1. Introduction
Several lines of evidence link sleep with brain functions that depend on brain plasticity, such as brain development, experience-dependent brain plasticity and memory [
1,
2,
3]. High resolution imaging techniques have provided strong evidence that sleep induces morphological changes at synapses (i.e., structural plasticity) in developing and adult rodents, supporting a model in which both strengthening and weakening of synaptic connections occur during sleep (reviewed in [
4,
5,
6]). However, the molecular mechanisms that contribute to these plastic changes during sleep remain still unclear.
One process that we, and others, have shown to be modulated during sleep is protein synthesis (reviewed in [
5]), which is critical for the consolidation of memory [
7,
8] and developmental brain plasticity [
9]. The importance of mRNA translation in sleep was supported by early studies that measured radioactive amino acid incorporation during protein synthesis in the brain. It revealed that translation rates increased during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep in the brain of rats and primates [
10,
11]. Furthermore, it was found that the levels of regulators of translation initiation and elongation, such as eukaryotic initiation factor 3 (eIF3), eIF5, eukaryotic elongation factor 2 (eEF2) and mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR), were elevated during sleep compared to wakefulness [
12,
13,
14]. Finally, activation of translation, via phosphorylation of the mTOR-eIF4E-binding proteins (4E-BPs) pathway, has been directly implicated in memory and experience-dependent brain plasticity consolidation [
15,
16].
4E-BPs is an important regulator of cap-dependent translation initiation as it binds to and represses the activity of eIF4E. When 4E-BPs is phosphorylated, it releases eIF4E which can assemble with eIF4A and eIF4G to form the eIF4F complex at the 5′ cap structure of mRNAs. Once formed, the eIF4F complex initiate ribosomes recruitment, necessary for translation initiation [
17,
18]. There are 3 isoforms of the 4E-BPs family that can bind to eIF4E: 4EBP1, 4EBP2, and 4EBP3 [
19,
20]. Those isoforms differ in terms of expression, with 4E-BP2 being the most highly expressed in the brain [
17,
20,
21]. Depending on translation activation needs, 4E-BPs can be present in its hypophosphorylated (α), phosphorylated (β) and hyperphosphorylated (γ) forms, which can be distinguished by their different electrophoretic mobility on SDS-PAGE. The hyperphosporylated (γ) form is considered to fully release its binding to the translation initiation factor eIF4E and allows for cap-dependent translation to initiate. Sequential phosphorylation at various residues regulates the different phosphorylation forms [
22,
23]. Thr37/46 are the first two sites to be phosphorylated, followed by Thr70 and finally Ser65, leading to the fully, hyperphosphorylated 4E-BPs forms. Therefore, phosphorylation of 4E-BPs at Thr37/46 is a good marker of cap-dependent translation initiation and probing these sites allow to assess to conversion from hypo- to hyperphosphorylated forms [
18,
24,
25,
26,
27].
In the context of sleep, Tudor et al. [
16] showed that if sleep was prevented after learning, using short (5 hours) sleep deprivation (SD), memory consolidation was impaired and 4E-BPs phosphorylation at Thr37/46 decreased in the hippocampus of mice. Remarkably, rescuing hippocampal 4E-BPs phosphorylation in sleep deprived mice, rescued memory performance, thus linking 4E-BPs phosphorylation to both sleep and cognitive function [
16]. We showed that pharmacological inhibition of the mTOR pathway in the cortex of kitten during sleep, but not wakefulness, impaired sleep-dependent enhancement of a form of developmental plasticity in the visual cortex (i.e., ocular dominance plasticity) [
15]. Moreover, 4E-BPs phosphorylation at Ser65, which is indicative of the fully phosphorylated γ form, in the same model was dependent on both sleep and visual experience. These data suggest experience-dependent increase of translation initiation during sleep in the cortex [
15].
Within neurons, mRNA translation can occur locally at synapses and all the components of the translational machinery are present near or at synapses, including 4E-BPs [
15,
28]. Localised mRNA translation near or at synapses facilitates the synthesis of proteins that are required to remodel neuronal connections in a synapse-specific manner [
29,
30,
31]. While 4E-BPs seem to be important for plasticity mechanisms occurring during sleep, it is currently not known whether sleep-dependent 4E-BPs phosphorylation occurs specifically only at synapses or other parts of neurons as well. To address this question, we exposed rats to an enriched environment (EE) to induce brain plasticity and assessed the influence of sleep and experience on 4E-BPs phosphorylation at synapses located in the cortex and cerebellum. EEG recordings in the same animals allowed to further investigate change in brain activity induced by EE exposure and their association with 4E-BPs measures.
