1. Introduction
The importance of vultures has always been recognized ecologically and culturally [
1,
2,
3]. Vultures are keystone species and they are important indicators of terrestrial biodiversity and the environmental health in different landscapes [
4,
5,
6]. The range of African vultures have declined rapidly over the past 30 years. Of the eight species of Africa’s vultures, seven of these species populations have declined by over 80% [
7,
8] and have been listed as critically endangered by the International Union for the Conservation of Nature) [
9]. The extent of vulture declines is complex and varies throughout the range. For example, in West Africa, some species may be extirpated especially in Nigeria, save for a few areas in Ghana and Niger where a substantial number of vultures persist [
10]. Declines are also recorded in Kenya, Somalia, South Sudan and Sudan [
7,
11], although their status is apparently more stable in Uganda, Tanzania and parts of Southern Africa [
12]. In Southern Africa, vulture declines have been estimated at 90% (range 75–95%) over the last three decades [
7]. Many countries have therefore set species action plans to mitigate pressures facing the vultures. In this regard, studies that focus on the feeding ecology of vultures have also become very important across the African range.
Most African vulture species assemble at dead animals and they play different critical roles on the carcass while feeding. Resource partitioning by vultures may be influenced by many factors, both behavioral and extrinsic [
13,
14]. Aggressive encounters and interspecific competition have been minimized through various forms of partitioning. The differences in body size and beak morphology have been documented to facilitate the consumption of different body parts on large carcasses [
15,
16]. Body size also has a strong influence on dominance hierarchies [
17] and thus resource partitioning in several guilds of vultures [
18]. It is also been noted that the carcass decomposition state has a significant effect on the number and proportions of vultures feeding on it [
19].
Spatio-temporal apportioning of resources by vultures has been assessed through studies of differences in habitat use and resource dispersion [
20]. For example, the exploitation of carcasses by the Cape Vulture
Gyps coprotheres and White-backed Vulture
Gyps africanus were dependent on the bird’s abilities to take flight in different vegetation structure types in southern Africa [
21]. In other cases outside the African continent, social dominance factors were also found to be critical when the migrant and resident Turkey Vultures
Cathartes aura utilized various habitat types and foraging strategies [
22]. Resource distribution, whether aggregated or scattered, appears to be important in determining the community composition of scavengers at ungulate carcasses in temperate-zone ecosystems [
14]. Competition with predators at carcasses may also influence the scavengers’ community structure at carcasses killed by mammalian predators [
23]. There are however fewer studies that explore how the vulture interactions may affect the food acquisition in African vultures.
This study seeks to investigate the vulture composition and interactions on carcasses found in Sinamatella, Hwange National Park, Zimbabwe. We hypothesized that 1) vulture composition while feeding on dead animals is influenced by the carcass species and stage of decomposition and 2) the large bodied vulture species tend to dominate in the inter-species interactions towards food acquisition. This work is important as it provides information on vulture composition in a typical protected area that exists in a mosaic of other land uses such as mining and human settlements.
2. Materials and Methods
The study was carried out in Sinamatella section of Hwange National Park (HNP which is 14 651km
2 in size and is the largest National Park in Zimbabwe). HNP is located between latitudes 19°7'26.59"S and longitude 26°35'33"E and has an average altitude of 1000m above sea level. Sinamatella section is managed as an Intensive Protection Zone (IPZ) and it covers an area of 1 328 km
2 (as illustrated in
Figure 1).
We collected data from animal carcasses that were opportunistically found and the site could be reached with reasonable effort (i.e. within 5km from an established road/fire guard). Camera traps (
Bushnel 119447 C) were set (one per carcass site over daytime hours only), and programmed to take 2 minute videos after every 5 minutes from the day the carcass was discovered until vultures disappeared from it. The batteries used for these cameras could last for more than 10 days, which was sufficient to cover the whole period until vultures disappeared from the carcass. The camera trap method ensured minimal disturbances to predators/scavengers by humans. For the extraction of data from the camera traps, the Time-lapse software was used where a template of containing the date, time of the day, decomposition state of the carcass, scavenger species, numbers, interactions (both inter and intra-specific) and duration of interaction was designed. The videos from the camera traps were analyzed manually while capturing the information onto the Time Lapse designed data template. In addition to the information captured on Time Lapse, the carcass species, carcass GPS position, the initial carcass decomposition state and vegetation characteristics of the carcass site was also recorded. From the videos, scavenger interactions were recorded as grabbing meat, pecking, chasing away and stalking. The carcass decomposition state was recorded as fresh (defined as the time the animal dies up to the state before signs of bloating show up); active decay (the carcass was at the bloating stage/increasing in size); advanced decay (defined by the shrinking in size of the carcass after its rupture when body fluids flow out and flesh rotting) and dry (when most flesh has been consumed and there is no more water coming from out of the carcass). These classifications were a modification from Valverde, Espín [
24]. The captured information from the Time Lapse template was exported as an excel file for analysis.
