Preprint
Article

Big Five Personality Factors and Aggression Among School Students

Altmetrics

Downloads

301

Views

94

Comments

0

Submitted:

04 July 2023

Posted:

11 July 2023

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
The study sought to determine the impact of the Big Five personality characteristics on aggressiveness among school children aged 12 to 16 in the Jenin governorate. The study employing the Big five-factor scale of Macri and Costa and the scale of aggressive behaviour in three areas [aggressive behaviour against oneself, aggressive behaviour against others, and aggressive behaviour against property]. The sample size in this study was 314 students. The quantitative study tool [the questionnaire] revealed the following correlation between the five major personality factors and aggression: neuroticism [+], openness [-], conscientiousness [-], extraversion [-], agreeableness [-]. The most prevalent main personality variables were conscientiousness and neuroticism. The degree of aggressiveness in the Jenin governorate was relatively low on the overall scale.
Keywords: 
Subject: Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Introduction and Background

Research on student behaviours has a long tradition. Many studies tend to focus on student behaviours that impede the development of learning and instruction. These behaviours are impacted directly or indirectly by the particular persona of the learner [1].
Personality traits are the universally constant characteristics of thought, behaviour, mentation, manner, and effect expressed by an organism in various situations [2]. These characteristics have a persistent driving force on an organism's actions and thoughts, having this in mind, specific behaviour manifestations will change depending on the circumstances [3]. Personality models differ in their overall knowledge of nature and the number of basic elements that influence behaviour [4].
Recent theoretical developments have revealed that existing personality models include numerous qualities that fall into the broad approach dimension, such as extraversion for McCrae & Costa, the Behavioural Approach System for Corr, liveliness for Cattell, positive emotionality for Tellegen, neuroticism for McCrae and Costa, emotional stability for Cattell, the Behavioural Inhibition System for Corr, and negative emotionality for Tellegen [5].
For decades, one of the most popular ideas in literature has been McCrae & Costa's methods. For instance, the Big Five personality factors refer to a hierarchical organization of five basic dimensions of personality features. These factors are [a] Neuroticism, [b] Extraversion, [c] Openness to Experience, [d] Agreeableness, and [e] Conscientiousness [6].
McCrae and Costa stated the link between basic and characteristic trends, they stated that the core of their theory is "the distinction between fundamental tendencies and typical adaptations, the very difference we have to make on personality stability" [7].
Human conduct reveals a particular endeavour that a person complies with to achieve their needs. The demands of the endeavour are various, and they might push the individual to commit acts that are socially unacceptable or, on the contrary, perform actions that are praise-worthy [8].
There are growing appeals Human conduct. Aggression in teenagers nowadays is one of the most frequent concerns. In recent years, the degree of aggressiveness and violence among teenagers has dramatically grown [9].
The current research focuses on identifying the relationship between the Big Five personality factors and aggressive among school students from the age category of [12-16 years] in the Jenin governorate.
To achieve the research study goals, a prior assessment of the Big Five personality factors and aggressive were tested. Then, the associations between them are examined.

The Big Five Factors Model

The personality characteristics of every human being are different from one another. Various kinds of cultures in this world make the personality characteristics of each nation vary [10].
Personality is conceptualized at a different degree of abstraction or scope from a range of theoretical viewpoints. The most frequent unit for measuring individual differences remains unchanged in personality research. Today, a consensus appears to arise as a broad taxonomy of Goldberg's personality traits known as the Big Five. Plaisant et al., [11] reveal that the Big Five personality factors are five abstract personality factors that are mostly presented by the personality approach, which consist of conscientiousness, emotional stability, openness to experience, agreeableness, and extroversion.
Pervin, Cervone & John [12] said the Big Five personality factor is a factor theory approach. The five categories of factors can be included in emotionally, activity, and sociability factors.
Landy and Conte [13] reveal that the five-factor model is the introduction of five different components. When they are presented together will give a true picture of how a person's type responds to a situation or to another person. These components consist of conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism, and openness to experience.
Several psychologists including Louis L. Thurstone, Raymond B. Cattell, and Donald W.Fiske used factor-analytic approaches to personality trait descriptors in the 1940s and 1950s. Thurstone and Fiske discovered that their data could be explained by five main personality variables. They did not follow up on their discoveries and instead went on to other study projects. Cattell claimed to have discovered over 12 variables but in reality, only five were replicated in reanalysis of his data [14].
The model of personality is known as 'The Big Five' outlines the five main aspects that make up our personality. Paul Sinclair, a psychologist and psychometrics specialist, supported this overview and explanation [15].
Many psychologists have studied and verified the Big Five super characteristics. Moreover, they are at the foundation of many other personality surveys [WT Norman 1963, McCrae & Costa 1987, Brand & Egan 1989, LR Goldman 1990, and P Sinclair 1992]. While Raymond Cattell discovered 16 characteristics in the creation of the 16PF using factor analysis [a statistical method of eliminating many unrelated clusters], no one else has been able to duplicate his work [15].

