Preprint
Article

Sex/gender Differences in the Language Profiles of Italian Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Altmetrics

Downloads

142

Views

57

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

22 June 2023

Posted:

26 June 2023

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Sex/gender (S/G) differences in ASD language profiles have been poorly investigated. The present study aims to explore whether male (M) and female (F) children with ASD and with normal non-verbal cognitive abilities differ in their linguistic profiles. A sample of 76 Italian children with ASD (range: 4.9 – 8 years), including 50 M and 26 F, was retrospectively recruited. Language profiles were analyzed using standardized tests for the evaluation of receptive and expressive vocabulary as well as grammar. Grammatical comprehension was the most impaired domain compared to the other language measures in both M and F children. Comparing language profiles between S/G, F showed significantly better scores than M in grammatical production (p=.002), and M showed better active negative sentences comprehension (p=.035). Moreover, comparing the language profiles between M and F with receptive disorder, F had significantly worse grammatical comprehension and better grammatical production than M. Even among children without receptive disorder, F had significantly higher grammatical production scores. The S/G differences in language profile, particularly better expressive language in F than M, can partially contribute to the delayed ASD diagnosis or underdiagnosis of F without intellectual disability. Finally, our results document the importance of accurately investigating both expressive and receptive abilities in children with ASD.
Keywords: 
Subject: Medicine and Pharmacology  -   Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by persistent deficits in reciprocal social interaction, communication, and the presence of re-stricted, repetitive behaviour and interests [1]. Prevalence data from the most recent study conducted by the American Centers for Disease Control and Prevention reported that ASD occurs in approximately one in every 36 children aged 8 years [2]. In Italy the prevalence resulting from a cross-sectional epidemiological study conducted in the metropolitan area of Pisa (Tuscany, Central Italy) was of 1 out of 87 among children aged 7 to 9 years [3].
According to the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [1], language impairment is no longer a core symptom of ASD but an ASD ‘specifier’, used for a more detailed description of the patient’ characteristics. Indeed, the level of language skills is highly heterogeneous in ASD, ranging from individuals who never develop spoken language (approximately one third of subjects, according to Koegel et al., [4]) to individuals with preserved expressive language abilities, but with deficits in the pragmatic use of language [5]. Moreover, most toddlers show a delay in standard early language milestones that in some children can be recovered around three or four years of age, while others may show a regression of language after the acquisition of first words at 12-15 months [6].
The over-representation of boys with respect to girls is one of the most replicated findings in the ASD literature [7,8,9]. Indeed, the sex imbalance prevalence in the ASD, with an approximately a 4, 1 male (M) to female (F) ratio [10], that shifted to-ward a 3, 1 ratio in epidemiological screening surveys on the general population [11], has historically impacted on the scientific knowledge (clinical, genetic and neuroanatomical) of ASD in F [12]. This topic has been poorly investigated, and only recently a growing number of studies have focused on it [11].
To note, that given the difficulties to disentangle the effects of sex and gender on ASD features, the term “sex/gender” (S/G) will be used throughout this article to acknowledge the overlap between these two concepts [13].
In toddlerhood, language delay is a major cause for concern among parents of infants who go on to receive an ASD diagnosis [14], and this symptom is more prominent in autistic M compared to F [15]. Moreover, the production of first words and sentences occurs earlier in ASD F than in ASD M [16]. Indeed, S/G differences in language delay may contribute to the delayed ASD diagnosis in some F compared to M [17,18], since earlier ASD diagnoses are associated with parent-reported expressive language delays [19]. According to this view, findings from a retrospective investigation reported that ASD F who received a diagnosis of autism after the age of 5 years displayed more advanced social communication skills, including vocabulary [20]. Vice-versa, autistic F receiving a diagnosis during toddlerhood often displayed co-occurring language delays and/or intellectual disability [21].
Controversial findings regarding S/G differences in language and communication domains were reported. Some studies did not find any statistically significant differences on basic vocabulary, on grammar skills and on narrative abilities between F and M [22,23], whereas others showed S/G differences in pragmatic and narrative abilities [23,24,25,26,27,28,29]. Regarding semantic abilities, Sturrock et al. [23] and Goddard et al. [30] found that ASD F performed better than ASD M using similar word-generation/fluency tasks.
Moreover, ASD F performed better than ASD M on narrative skills (including salient storytelling, rich narrative details, use of internal state language) [27], and on clinical observations of pragmatic abilities [29].
The abovementioned studies focused more on social and pragmatic domains rather than on basic structural language, and identified pragmatic and associated higher-level structural language skills as areas of difference between the M and F phenotype of ASD [23,24,27]. Indeed, S/G differences appear to exist mainly in domains where meaning of structural language is mediated by social context, inference, language of emotion and internal state, and pragmatic behaviours in discourse and narratives [31].
To address the abovementioned knowledge gap, in the present study we aimed to investigate S/G differences in basic structural language profiles in a cohort of children with ASD. Specifically, we directly compared expressive and receptive language abilities between M and F Italian children with ASD, using both naturalistic and a standardized assessment performed by speech-language therapists with expertise in communication disorders. Since a significant association between non-verbal IQ and language abilities was previously detected [32,33], we decided to focus our investigation on a homogeneous group of children without intellectual disability.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