The vegetation type was categorized into three broad classes i.e. closed woodland (dense vegetation, with trees within 3m of each other), open woodland (sparse vegetation, with trees greater than 3m from each other) and riverine (carcasses on a river bed or within 50m river/stream banks) [
25]. The time spent by the scavenger species being engaged in particular interactions was expressed in seconds. A new variable “offender” was derived in which the individual that was dominating/winning a foraging opportunity in an interaction was noted. Another variable “victim” was created in which the individual that was being subdued/losing a feeding opportunity in an interaction was noted. In the cases of intraspecific interactions, the offender and the victim was the same species. A table was used to illustrate the composition of scavengers per carcass under a given decomposition state. Beyond the description of the scavenger community on the carcass, further analysis excluded non-vulture species as our aim was focused on vultures. To separate the observations on a carcass, a new variable “carcass state ID” was developed which was unique for each carcass, at a particular location and during a given decomposition state. The maximum number of vultures recorded per carcass state ID was extracted. The package “vegan” was used to calculate the Shannon diversity index of the vulture community per carcass state ID [
26].
The relationships between the response variables (Shannon diversity and vulture abundance) and the dependent variables (vegetation type and decomposition state) were explored using generalized linear models (glms) since the response variables failed to conform to normality assumptions even after transformations attempts. The package ggplot2 were used to illustrate the results.
Subsets of cases under which each vulture species was the offender (i.e. it was benefiting from the interaction in terms food acquisition) were created. In these subsets, the duration of interaction time also failed to meet normality assumptions. Accordingly, glms were performed (one for each vulture species observed) to test if the duration of the offenders` interaction time was affected by the vegetation type, carcass species and the interaction between the victim and the carcass decomposition state. All data sorting and analysis were performed in the R Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [
27].
4. Discussion
The carcass detection ability of most vulture species and the landscape of Sinamatella section of HNP allows some discussion of the observed scavenger composition and vulture interactions. Although it is acknowledged that the sample size was relatively low (for example, there was only one observed carcasses for the giraffe and kudu species), the known feeding patterns of vultures allow some analysis given the frequency of capturing of video clips over day time hours in this study (between run rise and sunset).
Vulture diversity and abundance on carcasses and decomposition states
Contrary to the hypothesis of this study that vulture diversity would be different across decomposition states and habitat types, the diversity of vulture species were not different across vegetation types and decomposition states. However, the early stages of carcass decomposition (fresh stage, active and advanced decay stages) had a greater abundance of vultures than the dry stages of decomposition. The study conducted by Mundy, Butchart [
6] revealed that the White-backed and the Lappet-faced Vultures normally arrived early at carcasses and have the capacity to open up the carcass skin. Also, these two vulture species have been observed in high numbers at carcasses that still had flesh [
4]. This study was probably unable to detect such differences as vultures were already gathered by the time the camera traps were set. Thus a better description would be given if carcasses could be monitored from the time the animal die, and this could be practical when sick/weak animals are followed or where an intact carcass can be experimentally set up. Such experimental set ups would need to be need to be varied across species with soft skins and those with hard ones [
28].
Although this study had hypothesized that large bodied carcasses would host more vultures than small ones, this trend was not significant. What is clear however, is that the four vulture species that were recorded in this study seem to be congregating in similar proportions across carcass decomposition states. It therefore suggested that the amount of meat available on a carcass plays an important role in determining the vulture activities on a feeding patch in Sinamatella. To have a better understanding of the effect of carcass body sizes, this study may need to be extended so that small animals are also included. As the savannah system has a large guild of herbivores [
29,
30], smaller herbivores such as impalas could be experimentally set-up. In such set-ups, other mammalian scavengers may need to be excluded to prolong the longevity of the monitoring period on the carcass [
31].