Aggression

Schools seek to provide a secure learning environment and avoid mishaps. Inappropriate conduct, intimidation, bullying, stealing, and hostility are just a few examples of unwanted behaviours from students. That is why schools invest in a strong trusting connection, a nice working environment, and clear guidelines for how you should treat one another. But also, through recognizing and responding to unacceptable behaviour in a timely manner.
Several theoretical models have suggested a link between aggressive cognitions and violent conduct in the past, with aggressive cognitions seen as a link between furious emotions and aggressive action. [16].
Aggressive cognitions relate to a mindset that involves a disdain for and unfavorable assessment of others, cynicism, and mistrust [17], While stereotypes induce expectations about intergroup affect [emotional bias] and conduct [action readiness]. In like manner, emotions are the most important proximal effect on how stereotypes are converted into action [18].
According to Karneli, Neviyarni & Yulidar [19], aggressive conduct is physical or verbal behaviour that is meant to hurt or injure other people, causing pain and both physical and psychological discomfort. Over-aggression can range from verbal hostility to killing, and there have been several examples of physical and verbal abuse.
Aggressive behaviours have been a major focus in the health promotion of children and adolescents across the world. It has been proven that bully behaviours impact at least 8% of children worldwide, with around 50% of them including aggressiveness. Behavioural issues such as adolescent disobedience, adult aggression, and criminal activity may stem from early childhood exposures to aggressive behaviours [20].
Aggressive behaviour of school-age children arises at the age of 6–14 years old in the form of anger, irritation, envy, greed, jealousy, and the desire to criticize [21].
Failures in a child's formation as a person are one of the primary causes of aggressive conduct, whether he is 3-4 years old, 10, or a teenager [22], The comparative studies identify the following economic, family, school, and social variables as the primary drivers of aggressive /violence. [23]
There are many forms of aggressive behavior and vary according to the theoretical approaches that explain aggressive behavior, such as Delut in [24] and Buss in [21].
The researcher chose three manifestations of aggressive behavior in her research study, which are aggressive behavior towards oneself, aggressive behavior towards others, and aggressive behavior towards property.

2. Materials and Methods

Participants

The study used the descriptive-analytical method due to its relevance to the nature of the current study. " The Relationship Between the Big Five Personality Factors and Aggression Among School Students from the Age Category Of [12-16 Years] in Jenin Governorate." This approach includes research on what is now in the life of man and society.
The study sample consisted of 314 male and female students, Table 1 and Table 2 shows for the Distribution and characteristics of the sample members according to demographic variables.

Methods:

The researcher used two tools for the study:
  • List of the Big Five Factors prepared by McCrae and Costa [25] and Arabization of Al-Ansari [26]
The first version of the list appeared in 1989, and McCrae and Costa [25] prepared it. The things on the list were translated from English into simple Standard Arabic by Al-Ansari [26], and the translation was then subjected to various review cycles by professionals. It is considered the first objective tool to measure the basic dimensions of personality through a set of items [60 items] extracted by factor analysis of a large number of items derived from many personality tests. It has 180 items, and many studies have been conducted on it and various normal samples. Some modifications were made to it to reduce the number of items until the second version of the list was released in 1992, which has 60 items and includes five factors: [1] Openness [2] Conscientiousness [3] Extraversion [4] Agreeableness [5] Neuroticism.
Each sub-factor has twelve statements, each with a five-option response scale: [Strongly Agree, Agree, Neutral, Disagree, Strongly Disagree] These options are rated on a scale of [1,2,3,4,5]. The list assesses five personality traits known as the Big Five Personality Factors by the creators, The study conducted a reliability analysis using Cronbach's alpha and discovered that the personality scale has a high level of reliability, which is 86%.
  • The Aggressiveness scale that was prepared by Al-Saleh [8].
The study employed a scale developed by Al-Saleh [8] to quantify aggressive behaviour. The scale consists of two axes, each branching into multiple fields. Finally, there will be 34 paragraphs spread over the three axes to manifestations of aggressive behaviour [Verbal and physical aggressive behaviour towards oneself, Aggressive behaviour towards others, Aggressive behaviour directed towards property].
Each with a five-option response scale: [Strongly Agree, Agree, Neutral, Disagree, Strongly Disagree] These options are rated on a scale of [1,2,3,4,5], Regarding the aggression scale's reliability, it was discovered that the scale's stability is reliability high, at 95%, which is ideal for the study.