The study was conducted on a sample of 76 children with ASD (50 males and 26 females) aged 4.9 – 8 years (mean age: 6.4 years; SD: 12.1 months) retrospectively recruited in a tertiary care University hospital from February 2009 to November 2020.
Inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) Diagnosis of either Autistic Disorder according to DSM-IV-TR criteria [34] or Autism Spectrum Disorder according to DSM-5 criteria [1]; (2) Non-verbal IQ or developmental quotient ≥ 70, as assessed through standardized psychometric tests (i.e. Wechsler scales; Griffiths scales); (3) Expressive language at the level of multi-words production.
Informed written consent was obtained from the parents of all participants. This study was approved by the Paediatric Ethical Committee of the Tuscany Region (approval number: 178/2016), and was conducted according to the Helsinki Declaration.

2.2. Instruments

The language profile was evaluated by the following struments:
  • Grammatical Comprehension Test for Children (TCGB) [35] is standardized on Italian children aged 3.6 – 8 years. The TCGB is a picture multiple-choice language test composed by 76 sentences pertaining to eight main blocks of grammatical structures (locatives, inflectionals, both affirmative and negative actives and passives, relatives and datives).
  • Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised (PPVT-R) [36] is a multiple-choice language test for receptive vocabulary, for 3.9-11.6 years old children.
  • One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test [37] is an expressive picture-naming test for high and low-frequency words), for 4.6-10.8 years old children.
  • The ‘Grid of Analysis of Spontaneous speech’ (GASS) [38,39] is a grid for the analysis of spontaneous language performed according to six levels rating system based on syntactical and morphological criteria.
For all language tests, (with the exception of the GASS), z scores below –1.5 SD of the mean were considered as clinically significant. For a detailed description of TCGB and GASS, see Table S1 and Table S2 in Supplementary Material.
WPPSI-III [40] performance IQ or Perceptual Reasoning Index at WISC-IV [41] or Griffiths [42] developmental quotient of the performance scale were used as measures of the non-verbal intellectual functioning level.
ADOS-G [43] or ADOS-2 [44] was performed with ASD children for the evaluation of the autistic severity.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Skewness and Kurtosis statistics did not demonstrate a normal distribution for language-related variables, thus non-parametric tests were used.
The Mann–Whitney U test was used to compare age, language scores (z scores for TCGB and One-Word Picture Vocabulary, Lexical Quotient for PPVT-R and GASS level), non-verbal, verbal and total cognitive scores and ADOS severity scores:
  • between S/G (50 ASD M and 26 ASD F);
  • between S/G (50 ASD M and 26 ASD F);
  • between ASD M and ASD F without receptive disorder.
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 21 software (IBM SPSS Statistics, Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