Diversity and abundance of vultures on differing habitat types
As hypothesized, the results from this study showed that there were more vultures in open woodlands than in closed woodlands and riverine habitats. This could be due to the fact that in open woodlands, carcasses were easily located than in the closed woodland and the riverine habitats. Hunter, Durant [
32] confirmed that the White-backed Vultures located carrion by sight only and they rarely discovered carcasses concealed in trees or otherwise covered [
33]. Indeed this study observed more White-backed Vultures and some marabou storks in carcasses in riverine habitats when compared to the open woodlands. It is however acknowledged that since this study was mostly conducted in the dry season, most trees had shed off their leaves and somehow the effect of vegetation may have been suppressed.
The diversity of vultures were however similar across the habitat types, which was contrary to our hypothesis. We believe that food scarcity in the system [
34,
35] and the general ability of vultures to discover carcasses [
36] probably explain the insignificant results. As already discussed, the status of the trees during the dry season did not result in strongly different scenarios which would be detectable in terms of species diversity – more so for a highly specialized guild like vultures [
37].
Vulture dominance and interactions on actively decaying carcasses
The White-backed Vultures were observed dominating the other three vulture species on actively decaying carcasses. The beaks of White-backed Vultures are medium-sized [
38] and they are not adapted for tearing tough skin [
39] (hence less dominating occasions on fresh carcasses). Thus they are known for eating soft tissues, such as the viscera, using a pulling feeding tactic [
6]. The White-backed Vultures are the first to arrive on carcasses in relatively large numbers when compared to other vultures and thus their dominance could be partly due to their numerical advantage.
The Hooded Vultures were observed dominating only in intraspecific interactions and against other non-vulture avian species. This may be accounted to the small body size of this species, which may be linked to their inferiority when competing with the large vultures species like the White-headed and the Lappet-faced Vultures. The average weight of the Hooded Vultures range from 1.5-2.6kg [
40] is lighter when compared to the White-backed Vultures 4.2-7.2kg [
41], White-headed Vultures (female average weight of4.7kg) [
42] and Lappet-faced Vultures (4.4-9.4kg) [
43]. These species are unable to access much material on carcasses and instead, they feed primarily on scraps on the ground around carcasses [
44]. The Hooded Vultures may be attracted to other vultures landing as a cue to carrion. They are also known for supplementing food from carrion by actively hunting. [
4] also noted that Hooded Vulture have a more varied diet, using several food sources other than carrion, but when feeding at a carcass tend to peck on scraps.
The White-headed Vultures were observed dominating other vultures except the White-backed Vultures. Despite the large size of the White-headed Vultures, the numerical advantages of the White-backed Vultures probably play an important role in winning over the White-headed Vultures. Monadjem, Anderson [
12] confirmed that White-headed Vultures assemble at carcasses in small numbers (as they are mostly solitary). Kemp, Kirwan [
45] also confirmed that the White-headed Vulture is unique among African vultures because it does not rely on large carcasses, but often scouts for smaller carcasses with less competition. Also, Kemp, Kirwan [
45] noted that White-headed Vultures do not rely on carrion for survival but rather they are known to hunt and kill their own prey such as stranded fish, mongoose, lizards, snakes, insects, piglets, and birds [
46]. Future studies may also assess the contribution of rodents, fish, and lizards to the feeding ecology of vultures.
The Lappet-faced Vulture were never recorded dominating over other vulture species. This is in contracts to other observations, for example, Hockey, Dean [
47], noted that they are “dominant over all other species at carcasses”. This may be explained by the fact that the other vulture species actually know the Lappet-faced Vultures are dominant and simply keep out of their way and thus there may be no physical dominance. Hockey, Dean [
47] also reported that the Lappet-faced Vultures “may loaf for hours near carcass after feeding”. Thus if the individual Lappet-faced Vultures that were clear on the video footages were actively not feeding then the dominant behavior could have been missed.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, T.T. and L.M.; methodology, L.M., S.L and T.M, software, L.M.; validation, L.M. and T.M; formal analysis, T.T. L.M and T.M; investigation, T.T.; resources, S.L.; data curation, L.M.; writing—original draft preparation, L.M.; writing—review and editing, T.T. and T.M; visualization, T.T.; supervision, T.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.