3. Results

3.1. The level of aggression

The researcher used One Sample T-Test to calculate levels of aggression, [See Table 3]
The research found that, on average, the level of aggression among school students in the age group [12-16 years] in Jenin Governorate; t [308] = 3.4, p = .001.
From table [3], it is clear that the manifestations of aggressive behaviour as seen by the sample members appeared among middle school students aged [12–16] in Jenin governorate, with an arithmetic mean of [1.93] and a standard deviation of [.67] on the total score of the domains and its Low. Where the arithmetic averages of the study indicated that individuals' responses to the domains ranged between [2.19-1.6]
With regard to the ranking of the domains, the highest average of individuals’ responses to the domain of Verbal and physically aggressive behaviour towards oneself, where the arithmetic means were [2.19, SD = .81] and are Low grade, and in the second rank, Aggressive behaviour towards others, where the arithmetic mean was [1.88, SD = .78] and are low grade, while in the third rank, the area of aggressive behaviour towards the property, where it had arithmetic mean of [1.69, SD = .67] and are very low grade.

3.2. The most common major personal factors

The researcher used Mauchly's Test of Sphericity to adjust the degrees of freedom for the averaged tests of significance. Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity had been violated, χ2 [9] = 52.351, p < .05. Therefore, degrees of freedom were corrected using Huynh–Feldt estimates of sphericity [ε = .940].
The difference between the means is statistically significant, F = [6.76, 1161.7] = 19.57, p = 0.000 The statistically significant differences between the arithmetic averages of the Big Five personality traits [p = 0.000].
Table 4. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts.
Table 4. Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts.
Source Per Type III Sum of Squares df M S F P.
Per N vs. C .686 1 .686 1.959 .163
E vs. C 13.455 1 13.455 39.147 .000
O vs. C 8.611 1 8.611 35.107 .000
A vs. C 16.109 1 16.109 46.543 .000
Error(per) N vs. C 108.210 309 .350
E vs. C 106.205 309 .344
O vs. C 75.792 309 .245
A vs. C 106.947 309 .346
When the four factors are compared, it is evident that the conscientiousness component is the most prevalent, followed by the neurotic factor by a tiny difference, and the other three factors are the least common.
the most common five major personality factors among middle school students aged 12–16 in Jenin Governorate are the Conscientiousness factor, with arithmetic, a mean of [3.70]. Then it is followed by the Neuroticism factor, with an arithmetic mean of [3.65]. In the third place, Openness [3.53] was followed by the Extraversion factor in the fourth place, with an arithmetic mean of [3.49], and in the fifth and last place was the Agreeableness factor, with an arithmetic mean of [3.47].