Considering the mean z scores of the whole sample (see Table 1), grammatical comprehension (TCGB) was the most impaired domain compared to the other language measures (receptive and expressive vocabulary) in both M and F groups. The TCGB mean total z score and the mean z score of the different language structures (except for active negative sentences in the M group) fell below -1,5 SD of the mean (see Table 1). The expressive vocabulary (One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test) was the better linguistic area in both groups (mean z score > 1.5).
The analysis of the number of children who presented deficient expressive grammatical abilities (GASS) showed that there was a higher percentage of M (15%) than F (4%) with impaired performance. Moreover, receptive vocabulary and grammar were the most deficient areas in a high percentage of children in both the M and the F groups (receptive vocabulary: M=42%, F=56%; grammatical comprehension: M=62%, F=65%).
Statistical analysis with Mann–Whitney U test did not show any significant differences in age, cognitive abilities and severity of autistic symptoms between M and F (Table 1). Instead, the two groups showed significant differences in expressive grammatical abilities and in active negative sentences comprehension. In particular, F showed significantly better scores than M in grammatical production (p=.002), whereas M had better active negative sentences comprehension (p=.035). Moreover, M showed a better grammatical comprehension total score than F, though this finding did not reach statistical significance.
Given the importance of receptive difficulties, Mann–Whitney U tests have been conducted to compare the functional profiles of M and F with and without receptive disorder. Both analyses (comparisons between M and F with and without receptive disorder) did not show any significant differences in non-verbal cognitive scores and severity of autistic symptoms between the groups. Both receptive (p=.014) and expressive (p=.019) grammatical abilities differed significantly between M and F in children with receptive deficits. In particular, F had significantly worse grammatical comprehension, but significantly better grammatical production than M.
Statistical comparison between M and F without receptive disorders confirmed also in this group that F had significantly better expressive grammatical abilities (p=.028) than M.

4. Discussion

The present study aims to explore whether male (M) and female (F) children with ASD and without intellectual disability (at the non-verbal level) differ in their linguistic profiles.
An extensive literature on typical development reported that M and F differ in the rates of communication and language development. In particular, F demonstrate an earlier acquisition of first words [45], a better and earlier integration of language with gesture [46], and an earlier use of the social-emotional words, and of more complex linguistic forms during spontaneous speech [47]. Regarding conversational skills, F use more collaborative and negotiated discourse [48] and focus on person-centered topics and emotions [49]. These profiles are crucial for social-linguistic interaction and integration with female social groups [49,50]. Conversely, S/G differences in language profiles of children with ASD are still poorly investigated.
In the present study we found that both M and F groups displayed a deficit in grammatical comprehension, and this weakness was more evident in ASD F than in ASD M. Moreover, in our sample F presented significantly better grammatical production skills than M.
These results thus confirm not only the presence of a marked impairment of receptive skills in children with ASD [51,52,53,54,55,56], but also, especially in the F group, a strong discrepancy between language comprehension and production already documented in the literature [56,57,58]. This discrepancy between grammatical production and comprehension may make it difficult to identify the receptive disorder. In fact, the better production skills may mask the comprehension deficits, thus not allowing the access to specific rehabilitation interventions.
The above data should be interpreted with caution, as the few papers that addressed this issue report conflicting results. For example, Sturrock et al. [23] compared ASD M and F with PIQ ≥ 70 and proposed them a battery of direct assessments targeting expressive and receptive language at multiple levels: word, simple and complex sentences, narratives, word knowledge (semantics), inference and vocabulary of emotion. These Authors did not find any statistical differences in grammar skills (and basic vocabulary) between M and F. Moreover, in a recent paper, Sturrock et al. [31] provides a synthesis of recent studies investigating language and communication difficulties in autistic F without intellectual disability. The Authors found that autistic F appeared to perform below typically developing F on measures of pragmatics, semantics, and above sentence-level structural language; however, vocabulary and basic grammar (receptive and expressive) appeared to be unaffected. These data are consistent with the review of McFayden and colleagues [59] in which S/G differences are most evident when social communication is evaluated in a naturalistic context rather than based on a standardized assessment. In fact, parents of F described greater difficulties than those detected through direct standardized measurement [60,61].
Conversely, our results are in line with some works detecting better performance of F than M in some expressive language tasks. In this context, Sturrock et al. [23] and Goddard et al. [30] found that autistic F performed better than autistic M using similar word-generation/fluency tasks.
Other studies demonstrated S/G differences in pragmatic elements of narratives, with autistic F generating richer character depictions and descriptions of internal states, cognition, perception and judgment [24,26,27] and overall better skills in retelling salient story elements [27]. For instance, autistic girls used significantly more social words than autistic boys during the conversation section of the ADOS-2 Module 3 [62].
All these data, documenting the best expressive language and pragmatic abilities, including social words used and grammatical production (as found in our study), can partially contribute to the undiagnosed or late-diagnosed ASD in F. Moreover, the specific linguistic profile of high-functioning F with ASD has been related to the “camouflage” abilities of these individuals (for a recent review, see Tubío-Fungueiriño et al., [63]). Indeed, the term “camouflaging” is used to describe the strategies, either voluntarily or involuntarily, adopted by ASD subjects for masking or compensating the social impairment experienced during the social interactions [25]. Even if the majority of the investigations on social camouflaging to date focused on F adolescents and adults with ASD, this feature is also present in children with ASD [64,65,66]. Recent research suggests that girls with ASD may “camouflage” real struggles with social communication by engaging in verbal and social communication, social mimicry and behaving in ways that are superficially typical, and these factors, combined with male-referenced diagnostic criteria and unequal societal expectations for boys’ and girls’ social interaction skills across development, may complicate ASD diagnosis [11,59,67,68,69,70]. Accordingly, a late or inaccurate diagnosis for females with ASD, may result in a difficulty of accessing to evidence-based interventions and in a lack of social supports. In this way there is a high risk of experiencing social rejection, and poor mental health outcomes [59,71,72].
The results of the present study suggest the presence of distinct linguistic profiles in M and F with ASD. They also provide evidence of the importance of accurately investigating both expressive and receptive language abilities for the choice of effective and personalized interventions aimed at promoting language development on the basis of the specific language profile.
The present study has certain limitations that must be acknowledged. First, the rather low sample size and the retrospective nature of this single center investigation make the study susceptible to bias: therefore, the results obtained need to be further validated on a larger sample of ASD children. Second, only ASD subjects without intellectual disability were included. This selection criterion was justified by the need to homogenize the sample of children with ASD, but it does not allow to generalize results to the rest of the ASD population. Third, the lack of a matched control group of typically developing children was a limitation of the study, but was mitigated by the use of standardized tests.
Future studies should integrate clinical assessment performed by trained professionals with parent-report measures of communication abilities in order to obtain a more comprehensive picture of S/G ASD differences in the use of language in daily living situations.