4. Discussion
We investigated the effects of short-term EE exposure on EEG and 4E-BPs phosphorylation as molecular marker of plasticity consolidation during waking experience and subsequent sleep. We used a paradigm that is composed of a set of various sensorimotor, cognitive, and social stimuli to trigger global brain plasticity enhancement. Importantly, to untangle the effects of sustained wakefulness from those of complex stimuli processing on brain physiology, we compared measures from rats that were maintained awake in standard as well as in EE cages. This comparison is important as current studies looking at the effect of sleep deprivation or learning on sleep and plasticity measures use paradigms that often mix both interventions, leaving open the questions of their respective contribution to physiological read outs [
37]. We found that staying awake in an EE or in a HC shares common electrophysiological and molecular features in the brain but also exhibits specific changes across the sleep and wake.
Sleep following EE enhances conversion of 4E-BPs to hyperphosphorylated state at synapses.
At the molecular level, we found that the first hours of the rest/light phase, with or without sleep, promotes overall phosphorylation of the translational repressor 4E-BPs at synapses across the cortex and cerebellum. A circadian control of 4E-BPs phsophorlyation was also detectable in whole cells, suggesting a cell-wide regulation (
Figure 4B). This confirms previous findings in the hippocampus of mice showing a gradual increase in 4E-BPs phosphorylation during the light phase, peaking at ZT8 [
49]. However, in the later study, neither phosphorylation forms nor sleep-wake cycle were considered. We show in this study that exposure of rats to an EE prior to sleep led to substantial conversion of 4E-BPs to its hyperphosphorylated (γ) form, which is considered a necessary step for the efficient initiation of cap-dependent translation [
17], only visible at synapses. Our data thus suggest that 4E-BPs hyperphosphorylation (γ form) at synapses is mainly driven by the combination of experience with sleep, and that circadian rhythms also contribute to the overall regulation of 4E-BPs phosphorylation levels cell-wide (
Figure 4B).
Our results are novel for several reasons. First, while phosphorylation of 4E-BPs has been linked to sleep and experience [
15,
16], this is the first study showing that sleep, in combination with experience, altered 4E-BPs phosphorylation specifically at synapses, an effect that is not seen when total cellular fraction is probed (
Figure 4 and
Figure S4). Furthermore, very few in vivo studies investigated changes in the different 4E-BPs phosphorylation forms (but see Ayuso et al., 2010), which seems to be significantly modulated by the sleep-wake cycle and experience as shown here. Our results also align with a recent large-scale transcriptomics and proteomics study in brain synapses [
50]. Specifically, the study showed a complementary role for circadian and sleep-wake cycles in the regulation of the synaptic transcriptome and proteome, respectively [
50]. While only hypothesized in the study, we provide support for a role for sleep in the conversion of the circadian-regulated mRNAs into function proteins at synapses. Our results further suggest that sleep-dependent mRNA translation at synapses is modulated by the previous waking content through increased 4E-BPs phosphorylation. In the context of plasticity, 4E-BPs expression and phosphorylation has been implicated in memory consolidation [
16,
22], conversion of early to late-LTP [
22] and long-term LTD [
48] in the hippocampus. This suggests that 4E-BPs supports various forms of long-term plasticity which expression depends probably on the translation of different pools of mRNAs. The types of plasticity supported by 4E-BPs during sleep in the cortex and cerebellum remain to be determined and may well depend on the types of experiences prior to sleep. Thus, to understand the role of sleep in brain plasticity and memory, it will be important to investigate the pools of mRNAs that are translated at synapses during sleep in relation to the types of waking experiences, especially given the variety of paradigms that are currently used in the sleep and brain plasticity field (e.g., fear learning, perceptual learning, spatial learning, novelty, sleep deprivation [
37]).
Effect of enriched environment on brain activity across wakefulness and sleep
In terms of brain activity, we show that 3 hours of SD is enough to trigger a sleep homeostasis response as illustrated by an increase in NREM Slow and Delta oscillations (0.5–4 Hz) in both groups, that was larger, especially in the frontal cortex, when wakefulness is accompanied by complex stimuli (i.e., EE) (
Figure 3B). Since the only difference between the groups is the exposure of rats to a stimulating environment known to induce plasticity, our results suggest that NREM Slow and Delta oscillations, also termed slow wave activity (SWA), which is a measure usually associated with sleep homeostasis, may be more sensitive to wake content rather than wake duration. Our results thus align with the proposition that a marker of sleep homeostasis (i.e., SWA) is correlated with increased brain plasticity [
51]. More systematic studies testing various types of wake content and length would provide important insight into this question and clarify the driver and nature of sleep homeostasis.