3.3. The Relationship between the Big Five Personality Factors and Aggression due to the Demographic Variables.

In order to know the relationship between aggression and the Big Five factors, gender, place of residence, and academic achievement, using the regression models in table [5] were used. [The Big Five factors, gender, place of residence, and academic achievement] were considered as explanatory variables and aggression as a dependent variable.
Table 5. Regression Result [stepwise].
Table 5. Regression Result [stepwise].
Model Unstandardized Coefficients t P R R Square F P
B Std. Error
1 (Constant) 3.360 .199 16.919 .000 .471 .222 56.419 .000
Conscientiousness -.033- .004 -7.511- .000
2 (Constant) 2.386 .269 8.884 .000 .537 .289 43.833 .000
Conscientiousness -.042- .005 -9.193- .000
Neuroticism .031 .006 5.154 .000
3 (Constant) 2.869 .273 10.527 .000 .580 .337 41.431 .000
Conscientiousness -.032- .005 -6.526- .000
Neuroticism .035 .006 5.979 .000
Agreeableness -.027- .005 -5.358- .000
4 (Constant) 2.778 .264 10.509 .000 .601 .361 38.713 .000
Conscientiousness -.032- .005 -6.899- .000
Neuroticism .032 .006 5.522 .000
Agreeableness -.029- .005 -5.854- .000
place of residence .254 .054 4.692 .000
5 [27] 2.991 .267 11.188 .000 .611 .373 34.357 .000
Conscientiousness -.029- .005 -6.232- .000
Neuroticism .033 .006 5.774 .000
Agreeableness -.029- .005 -6.147- .000
place of residence .234 .054 4.375 .000
academic achievement -.108- .032 -3.401- .001
6 [27] 3.081 .268 11.499 .000 .618 .381 30.023 .000
Conscientiousness -.024- .005 -4.641- .000
Neuroticism .035 .006 6.177 .000
Agreeableness -.026- .005 -5.183- .000
place of residence .217 .054 4.045 .000
academic achievement -.096- .032 -3.002- .003
Openness -.014- .006 -2.387- .018
7 [27] 2.972 .272 10.932 .000 .393 .155 26.606 .000
Conscientiousness -.026- .005 -4.931- .000
Neuroticism .037 .006 6.449 .000
Agreeableness -.029- .005 -5.557- .000
place of residence .201 .054 3.724 .000
academic achievement -.091- .032 -2.856- .005
Openness -.020- .007 -3.032- .003
Extraversion .011 .005 2.050 .041
The regression model results showed a statistically significant relationship between aggressiveness and the explanatory variables.
This can be inferred from the value of t and its associated significance. It shows the strength of the relationship between the dependent variable [aggressive] and the explanatory variables. And it takes into account the beta value, which shows the relationship between the aggressive variable and the explanatory variables.
When looking at the value of F and its significance, it can be concluded that the model is valid and that there is a correlation between aggressiveness and explanatory variables.
Thus, the results refer to the following equation:
Aggressive = 3.360 + -.033 * b + 2.386 *c + 2.869 * d + 2.778 * e + 2.991 * f + 3.081 * g + 2.972 * h
b. Predictors: [27], C
c. Predictors: [27], C, N
d. Predictors: [27], C, N, A
e. Predictors: [27], C, N, A, place of residence
f. Predictors: [27], C, N, A, place of residence, academic achievement
g. Predictors: [27], C, N, A, place of residence, academic achievement, O
h. Predictors: [27], C, N, A, place of residence, academic achievement, O, E
According to correlation between aggression and big five factors personality, proved to be negatively correlated with Extraversion [r = -.232; p < 0.01] and Openness [r = -.376; p < 0.01] and negatively with Agreeableness [r = -.385; p < 0.01] and Conscientiousness [r = -.393-; p < 0.01].
The researcher notes that the Neuroticism factor entered the regression equation, indicating the relationship with aggression. It did not have a statistically significant correlation in the correlation table, but in the regression results, the effect was positive and statistically significant [B = .031; p < 0.05].
Looking at Table [5] [the variables included in the regression model], the gender variable is excluded because it does not affect the average responses of individuals on the scale.
The regression equation "academic achievement" was inserted, and the relationship was negative [B = -.108-, SE = .032, P < 0.05] statistically significant functions on the scale score at the p<.05. Given the data entered statistically from "weak" academic achievement to "excellent" academic achievement, the relationship is negative with aggression. Therefore, the higher the academic average, the less aggressive the student is.
The regression equation " place of living " was inserted, and the relationship was positive [B = .254, SE = .054, P < 0.05] statistically significant functions on the scale score at the p<.05. Given the data entered statistically from "camp" place of living to "city" place of living, the relationship is positive with aggression. Therefore, the higher the place of living, the most aggressive the student is.