Supplementary Materials

The detailed description of TCGB and GASS can be downloaded at: Preprints.org, Table S1, The Test of Grammatical Comprehension for Children TCGB; Table S2, Grid of Analysis of Spontaneous Speech GASS.

Author Contributions

J.B. and S.C. conceived the idea for this original research and all other authors contributed to the conception and the design of the study. J.B., G.M., R.N., and L.P. carried out the enrollment of all children for the study. A.M, A.C., and R.T. performed the neuropsychiatric assessment and data curation. J.B. and A.N. carried out the statistical analysis. J.B., G.M. and S.C. conceived and prepared the manuscript. A.N., A.M.C. and P.C. were responsible for the writing, reviewing and editing. S.C.: funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

The work was supported by the Italian Ministry of Health Grant RC and 5 × 1000 Health Research and by AIMS2-Trials, http://aims-2-trials.eu (S.C.)

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was approved by the Paediatric Ethical Committee of the Tuscany Region (approval number: 178/2016, date of approval: 22 November 2016), and was conducted according to the Helsinki Declaration.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data created or analyzed during this study is available upon request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association: Washington (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Maenner, M.J.; Warren, Z.; Williams, A.R.; Amoakohene, E.; Bakian, A.V.; Bilder, D.A.; Durkin, M.S.; Fitzgerald, R.T.; Furnier, S.M.; Hughes, M.M.; et al. Prevalence and Characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8 Years — Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2020. MMWR. Surveill. Summ. 2023, 72, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Narzisi, A.; Posada, M.; Barbieri, F.; Chericoni, N.; Ciuffolini, D.; Pinzino, M.; Romano, R.; Scattoni, M.L.; Tancredi, R.; Calderoni, S.; et al. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder in a large Italian catchment area: a school-based population study within the ASDEU project. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 2018, 29, e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Koegel, L.K.; Bryan, K.M.; Su, P.L.; Vaidya, M.; Camarata, S. Definitions of Nonverbal and Minimally Verbal in Research for Autism: A Systematic Review of the Literature. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 50, 2957–2972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Grzadzinski, R.; Huerta, M.; Lord, C. DSM-5 and autism spectrum disorders (ASDs): an opportunity for identifying ASD subtypes. Mol. Autism 2013, 4, 12–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Tager-Flusberg, H.; M, R.; M, B.; C, J.; Droucker, D.; Curtin, S.; Vouloumanos, A.; Gillam, S.L.; Hartzheim, D.; Studenka, B.; et al. Risk Factors Associated With Language in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Clues to Underlying Mechanisms. J. Speech, Lang. Hear. Res. 2016, 59, 143–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Idring, S.; Lundberg, M.; Sturm, H.; Dalman, C.; Gumpert, C.; Rai, D.; Lee, B.K.; Magnusson, C. Changes in Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders in 2001–2011: Findings from the Stockholm Youth Cohort. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 1766–1773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Werling, D.M.; Geschwind, D.H. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2013, 26, 146–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Brugha, T.S.; McManus, S.; Bankart, J.; Scott, F.; Purdon, S.; Smith, J.; Bebbington, P.; Jenkins, R.; Meltzer, H. Epidemiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Adults in the Community in England. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2011, 68, 459–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Zeidan, J.; Fombonne, E.; Scorah, J.; Ibrahim, A.; Durkin, M.S.; Saxena, S.; Yusuf, A.; Shih, A.; Elsabbagh, M. Global prevalence of autism: A systematic review update. Autism Res. 2022, 15, 778–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Loomes, R.; Hull, L.; Mandy, W.P.L. What Is the Male-to-Female Ratio in Autism Spectrum Disorder? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 56, 466–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Calderoni, S. Sex/gender differences in children with autism spectrum disorder: A brief overview on epidemiology, symptom profile, and neuroanatomy. J. Neurosci. Res. 2022, 101, 739–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lai, M.