Compared to a similar awake period in a HC, rats exposed to an EE also display a larger increase in faster oscillations in the theta to beta (4–30 Hz) range during NREM sleep (
Figure 3B,C). Similar observations have been made by others in rats [
52] and in the 129/Ola mice strain [
53] when animals were sleep deprived with methods that stimulate explorative behaviour. It is important to stress the fact that changes in slower and faster oscillations show reverse dynamics (
Figure S3A) during post-EE NREM sleep which may support different functions for those oscillations. Future studies looking at experience-dependent EEG changes during sleep should focus more on the fine grain dynamics covering the entire frequency spectrum to understand those changes. Associated with the faster oscillations, spindles density during NREM sleep also increased following experience (
Figure 3D). Similar changes have been previously reported in both humans and rodents and tend to occur within the first hours of sleep following a learning experience (reviewed in [
54]). These results support the long standing observation that among the different spindles’ characteristics, density varies the most in relation to cognition, with a specific decrease in spindle density in many brain disorders (reviewed in [
43]). Among the faster oscillations, the most significant increase was observed for beta oscillations (
Figure 3C). Similar to a previous study published in rats [
35], we found that spindle density correlates with both sigma and beta power changes during NREM sleep in individuals (
Figure 3D), suggesting a close relationship between spindles with sigma
and beta oscillations in rats. Two functional interpretations can be put forward that are not mutually exclusive. First, this relation reflects the fact that spindle activity is coupled with beta rhythms in the cortex, as shown in humans [
55,
56,
57]. The role of NREM beta oscillations in the context of plasticity and memory is not known. A more provocative interpretation is that spindle themselves possess a frequency in rats that extend beyond the traditional sigma band, to the lower beta band, which is supported by spectral analysis of individual spindles in local field potential recordings in this species (see
Figure 2A in [
43]). In this case, our results align with a selective role of high frequency spindles (e.g., ‘fast spindles”) during brain development and memory (review in [
43,
54]). A link between fast oscillations and brain plasticity is further supported by our observation that spindle frequency, and not density or duration, was positively correlated with 4E-BPs hyperphosphorylation at synapses (
Figure 5D).
One of the most robust results we report is the increase of oscillatory activity in the alpha frequency band during AW when rats are housed in the EE (
Figure 2B). Reports of brain activity, in particular EEG,
during sleep deprivation and/or novel object exposure are rare. Some studies using novel objects as wake stimulant in rodents have reported EEG and show a similar increase in the alpha power band (8–10 Hz peak) but do not highlight or discuss these results [
52,
58,
59]. Using novel objects exposure and cage change, Franken et al. found an ~150% increase from baseline after 6 hours of SD during the light phase in rats [
52] while Huber and colleagues show an increase of more than 300% after only 4 hours of SD during the light phase in mice [
59]. While the increase observed is specific to frequencies in the alpha band (8–12 Hz), our data show that it is the result of a shift in the dominant theta peak during AW (peak from 8 Hz to 9 Hz,
Figure 2B). Changes in theta frequencies in the hippocampus during exploration is a well-known phenomenon and has been associated with the kinetics of behaviour [
60,
61]. It is thus not surprising to observe such as shift in theta peak mainly in the parietal cortex, which EEG is strongly contaminated by hippocampal activity, and in the EE cages, when the animals are actively exploring for hours. The amplitude of the shifted theta peak during AW, represented by frequencies in the alpha band in our study, was correlated with NREM beta oscillations and spindle duration during subsequent sleep (
Figure 3F) suggesting a relationship between amounts of exploration and experience-dependent EEG changes in sleep. We also found that theta activity (4–9 Hz) during AW correlated with 4E-BPs hyperphosphorylation at synapses (
Figure 5A,B). This means that information other than movements carried by theta oscillations, such as attention and time coding [
60], may contribute to experience-dependent changes in the sleeping brain.
The relations we observe between EEG and 4E-BPs phosphorylation are interesting but should be taken with a grain of salt due to their correlative nature. To understand the role of neural oscillations in brain plasticity and memory, future experiments should include different types of experiences and manipulations of brain activity to explore further the link between theta activity during wake and sleep spindle oscillations with various molecular markers of plasticity during sleep.