4. Discussion

The results revealed that the conscientiousness factor was more prevalent among school students in the age range [12–16] in Jenin Governorate than the other five factors, with the neurotic factor ranking second. This shows that most students are motivated to succeed and endure in completing numerous assignments with merit and tenacity. However, some of the pupils exhibit neuroticism [impulsivity and hostility].
The results revealed that concerning the ranking of the domains, the highest average of individuals is responses to the domain of verbal and physically aggressive behaviour towards themselves and others. The third rank was the area of aggressive behaviour towards a property.
The findings indicated that Jenin Governorate middle school pupils had a very low level of aggressiveness on the overall scale in the three areas of aggression [aggressive behaviour against oneself, violent behaviour towards others, and hostile conduct towards property]. The findings are nearly identical to those of Saleh's [8] study, which found moderate hostility among the [6–15] age range in the northern West Bank.
The study attributes this to parents who do everything in their efforts to raise their children correctly and soundly. Also, families' efforts to protect their children from anything that may cause behavioural deviation as well as to protect them from Israeli occupation practices in light of the conditions under which the Palestinian people live. School also aims to raise a conscious generation educated on good behaviour. The emergence of results about the family and the school may result from the existing cooperation between these two environments [the family and the school] because the school and the home are complementary to each other, and the school's keenness on behaviour before education.
Whereas animosity against oneself was higher than in the other groups, students are hiding psychological discomfort, which might be due to a lack of psychological unloading mechanisms in their lives.
When considering the correlations between the five factors of personality and aggression found in this study, there were significant positive relationships between neuroticism and aggression. As a result, people with high neuroticism scores are more likely to exhibit aggressive emotions and aggressive behaviour. In addition, it concurred with some of the findings of the Cavalcanti and Pimentel [28] study. Their study found a positive association between aggression and neuroticism. It also stated that there is a negative relationship between aggression and conscientiousness. At the same time, the results differed with a positive relationship between aggressiveness, extroversion, and openness.
The relationship with the other four [extraversion [-], agreeableness [-], openness [-], and conscientiousness [-]] associated factors was inverse. This is to say, the higher the level of the factor in the personality, the less aggressive it was. This agrees with Gitonga, Chege, and Karuku's [29] study, which showed a negative correlation between personality factors and aggression and agreed with Ang et al. [2004] study. According to the findings of Barlett and Anderson [30], there are both direct and indirect correlations between violent behaviour and the five characteristics. The present study confirms this.
It agrees with Antoanzas's [31] study of the existence of a correlation and the effect of the five major personality factors on aggression.
Results also stated that there is an inverse relationship between extroversion and aggression. In other words, the higher the extroversion, the less aggressive the person is, so that the person is introverted and their level of aggression increases. The researcher attributed this to psychological pressures experienced by the student, whether at home or school.
It is tied to the student's self-esteem; the lower his self-esteem, the more violent sentiments and hostile conduct he shows against himself, others, or his belongings.
According to social and educational culture, extroverted people are defined by friendliness, social politeness, and a degree of emotional equilibrium accompanying the situation and managing its actions, according to Bakhshi and Sharma's [32] study.
However, to describe the three components of extroversion, openness, and conscientiousness that are adversely connected with aggressiveness, it may be stated that people with low extroversion scores are non-social and inactive. They do not wish to speak and express their feelings correctly.
People who lack openness do not embrace moral and social traits concepts and therefore have poorer health and behavioural stability. Furthermore, those who lack conscientiousness are non-purposeful, irregular, less conscientious, and less successful in managing their conduct [33]. It agreed with the study of Kokkinos, Karagianni, & Voulgaridou [27] that adolescents with poor conscientiousness were more likely to engage in reactive relational aggression when they made hostile attributions to relational provocations.
As a result, people with less control over their conduct are more prone to exhibiting violence. They also had a weak propensity to experience fear, a combination of low empathy, contempt for, and lack of intimate ties with others, leading to excessive proactive aggressiveness.
Neurotic people can have excessive emotional and behavioural responses. Because they are worried and easily agitated, they are predisposed to mental disease when emotional stress increases. As a result, they are more aggressive than people who exhibit emotional equilibrium. According to the findings of Gitonga, Chege and Karuku [29], various personality types would react to circumstances in either an aggressive or non-aggressive manner. People who are unable to resist their urges and temptations and engage in aggressive conduct are considered to have high degrees of neurosis. Furthermore, excessive flexibility can occasionally lead to violent conduct [33].
The agreeableness component describes people who are inclined toward interpersonal interactions and the needs of others. Trust, straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, humility, and tender-heartedness are all aspects of agreeableness. Antagonism is the polar opposite of agreeableness. Furthermore, people with high antagonism tend to mistrust and have a poor regard for others, and as a result, they act in ways that exclude or snub those who are despised or inferior. Antagonistic people may lack emotional expressiveness and interpersonal attachment—"they are frigid or cold, disdainful, heartless, and unfeeling." [34].
Surprisingly, data suggests that the existence of high neuroticism may not be sufficient on its own to encourage aggressive and violent conduct but must be combined with low agreeableness. Thus, one interpretation of the interaction between neuroticism and agreeableness is that high neuroticism makes the individual more sensitive to situational triggers such as provocations or perceived insults, which may facilitate hostile and aggressive behaviour when combined with low agreeableness. In turn, this might contribute to the detrimental influence on mental health associated with high neuroticism, since frequent aggressive and confrontational encounters with others may negatively reinforce the propensity to view the world and others' motives [35].
The findings revealed no statistically significant differences [a = 0.05] in the averages of students' replies on the overall scale based on gender. The result agreed with the study of Al-Damour [36].
The researcher attributes this reason to the fact that both genders have relatively complete freedom in expressing their feelings and behaviour. Also, they have openness to the world. Education helps in that, no matter how different the manifestations of aggressive behaviour between the sexes are, it eventually falls under aggressive behaviour.
It can be said that the similarity of the social environment and the public policy in which both sexes live helped ensure that there was no difference between them.
The findings revealed an effect statistically significant of the Big Five personality factors on aggression at the significance level [α = 0.05] among school students from the age category of [12-16 years] in the Jenin governorate due to the variable academic achievement.
The impact favoured low academic achievement, and aggressiveness rates were lower among kids with high academic achievement. The study by Barthelemy and Lounsbury [2009] demonstrates this and is consistent with Barthelemy's [37] study on the relationship between aggressiveness and student achievement scores.
Studies confirm the positive predictive relationship between school refusal and high levels of aggressive behaviour. As a result of these behaviours, characterized by a lack of emotional control, violent conduct may arise as a method of escape. How an individual responds in different situations may impact the expression of emotions such as rage or aggression, raising the chance of impulsive or violent conduct that may result in school rejection. As a result, emotionally susceptible children may behave aggressively to cope with difficult school settings. They exhibit symptoms of anxiety and depression [38].
In terms of predictive analysis, school refusal serves as a positive and statistically significant predictor of violence. The link between the two variables implies that, in most cases, the various causes of school rejection operate as predictors of high aggressiveness scores.
The findings revealed a favourable association between the averages of the students' responses and the overall score on the scale in favour of the city's pupils. The results showed high levels of aggressive behaviour among the students of the city's residents compared to the students of the camp and the village.
This may be attributed to the peculiarity of the city of Jenin in the face of repeated attacks by the occupation on a frequent basis.
Where the occupation stormed the city of Jenin and abused its residents, made its youth prisoners, and caused the martyrdom of others.
In fact, when such confrontations occur between the occupation and the youth in general, especially those aged 11–14, the temperamental, volatile, and unbalanced moods are exacerbated in some different situations in everyday life.
The loss and martyrdom of a person affect individuals as a whole from pent-up grief that comes out in a negative way in one way or another.