-C.; Lombardo, M.V.; Auyeung, B.; Chakrabarti, B.; Baron-Cohen, S. Sex/Gender Differences and Autism: Setting the Scene for Future Research. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2015, 54, 11–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Herlihy, L.; Knoch, K.; Vibert, B.; Fein, D. Parents' first concerns about toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: effect of sibling status. Autism 2015, 19, 20–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Dillon, E.F.; Kanne, S.; Landa, R.J.; Annett, R.; Bernier, R.; Bradley, C.; Carpenter, L.; Kim, S.H.; Parish-Morris, J.; Schultz, R.; et al. Sex Differences in Autism: Examining Intrinsic and Extrinsic Factors in Children and Adolescents Enrolled in a National ASD Cohort. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2021, 53, 1305–1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Harrop, C.; Libsack, E.; Bernier, R.; Dapretto, M.; Jack, A.; McPartland, J.C.; Van Horn, J.D.; Webb, S.J.; Pelphrey, K. ; the GENDAAR Consortium Do Biological Sex and Early Developmental Milestones Predict the Age of First Concerns and Eventual Diagnosis in Autism Spectrum Disorder? Autism Res. 2020, 14, 156–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Giarelli, E.; Wiggins, L.D.; Rice, C.E.; Levy, S.E.; Kirby, R.S.; Pinto-Martin, J.; Mandell, D. Sex differences in the evaluation and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders among children. Disabil. Heal. J. 2010, 3, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Begeer, S.; Mandell, D.; Wijnker-Holmes, B.; Venderbosch, S.; Rem, D.; Stekelenburg, F.; Koot, H.M. Sex Differences in the Timing of Identification Among Children and Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 43, 1151–1156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Nitzan, T.; Koller, J.; Ilan, M.; Faroy, M.; Michaelovski, A.; Menashe, I.; Meiri, G.; Dinstein, I. The Importance of Language Delays as an Early Indicator of Subsequent ASD Diagnosis in Public Healthcare Settings. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2022, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Hiller, R.M.; Young, R.L.; Weber, N. Sex differences in pre-diagnosis concerns for children later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 2016, 20, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Reinhardt, V.P.; Wetherby, A.M.; Schatschneider, C.; Lord, C. Examination of Sex Differences in a Large Sample of Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 697–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hartley, S.L.; Sikora, D.M. Sex Differences in Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Examination of Developmental Functioning, Autistic Symptoms, and Coexisting Behavior Problems in Toddlers. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 1715–1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Sturrock, A.; Yau, N.; Freed, J.; Adams, C. Speaking the Same Language? A Preliminary Investigation, Comparing the Language and Communication Skills of Females and Males with High-Functioning Autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 50, 1639–1656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Kauschke, C.; van der Beek, B.; Kamp-Becker, I. Narratives of Girls and Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Gender Differences in Narrative Competence and Internal State Language. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 46, 840–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hull, L.; Mandy, W.; Petrides, K. Behavioural and cognitive sex/gender differences in autism spectrum condition and typically developing males and females. Autism 2016, 21, 706–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Boorse, J.; Cola, M.; Plate, S.; Yankowitz, L.; Pandey, J.; Schultz, R.T.; Parish-Morris, J. Linguistic markers of autism in girls: evidence of a “blended phenotype” during storytelling. Mol. Autism 2019, 10, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Conlon, O.; The Pathways in ASD Study Team; Volden, J. ; Smith, I.M.; Duku, E.; Zwaigenbaum, L.; Waddell, C.; Szatmari, P.; Mirenda, P.; Vaillancourt, T.; et al. Gender Differences in Pragmatic Communication in School-Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 1937–1948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cola, M.L.; Plate, S.; Yankowitz, L.; Petrulla, V.; Bateman, L.; Zampella, C.J.; de Marchena, A.; Pandey, J.; Schultz, R.T.; Parish-Morris, J. Sex differences in the first impressions made by girls and boys with autism. Mol. Autism 2020, 11, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Sturrock, A.; Marsden, A.; Adams, C.; Freed, J. Observational and Reported Measures of Language and Pragmatics in Young People with Autism: A Comparison of Respondent Data and Gender Profiles. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 50, 812–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Goddard, L.; Dritschel, B.; Howlin, P. A Preliminary Study of Gender Differences in Autobiographical Memory in Children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 2087–2095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Sturrock, A.; Adams, C.; Freed, J. A Subtle Profile With a Significant Impact: Language and Communication Difficulties for Autistic Females Without Intellectual Disability. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kjelgaard, M.M.; Tager-Flusberg, H. An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2001, 16, 287–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Nevill, R.; Hedley, D.; Uljarević, M.; Sahin, E.; Zadek, J.; Butter, E.; A Mulick, J. Language profiles in young children with autism spectrum disorder: A community sample using multiple assessment instruments. Autism 2017, 23, 141–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.), 2000.
  35. Chilosi, A.M.; Cipriani, P. TCGB Test di Comprensione Grammaticale Per Bambini; Edizioni Del Cerro: Tirrenia, Pisa, Italy, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  36. Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, L.M. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; Guidance Associates: Wilmingston, DE, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  37. Brizzolara, D. Test di Vocabolario Figurato; Technical Report of the Research Project 500.4/62.1/1134; Italian Department of Health to IRCCS Stella Maris: Calambrone, Pisa, Italy, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  38. Chilosi, A.; Pfanner, L.; Pecini, C.; Salvadorini, R.; Casalini, C.; Brizzolara, D.; Cipriani, P. Which linguistic measures distinguish transient from persistent language problems in Late Talkers from 2 to 4 years? A study on Italian speaking children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2019, 89, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cipriani, P.; Chilosi, A.M.; Pfanner, L.; Villani, S.; Bottari, P. Il ritardo di linguaggio in età precoce: Profili evolutivi e indici di rischio. In Indici di Rischio Nel Primo Sviluppo Del Linguaggio; Caselli, C., Capirci, O., Eds.; Franco Angeli: Milano, Italy, 2002; pp. 95–108. [Google Scholar]
  40. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 3rd ed.; Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  41. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  42. Griffiths, R. The Abilities of Young Children: A Comprehensive System of Mental Measurement for the First Eight Years of Life (Revised Edition); Bucks: A.R.I.C.D. Test Agency Limited: London, UK, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lord, C.; Risi, S.; Lambrecht, L.; Cook, J.E.H.; Leventhal, B.L.; DiLavore, P.C.; Pickles, A.; Rutter, M. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A Standard Measure of Social and Communication Deficits Associated with the Spectrum of Autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000, 30, 205–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; DiLavore, P.C.; Risi, S.; Luyster, R.J.; Gotham, K.; Bishop, S.L.; Guthrie, W. ADOS-2—Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition; Colombi, C., Tancredi, R., Persico, A., Faggioli, R., Eds.; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  45. Bleses, D.; Vach, W.; Slott, M.; Wehberg, S.; Thomsen, P.; Madsen, T.O.; Basbøll, H. The Danish Communicative Developmental Inventories: validity and main developmental trends. J. Child Lang. 2008, 35, 651–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Eriksson, M.; Marschik, P.B.; Tulviste, T.; Almgren, M.; Pereira, M.P.; Wehberg, S.; Marjanovič-Umek, L.; Gayraud, F.; Kovacevic, M.; Gallego, C. Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 30, 326–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bouchard, C.; Trudeau, N.; Sutton, A.; Boudreault, M.-C.; Deneault, J. Gender differences in language development in French Canadian children between 8 and 30 months of age. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2009, 30, 685–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Ladegaard, H.J.; Bleses, D. Gender differences in young children's speech: the acquisition of sociolinguistic competence. Int. J. Appl. Linguistics 2003, 13, 222–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Newman, M.L.; Groom, C.J.; Handelman, L.D.; Pennebaker, J.W. Gender Differences in Language Use: An Analysis of 14,000 Text Samples. Discourse Process. 2008, 45, 211–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Tierney, S.; Burns, J.; Kilbey, E. Looking behind the mask: Social coping strategies of girls on the autistic spectrum. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2016, 23, 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Charman, T.; Drew, A.; Baird, C.; Baird, G. Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). J. Child Lang. 2003, 30, 213–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Mitchell, S.; Brian, J.; Zwaigenbaum, L.; Roberts, W.; Szatmari, P.; Smith, I.; Bryson, S. Early Language and Communication Development of Infants Later Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2006, 27, S69–S78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Tager-Flusberg, H.; Caronna, E. Language Disorders: Autism and Other Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Pediatr. Clin. North Am. 2007, 54, 469–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hudry, K.; Leadbitter, K.; Temple, K.; Slonims, V.; McConachie, H.; Aldred, C.; Howlin, P.; Charman, T. Preschoolers with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared with expressive language abilities. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2010, 45, 681–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kjellmer, L.; Fernell, E.; Gillberg, C.; Norrelgen, F. Speech and language profiles in 4- to 6-year-old children with early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2018, ume 14, 2415–2427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Barsotti, J.; Mangani, G.; Nencioli, R.; Pfanner, L.; Tancredi, R.; Cosenza, A.; Sesso, G.; Narzisi, A.; Muratori, F.; Cipriani, P.; et al. Grammatical Comprehension in Italian Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Boucher, J. Research Review: Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder - characteristics and causes. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 219–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Davidson, M.M.; Ellis Weismer, S. A discrepancy in comprehension and production in early language development in ASD: Is it clinically relevant? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 2163–2175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. McFayden, T.C.; Putnam, O.; Grzadzinski, R.; Harrop, C. Sex Differences in the Developmental Trajectories of Autism Spectrum Disorder. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2023, 10, 80–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Ros-Demarize, R.; Bradley, C.; Kanne, S.M.; Warren, Z.; Boan, A.; Lajonchere, C.; Park, J.; Carpenter, L.A. ASD symptoms in toddlers and preschoolers: An examination of sex differences. Autism Res. 2019, 13, 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Carter, A.S.; Black, D.O.; Tewani, S.; Connolly, C.E.; Kadlec, M.B.; Tager-Flusberg, H. Sex Differences in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2007, 37, 86–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Cola, M.; Yankowitz, L.D.; Tena, K.; Russell, A.; Bateman, L.; Knox, A.; Plate, S.; Cubit, L.S.; Zampella, C.J.; Pandey, J.; et al. Friend matters: sex differences in social language during autism diagnostic interviews. Mol. Autism 2022, 13, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Tubío-Fungueiriño, M.; Cruz, S.; Sampaio, A.; Carracedo, A.; Fernández-Prieto, M. Social Camouflaging in Females with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Review. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 51, 2190–2199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Rynkiewicz, A.; Schuller, B.; Marchi, E.; Piana, S.; Camurri, A.; Lassalle, A.; Baron-Cohen, S. An investigation of the ‘female camouflage effect’ in autism using a computerized ADOS-2 and a test of sex/gender differences. Mol. Autism 2016, 7, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Dean, M.; Harwood, R.; Kasari, C. The art of camouflage: Gender differences in the social behaviors of girls and boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 2017, 21, 678–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Harrop, C.; Jones, D.; Zheng, S.; Nowell, S.W.; Boyd, B.A.; Sasson, N. Sex differences in social attention in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2018, 11, 1264–1275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Lai, M.-C.; Lombardo, M.V.; Pasco, G.; Ruigrok, A.N.V.; Wheelwright, S.J.; Sadek, S.A.; Chakrabarti, B.; Baron-Cohen, S. ; MRC AIMS Consortium, A Behavioral Comparison of Male and Female Adults with High Functioning Autism Spectrum Conditions. PLOS ONE 2011, 6, e20835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Lai, M.-C.; Baron-Cohen, S. Identifying the lost generation of adults with autism spectrum conditions. Lancet Psychiatry 2015, 2, 1013–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Parish-Morris, J.; Liberman, M.Y.; Cieri, C.; Herrington, J.D.; Yerys, B.E.; Bateman, L.; Donaher, J.; Ferguson, E.; Pandey, J.; Schultz, R.T. Linguistic camouflage in girls with autism spectrum disorder. Mol. Autism 2017, 8, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Dworzynski, K.; Ronald, A.; Bolton, P.; Happé, F. How different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic threshold for autism spectrum disorders? J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2012, 51, 788–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bargiela, S.; Steward, R.; Mandy, W. The Experiences of Late-diagnosed Women with Autism Spectrum Conditions: An Investigation of the Female Autism Phenotype. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 3281–3294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Pecora, L.A.; Hancock, G.I.; Mesibov, G.B.; Stokes, M.A. Characterising the Sexuality and Sexual Experiences of Autistic Females. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 4834–4846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Male and female sample characteristics (N= 76) and Mann–Whitney U test.
Table 1. Male and female sample characteristics (N= 76) and Mann–Whitney U test.
Males (N=50) Females (N=26)
Mean SD Meanz score Mean SD Mean z score p
Age 76.08 12.54 - 79.58 12.67 - ns
Grammatical Comprehension score (TCGB) 20.41 12.14 -2.93 21.08 9.59 -4.77 ns
Locative 2.60 2.27 -1.67 2.87 2.07 -1.90 ns
Inflectional 3.45 2.65 -2.36 3.15 2.01 -4.27 ns
Affirmative Active 2.14 2.04 -2.60 2.02 1.43 -4.06 ns
Negative Active* 1.94 1.78 -0.79 2.25 1.45 -1.70 .035
Affirmative Passive 3.25 2.29 -2.11 3.79 2.54 -4.57 ns
Negative Passive 2.54 1.82 -2.58 3.09 1.69 -3.45 ns
Relative 2.44 1.93 -2.53 2.35 1.32 -3.59 ns
Dative 2.08 1.37 -5.47 1.44 1.35 -4.23 ns
Receptive Vocabulary (LQ PPVT-R) 82.15 10.74 -1.19 81.39 16.61 -1.24 ns
Grammatical production level (GASS)* 4.16 0.61 - 4.62 0.56 - .002
Expressive Vocabulary for high-frequency words (One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test) 14.04 5.84 -0.47 12.42 3.73 -0.52 ns
Expressive Vocabulary for low-frequency words (One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test) 32.11 6.76 -0.72 32.69 4.56 -1.21 ns
NVIQ 101.35 14.83 - 100.38 16.63 - ns
VIQ 89.36 26.55 - 90.21 20.18 - ns
TIQ 94.18 13.52 - 90.33 20.59 - ns
ADOS Comparison Score 5.36 1.38 - 5.20 1.35 - ns
*p<.05. Abbreviations: SD: standard deviation; LQ: Lexical Quotient; NVIQ: Non-verbal Intelligence Quotient; VIQ: Verbal Intelligence Quotient; TIQ: Total Intelligence Quotient.
Table 2. Comparison between males and females with receptive disorder and between males and females without receptive disorder.
Table 2. Comparison between males and females with receptive disorder and between males and females without receptive disorder.
Males with
receptive
disorder
(N=31)
Females with
receptive
disorder
(N=17)
p Males withoutreceptivedisorder(N=19) Females withoutreceptivedisorder(N=9) p
Age 80.71 (11.43) 84.59 (8.52) ns 68.53 (10.65) 70.11 (14.23) ns
Grammatical Comprehension (TCGB, z score)* -4.4 (2.43) -7.02 (3.57) .014 -0.54 (0.61) -0.52 (0.66) ns
Receptive Vocabulary (PPVT-R. LQ) 80.09 (10.77) 76.22 (11.43) ns 84.67 (10.45) 86.56 (19.89) ns
Grammatical production (GASS level)* 4.00 (0.63) 4.47 (0.62) .019 4.39 (0.52) 4.89 (0.33) .028
Expressive Vocabulary for high-frequency words (One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, z score) -1.02 (1.46) -0.99 (1.23) ns 0.41 (0.86) 0.36 (0.88) ns
Expressive Vocabulary for low-frequency words (One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, z score) -1.06 (1.2) -1.64 (0.82) ns -0.16 (1.14) -0.40 (0.47) ns
NVIQ 98.77 (15.43) 93.59 (15.15) ns 105.42 (13.22) 113.22 (11.01) ns
ADOS Comparison Score 5.44 (1.42) 5.38 (1.31) ns 5.24 (1.35) 4.89 (1.45) ns
*p<.05. Abbreviations: LQ: Lexical Quotient; NVIQ: Non-verbal Intelligence Quotient.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated