. Moreover, this suggestion aligns with the ‘‘hemispheric activation model’’ [
14,
15], proposing that the distribution of attention in space is biased contralaterally to the more activated hemisphere. Therefore, we speculate that verbal processing activates the left language-dominant hemisphere more strongly than the right hemisphere. This activation could be responsible for shifting attention towards the right hemispace. In line with this hypothesis, neuroimaging investigations have implicated a network involving the left hemisphere’s parietal and frontal areas in the attention orientation in language tasks (Cristescu et al., 2006). Again, this hypothesis aligns with the two lesion studies’ clinical reports. Indeed, the aphasic patient described by Coslett et al. [
11] and the agrammatic patient described by Chatterjee et al. [
12] had left parietal lesions.
The combination between visuospatial attention and semantic components has also been recently investigated in a TMS study [
16], suggesting the left intraparietal sulcus as the neural correlate of such interaction.
The recent report on the modulation of linguistic functions following left hemispheric activation by spatial adaptation procedures [
17] also aligns with the model of interaction between spatial attention and language in the left hemisphere.
An interaction between spatial and linguistic information could also be interpreted in the theory of magnitudeCampo [
18] (ATOM) framework, proposing an interaction between different magnitudes (i.e., space, time, numbers) both at a cognitive and neural level. Although linguistic material cannot be strictly interpreted as a magnitude, some models suggest that semantic information can be represented in vectorial terms [
10,
19]. According to this view, when a task requires manipulation of linguistic material in terms of semantic distance, spatial factors could interact with semantics or phonology in the same way as they interact with numerical or time dimensions [
20].
In conclusion, the findings add new evidence to spatial attentional biases using linguistic stimuli, showing that semantic and phonological information may be internally mapped onto spatial representations.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, PT, MO.; methodology, PT and MO; formal analysis, PT.; data curation, AS, AC.; writing—original draft preparation, AS, AC; writing—review and editing, PT, MO; All authors contributed to the discussions on the interpretation of collected papers and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no external funding
Informed Consent Statement
All subjects gave written informed consent for participation in the study that was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Palermo (approval n. 25/2020). The experiments were done by the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Data Availability Statement
Data are available upon request from the corresponding author.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- Jewell, G.; McCourt, M.E. Pseudoneglect: A Review and Meta-Analysis of Performance Factors in Line Bisection Tasks. Neuropsychologia 2000, 38, 93–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Heilman, K.M.; Jeong, Y.; Finney, G.R. Spatial Cognition. Neurology 2004, 63, 1994–1996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oliveri, M.; Rausei, V.; Koch, G.; Torriero, S.; Turriziani, P.; Caltagirone, C. Overestimation of Numerical Distances in the Left Side of Space. Neurology 2004, 63, 2139–2141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Göbel, S.M.; Calabria, M.; Farnè, A.; Rossetti, Y. Parietal RTMS Distorts the Mental Number Line: Simulating “spatial” Neglect in Healthy Subjects. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 860–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gevers, W.; Reynvoet, B.; Fias, W. The Mental Representation of Ordinal Sequences Is Spatially Organized. Cognition 2003, 87, B87–B95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zorzi, M.; Priftis, K.; Meneghello, F.; Marenzi, R.; Umiltà, C. The Spatial Representation of Numerical and Non-Numerical Sequences: Evidence from Neglect. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 1061–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fischer, M.H. Cognition in the Bisection Task. Trends Cogn Sci 2001, 5, 460–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, B.H.; Kim, M.; Kang, S.J.; Park, K.C.; Kim, E.-J.; Adair, J.C.; Na, D.L. Pseudoneglect in Solid-Line versus Character-Line Bisection Tasks: A Test for Attention Dominance Theory. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2004, 17, 174–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mohr, C.; Leonards, U. Rightward Bisection Errors for Letter Lines: The Role of Semantic Information. Neuropsychologia 2007, 45, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turriziani, P.; Oliveri, M.; Bonnì, S.; Koch, G.; Smirni, D.; Cipolotti, L. Exploring the Relationship between Semantics and Space. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e5319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coslett, H.B. Spatial Influences on Motor and Language Function. Neuropsychologia 1999, 37, 695–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chatterjee, A.; Maher, L.M.; Heilman, K.M. Spatial Characteristics of Thematic Role Representation. Neuropsychologia 1995, 33, 643–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rinaldi, M.C.; Pizzamiglio, L. When Space Merges into Language. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 556–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowers, D.; Heilman, K.M. Pseudoneglect: Effects of Hemispace on a Tactile Line Bisection Task. Neuropsychologia 1980, 18, 491–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kinsbourne, M. A Model for the Mechanism of Unilateral Neglect of Space. Trans. Am. Neurol. Assoc. 1970, 95, 143–146. [Google Scholar]
- Capotosto, P.; Sulpizio, V.; Galati, G.; Baldassarre, A. Visuo-Spatial Attention and Semantic Memory Competition in the Parietal Cortex. Sci. Rep. 2023, 13, 6218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turriziani, P.; Chiaramonte, G.; Mangano, G.R.; Bonaventura, R.E.; Smirni, D.; Oliveri, M. Improvement of Phonemic Fluency Following Leftward Prism Adaptation. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 7313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walsh, V. A Theory of Magnitude: Common Cortical Metrics of Time, Space and Quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 483–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zannino, G.D.; Perri, R.; Pasqualetti, P.; Caltagirone, C.; Carlesimo, G.A. Analysis of the Semantic Representations of Living and Nonliving Concepts: A Normative Study. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2006, 23, 515–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicario, C.M.; Pecoraro, P.; Turriziani, P.; Koch, G.; Caltagirone, C.; Oliveri, M. Relativistic Compression and Expansion of Experiential Time in the Left and Right Space. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e1716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).