5. Conclusion

The study found that middle school students in Jenin Governorate displayed a high level of conscientiousness, with the neurotic factor ranking second. While there was some evidence of aggressive behavior, the overall level of aggressiveness was low, particularly towards others and property. The study attributes this to the efforts of families and schools to raise children with good behavior and protect them from external factors. There was a positive correlation between neuroticism and aggression, with other factors like extraversion, openness, and conscientiousness having an inverse relationship with aggressiveness. The study highlights the importance of psychological unloading mechanisms and self-esteem in preventing aggressive behavior. The findings suggest that different personality types react to circumstances in either an aggressive or non-aggressive manner, and people with less control over their conduct are more prone to exhibiting violence.

6. Limitations

The primary limitations of this research will be:
1. Sample of this research: a random sample of 314 male and female school students from the age category of [12-16 years] was selected to complete this study.
2. Study instruments include [Big-Five Factor Markers & Scales that measure aggression].
3. Time of this study. It took place in the first semester of the academic year 2021-2022.
4. And there were certain roadblocks, such as the Corona virus's health precautions, which limited direct engagement with schools and pupils, so they were reached by phone, social media, or by visiting their houses.

7. Ethical Consideration

Obtaining permission from An-Najah National University to carry out the study and gather data in order to achieve the desired outcomes. Inform the Palestinian Ministry of Education about the research community that will be used for the study. Obtaining formal permission from the student's guardian to participate in the survey research.
Note: see IRB Approval Letter in Appendix A.

Funding

No fund.

Conflicts of Interest

No conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Preprints 78567 i001

References

  1. Kazak, E.; Koyuncu, V. Undesired Student Behaviors, The Effects Of These Behaviors And Teachers’ Coping Methods. Anadolu Journal Of Educational Sciences International 2021, 11, 637–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Kenrick, D.T.; Funder, D.C. Profiting From Controversy: Lessons From The Person-Situation Debate. American Psychologist 1988, 43, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Fleeson, W. Moving Personality Beyond The Person-Situation Debate: The Challenge And The Opportunity Of Within-Person Variability. Current Directions In Psychological Science 2004, 13, 83–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Markon, K.E. Hierarchies In The Structure Of Personality Traits. Social And Personality Psychology Compass 2009, 3, 812–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Satchell, L.P.; Kaaronen, R.O.; Latzman, R.D. An Ecological Approach To Personality: Psychological Traits As Drivers And Consequences Of Active Perception. Social And Personality Psychology Compass 2021, 15, E12595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Diener, E.L., R. E., Personality traits. In R. Biswas-Diener & E. Diener (Eds). Noba textbook series Psychology, 2023.
  7. Feist, J.; Feist, G.J.; Roberts, T. Theories Of Personality, 6th ed.Mcgraw-Hill: New York, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  8. Al-Saleh, T. The degree of manifestations and causes of aggressive behavior among primary school students in government schools in the northern West Bank governorates, and ways to treat them from the teachers' point of view. 2012. [Google Scholar]
  9. Wani, M.A.; Sankar, R.; Raghavi, R.; Chinmaya, B. Aggression Among Annamalai University Students. Global Journal Of Intellectual & Development Disabilities 2017, 1, 001–004. [Google Scholar]
  10. Pratama, D.A.; Pali, M.; Nurcahyo, F.A. Pengaruh Kepribadian Berdasarkan The Big Five Personality Terhadap Kepuasan Kerja Karyawan Hotel. 2012. [Google Scholar]
  11. Palestinian Center for Human Rights. Targeted civilians: A PCHR report on the Israeli military offensive against the Gaza strip (27 December 2008 – 12 January 2009). Retrieved from http://www.pchrgaza.org/files/Reports/English/pdf_spec/gaza%20war%20report.pdf. 2009.
  12. Pervin, L.A.; Cervone, D.; John, O.P. Persönlichkeitstheorien; Reinhardt München: 2005.
  13. Landy, F.J.; Conte, J.M. Psicología Industrial: Introducción A La Psicología Industrial Y Organizacional; Mcgraw-Hill, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  14. John, O.P.; Srivastava, S. The Big-Five Trait Taxonomy: History, Measurement, And Theoretical Perspectives. 1999.
  15. Malik, G.; Singh, D. Personality Matters: Does An Individual's Personality Affect Adoption And Continued Use Of Green Banking Channels? International Journal Of Bank Marketing 2022, 40, 746–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dollard, J.; Doob, L.; Miller, N.; Mowrer, O.; Sears, R.; Ford, C.; Hovland, C.; Sollenberger, R. Frustration And Aggression 1939 New Haven; Conn Yale University Press.
  17. Clements, K.; Holtzworth-Munroe, A. Aggressive Cognitions Of Violent Versus Nonviolent Spouses. Cognitive Therapy And Research 2008, 32, 351–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Holley, S.R.; Ewing, S.T.; Stiver, J.T.; Bloch, L. The Relationship Between Emotion Regulation, Executive Functioning, And Aggressive Behaviors. Journal Of Interpersonal Violence 2017, 32, 1692–1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Karneli, Y.; Neviyarni, N.; Yulidar, I. Pengembangan Modul Konseling Kreatif Dalam Bingkai Kognitif Perilaku Untuk Menurunkan Perilaku Agresif Siswa. 2018.
  20. Keikha, M.; Qorbani, M.; Tabaee, M.S.K.; Djalalinia, S.; Kelishadi, R. Screen Time Activities And Aggressive Behaviors Among Children And Adolescents: A Systematic Review. International Journal Of Preventive Medicine 2020, 11. [Google Scholar]
  21. Sibarani, R. Overview Of Aggressive Behavior In School-Age Children At Sd Negeri 064990 Medan. Journal Of Midwifery And Nursing 2022, 4, 26–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bevilacqua, L.; Carli, V.; Sarchiapone, M.; George, D.K.; Goldman, D.; Roy, A.; Enoch, M.-A. Interaction Between Fkbp5 And Childhood Trauma And Risk Of Aggressive Behavior. Archives Of General Psychiatry 2012, 69, 62–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ohsako, T. Violence at School: Global Issues and Interventions. Studies in Comparative Education. 1997: ERIC.
  24. Dayakisni, T.; Hudaniah, P.S. Malang. Umm Press, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  25. Costa Jr, P.T.; Mccrae, R.R. The Five-Factor Model Of Personality And Its Relevance To Personality Disorders. Journal Of Personality Disorders 1992, 6, 343–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Al-Ansari, B.M. The Efficiency of the Big Five Factor List of personality in Kuwaiti society. Psychological studies, 1997. 7: p. p. 2.310-277.
  27. Kokkinos, C.M.; Karagianni, K.; Voulgaridou, I. Relational Aggression, Big Five And Hostile Attribution Bias In Adolescents. Journal Of Applied Developmental Psychology 2017, 52, 101–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cavalcanti, J.G.; Pimentel, C.E. Personalidade E Agressão: Uma Contribuição Do Modelo Geral Da Agressão. Estudos De Psicologia (Campinas) 2016, 33, 443–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gitonga, C.M.; Chege, E.; Karuku, S. Personality Types And Socio-Demographic Determinants Of Girls Aggressive Behaviors In Rehabilitation Programs In Kenya. International Journal Of Psychology And Counselling 2021, 13, 25–33. [Google Scholar]
  30. Barlett, C.P.; Anderson, C.A. Direct And Indirect Relations Between The Big 5 Personality Traits And Aggressive And Violent Behavior. Personality And Individual Differences 2012, 52, 870–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Antoñanzas, J.L. The Relationship Of Personality, Emotional Intelligence, And Aggressiveness In Students: A Study Using The Big Five Personality Questionnaire For Children And Adults (Bfq-Na). European Journal Of Investigation In Health, Psychology And Education 2020, 11, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Bakhshi, A.; Sharma, V. Big Five Personality Traits And Social Skills Of School Bullies And Victims. International Journal Of Health Sciences And Research 2019, 9, 223–227. [Google Scholar]
  33. Komasi, S.; Saeidi, M.; Soroush, A.; Zakiei, A. The Relationship Between Brain Behavioral Systems And The Characteristics Of The Five Factor Model Of Personality With Aggression Among Iranian Students. Journal Of Injury And Violence Research 2016, 8, 67. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  34. Bettencourt, B.; Talley, A.; Benjamin, A.J.; Valentine, J. Personality And Aggressive Behavior Under Provoking And Neutral Conditions: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological Bulletin 2006, 132, 751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Dam, V.H.; Hjordt, L.V.; Da Cunha-Bang, S.; Sestoft, D.; Knudsen, G.M.; Stenbæk, D.S. Trait Aggression Is Associated With Five-Factor Personality Traits In Males. Brain And Behavior 2021, 11, E02175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Al-Damour, M. Tthe relationship between personality patterns and aggressive behaviour for primary school students in Jordan, in Faculty of Educational and Psychological Sciences. 2011, Al-Ahliyya Amman University. Jordan: Almanthoma.
  37. Barthelemy, J.J. Aggression And The Big Five Personality Factors Of* Grades And Attendance; The University Of Tennessee, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gonzálvez, C.; Martín, M.; Vicent, M.; Sanmartín, R. School Refusal Behavior And Aggression In Spanish Adolescents. Frontiers In Psychology 2021, 12, 669438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Distribution and characteristics of the sample members according to demographic variable Academic achievement.
Table 1. Distribution and characteristics of the sample members according to demographic variable Academic achievement.
Variable Repetition Percentage
Academic achievement
Weak 26 8.28%
Good 55 17.5%
very good 135 43%
Excellent 98 31.21%
Total Summation 314 100%
Table 2. Distribution and characteristics of the sample members according to demographic variables Gender and Place of residence.
Table 2. Distribution and characteristics of the sample members according to demographic variables Gender and Place of residence.
Variable Repetition Percentage Variable Repetition Percentage
Gender Place of residence
Male 81 26% Village 92 29.25%
Female 233 74% Camp 8 2.5%
Total Summation 314 100% City 214 68.15%
Total Summation 314 100%
Table 3. One-Sample Test, the levels of aggression.
Table 3. One-Sample Test, the levels of aggression.
M S.D. DF Test Value = 2.6 Test Value = 1.8
T P T P
LEVEL1 2.1917 .81045 312 -8.913 .000 8.551 .000
LEVEL2 1.8860 .78959 311 -15.974 .000 1.923 .055
LEVEL3 1.6974 .67022 311 -23.787 .000 -2.703 .007
LEVELS 1.9319 .67852 308 -17.307 .000 3.418 .001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated