1. Introduction
Epilepsy is a chronic neurological disorder characterized by recurrent, spontaneous seizures resulting from abnormal bioelectrical activity in the brain [
1]. This prevalent neural pathology affects approximately 50-65 million individuals worldwide [
2,
3]. One of the most common forms of epilepsy is temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), in which the epileptogenic focus is located in the temporal lobe [
4]. The hallmark of TLE is the degeneration of hippocampal neurons, particularly in the areas of CA1 and CA3 [
5].
TLE also results in the manifestation of associated cognitive and psychoemotional disturbances that adversely affect patients' quality of life [
6]. Memory impairments are a common cognitive issue in patients with epilepsy [
7], with patients suffering from TLE particularly affected due to epileptogenic foci affecting memory consolidation structures, including the hippocampus [
8]. Additionally, epilepsy patients have a higher risk of developing anxiety disorders, personality disorders, psychosis, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [
9]. Patients with TLE experience challenges in social interactions, specifically in comprehending the mental state of others and identifying emotions [
10,
11].
Despite continuous research and the availability of anti-seizure medications, approximately 30% of patients with epilepsy do not achieve complete remission [
12]. Existing medications can prevent seizures but do not address the underlying mechanisms of epileptogenesis or prevent the development of epilepsy [
13,
14]. Additionally, many available anti-seizure medications have adverse psychological effects on patients [
15,
16]. Therefore, it is crucial to discover new treatment methods for epilepsy.
By focusing on the underlying disease mechanisms and primary signaling pathways, it may be possible to prevent or halt the progression of epileptogenesis and provide more effective treatments for epilepsy [
14]. Unfortunately, the precise mechanisms of epileptogenesis are still not completely understood, and potential targets for therapy development remain hypothetical [
17,
18].
Several mechanisms contribute to the development of epilepsy, and neuroinflammation is one of them [
19,
20,
21,
22]. Neuroinflammation has been described as a common pathogenic mechanism promoting seizures in animal models of acquired epilepsy and drug-resistant epilepsy in humans. Neuroinflammation involves structural and functional changes in glial and immune cells in the central nervous system, along with dysfunction of the blood-brain barrier (BBB). This imbalance results in heightened production of inflammatory mediators such as IL-1β, IL-6, TNF-α, IFN-γ [
20].
Previous studies, including our own, have demonstrated that anakinra, an interleukin-1 receptor antagonist, reduces epileptogenesis and improves seizure-related outcomes [
23,
24,
25]. Anakinra blocks IL-1 receptor-mediated signaling, which might modulate the inflammatory response and decrease neuronal hyperexcitability. Administering anakinra during the latent phase of the lithium-pilocarpine model significantly reduced the duration and frequency of spontaneous recurrent seizures (SRS) in rats during the chronic phase. Additionally, anakinra prevented certain behavioral impairments, such as motor hyperactivity and disturbances in social interactions, during both the latent and chronic phases. Histological analysis showed that anakinra also reduced neuronal loss in the CA1 and CA3 regions of the hippocampus, but did not prevent astro- and microgliosis [
23].
Lamotrigine, an anticonvulsant drug, is commonly prescribed for managing epilepsy. Its efficacy is linked to its ability to block voltage-gated sodium channels, which reduces abnormal electrical activity in the brain [
26]. More recently, evidence indicates that lamotrigine has antiepileptogenic properties. Lamotrigine pretreatment, administered at doses of 10 and 20 mg/kg, resulted in a significant decrease in seizure stages and generalized seizure durations in the rat pentylenetetrazole kindling model [
27]. Additionally, electrophysiological studies illustrated that lamotrigine pretreatment eliminated the heightened population spike amplitude in the hippocampus [
27]. The study by Stratton et al. (2003) demonstrated the antiepileptogenic-like effects of lamotrigine in a rat amygdala kindling model [
28]. However, a subsequent study by Nissinen et al. (2004) did not show whether lamotrigine had disease modifying or antiepileptogenic effects [
29]. In a rat lithium-pilocarpine model of TLE, Wang et al. (2019) found that lamotrigine decreased the frequency of SRS in a dose-dependent manner, and limited neuronal loss as well as astrogliosis in the hippocampus [
30].
Since both anakinra and lamotrigine have antiepileptogenic properties but work through differing mechanisms, we compared their individual and combined effects. We utilized the lithium-pilocarpine model to reproduce the major phases of epileptogenesis specific to temporal lobe epilepsy. Our assessment involved measuring the occurrence of SRS with different treatment options, as well as neuronal death and behavioral characteristics of the animals.
3. Discussion
In the present study, the rat lithium-pilocarpine TLE model [
34] was utilized to examine the effects of anakinra and lamotrigine alone and in combination on epileptogenesis. Administration of these drugs during the initial 10-day period following pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus lessened the severity of SRS. In addition, anakinra and lamotrigine alone and in combination significantly ameliorated a number of behavioral deficits and reduced, but did not completely abolish, hippocampal neuronal loss. The effectiveness of the combined treatment did not significantly vary from that of anakinra and lamotrigine monotherapy. Thus, it can be inferred that both anakinra and lamotrigine have a disease-modifying effect in this TLE model.
Since acquired epilepsy often appears to be pharmacoresistant, prevention of epileptogenesis is an important goal [
35]. Epileptogenesis is the complex process by which a healthy brain becomes epileptic. Several animal models are used to study epileptogenesis and provide valuable insights into the underlying mechanisms of epilepsy [
36]. These models allow researchers to study various aspects of epileptogenesis, including seizure development, neuronal loss, synaptic reorganization, neuroinflammation, and metabolic changes in the brain. Some commonly used animal models to study epileptogenesis include: (1) kindling models, in which epileptic activity is induced by repeated application of low-dose convulsant drugs or electrical stimulation of specific brain regions, such as the amygdala [
37]. (2) Post-status epilepticus (post-SE) models in which high doses of a convulsant agent such as kainate or pilocarpine are injected systemically [
37]; (3) brain injury models, in which epilepsy develops after brain damage or stroke [
38,
39]. Compared to kindling models, post-SE lithium-pilocarpine model is deemed more reliable in identifying drugs with antiepileptogenic properties. For instance, in models of kindling, drugs are usually given before the daily kindling sessions [
27,
28], making it impossible to rule out the possibility that the anticonvulsant impact of the drugs mitigates kindling-dependent epileptogenesis [
30].
Evidence of antiepileptogenic efficacy is increasing for numerous compounds. In a recent review, the authors listed 156 compounds with published reports of antiepileptogenic efficacy [
40]. In this study, we tested antiepileptogenic efficacy of lamotrigine and anakinra. Lamotrigine is a conventional antiepileptic medication utilized to treat both focal and generalized epilepsy [
41]. The pharmacological effect of lamotrigine involves the blockage of potential-dependent sodium [
42] and N- and P/Q-type calcium channels on presynaptic nerve terminals [
43,
44]. Lamotrigine prevents excessive release of glutamate, protecting nerve cells from glutamate-induced neurotoxicity [
45,
46]. After status epilepticus induced by pilocarpine, neuronal networks become more excitable with pathologically high background activation of the glutamatergic system [
47,
48]. To minimize this effect, we employed lamotrigine in our study. Previously, various experimental models of epilepsy, including the lithium-pilocarpine model of temporal lobe epilepsy, have shown the neuroprotective and anti-epileptogenic effects of lamotrigine [
30,
49].
Another factor in favor of choosing lamotrigine was its ability to positively impact the mental state of patients, setting it apart from numerous other anti-epileptic medications that can trigger psycho-emotional and cognitive dysfunctions. Research has demonstrated that lamotrigine displays a strong antidepressant impact, making it suitable for mood stabilization in individuals with bipolar disorder [
50]. It should be also noted that lamotrigine has demonstrated the potential to inhibit the release of proinflammatory cytokines in models of neuroinflammation [
51].
Anakinra, a recombinant interleukin-1 receptor antagonist, has shown potential in the treatment of some types of epilepsy, particularly in febrile infection-related epilepsy syndrome (FIRES)[
24,
52] and as antiepileptogenic drug [
23]. The mechanisms by which anakinra exerts its effects in epilepsy are not fully understood. However, it is believed to change expression of various genes, modulate the immune response and reduce inflammation [
23,
53,
54]. Neuroinflammation is associated with increased production of proinflammatory cytokines such as IL- β, IL-6, TNF-α, and others. Increased expression of pro-inflammatory genes, particularly IL-1b and TNF-α, was found in the hippocampus and anterior temporal cortex of patients with TLE and hippocampal sclerosis [
55,
56]. High levels of IL-1β enhance excitation in the CNS by increasing the release of excitatory transmitters such as glutamate or ATP [
57]. IL-1β enhances NMDA-mediated Ca2+ influx into the cell [
58], which may induce the characteristic hippocampal neuronal death seen in epilepsy [
59]. In addition, IL-1β may increase neuronal excitability by down-modulating the astrocytic glutamate transporter (GLT-1) [
60], which results in impaired glutamate clearance that has been identified as one of the causative factors in drug-resistant epilepsy [
61]. Increased levels of proinflammatory cytokines, including IL-1, may not only be associated with increased excitability, but may also be responsible for the development of behavioral abnormalities characteristic of epilepsy [
62]. We and other researchers have previously demonstrated that IL-1 receptor blockade therapy decreases seizure development and neurodegenerative changes in the brain, as well as reducing the severity of comorbid behavioral disorders. However, it does not completely prevent them [
23,
63,
64].
One of the key elements in epileptogenesis is neuronal death, which has been widely studied in the context of acquired epileptogenesis [
65]. Traditionally, it has been proposed that neuronal death is necessary for epileptogenesis, as the loss of synaptic input from dying neurons is considered a critical signal to induce axonal sprouting and rewiring [
65]. However, recent studies have challenged this concept and suggested that neuronal death may not be essential for epileptogenesis, particularly in the immature brain [
65]. Our study demonstrates that both lamotrigine and anakinra provide neuroprotective effects and reduce neuronal death in the hippocampus, but they do not fully prevent spontaneous recurrent seizures. Thus, our data are consistent with the results of previous studies, which also showed that preventing neuronal loss is an important but not always sufficient factor to prevent epileptogenesis [
66,
67,
68].
In our study, we also aimed to investigate the effect of drugs on behavioral disturbances in rats. This was motivated by the observation that a comorbid diagnosis of epilepsy and psychiatric disorders was found to predict pharmacoresistance [
69]. Analogous findings have also been reported in a rat model of epilepsy [
70]. Another point for this study is that epilepsy patients also have a higher risk of developing anxiety disorders, psychosis, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and various personality disorders [
9]. Patients with TLE have difficulties in social interaction, especially in modeling the mental state of others and recognizing emotions [
10,
11]. In addition, patients with TLE and hippocampal sclerosis show impairments in several types of memory [
8,
71] and social behavior [
72], which may also be related to hippocampal dysfunction [
73].
Similar behavioral abnormalities are observed in animal models of temporal lobe epilepsy. Pilocarpine and lithium-pilocarpine rodent models of TLE are characterized by hyperactivity, impaired memory, social behavior, and anxiety [
33,
74,
75], which was confirmed in the present study. Behavioral abnormalities often manifest in the latent period of the model when SRS is barely observed [
76].
We have shown that treatment with anakinra, lamotrigine, or their combination in the early stages of epileptogenesis ameliorated impairments in motor activity, exploratory behavior, and anxiety in the open field, reduced aggression in the social test, but only slightly improved TLE-induced impairments in communicative behavior and memory. At the same time, the efficacy of the combined treatment was almost identical to that of anakinra and lamotrigine monotherapy.
We have previously shown that administration of anakinra for 10 days after pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus attenuates, but does not completely prevent, some behavioral deficits that develop during the latent and chronic phases of the lithium-pilocarpine model of TLE [
23]. Mazarati et al. [
62] found that intrahippocampal administration of interleukin-1 receptor antagonist, an analog of anakinra, ameliorated psychoemotional disturbances in the lithium-pilocarpine model of TLE in rats. The present study mainly confirms the previously obtained data.
Our data obtained during treatment with lamotrigine are consistent with clinical observations showing that lamotrigine also has a positive effect on the mental state of patients with epilepsy. For example, Miller et al. found that lamotrigine reduced depressive symptoms in patients with epilepsy [
77], Kato et al. showed a decrease in aggression in patients with TLE after treatment with lamotrigine [
78]. In a study performed in rats in the lithium-pilocarpine model of TLE, Mahfoz et al. found that administration of lamotrigine attenuated pilocarpine-induced spatial memory impairments in the Morris water maze [
79]. However, in that study, lamotrigine was administered prior to the pilocarpine injection, which may account for a more pronounced effect than we found.
Overall, our study suggests that treatment with anakinra and lamotrigine has a protective effect on the development of some neurodegenerative and behavioral abnormalities during epileptogenesis. Although our study did not confirm the hypothesis that combined therapy is more effective than monotherapy with these drugs, the results suggest that both anakinra and lamotrigine, either alone or in combination, may have clinical value in preventing epileptogenesis.
Figure 1.
Effect of treatment with anakinra, lamotrigine and their combination on neurological parameters. (a) Kaplan-Meier survival curves. (b) Body weight dynamics. (c) Percentage of animals showing SRS during the 40 h observation period. (d) Number of SRS in the groups during 40 h of observation. (e) Duration of SRS episodes in the groups during 40 h of observation. (f) Seizure severity according to the Racine scale. Cntr – control rats; TLE – post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated group of rats; TLE+A, TLE+L, TLE+A+L – rats treated with anakinra, lamotrigine, and their combination. Each point represents one animal. Data are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (b) or median and interquartile range (d-f).
Figure 1.
Effect of treatment with anakinra, lamotrigine and their combination on neurological parameters. (a) Kaplan-Meier survival curves. (b) Body weight dynamics. (c) Percentage of animals showing SRS during the 40 h observation period. (d) Number of SRS in the groups during 40 h of observation. (e) Duration of SRS episodes in the groups during 40 h of observation. (f) Seizure severity according to the Racine scale. Cntr – control rats; TLE – post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated group of rats; TLE+A, TLE+L, TLE+A+L – rats treated with anakinra, lamotrigine, and their combination. Each point represents one animal. Data are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (b) or median and interquartile range (d-f).
Figure 2.
Representative Nissl-stained sections of the hippocampus of control rat (Cntr), post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated rat (TLE) and post-SE rats treated with anakinra (TLE+A), lamotrigine (TLE+L), or their combination (TLE+A+L).
Figure 2.
Representative Nissl-stained sections of the hippocampus of control rat (Cntr), post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated rat (TLE) and post-SE rats treated with anakinra (TLE+A), lamotrigine (TLE+L), or their combination (TLE+A+L).
Figure 3.
Statistical data on the number of neurons per 100 µm length of cell layer in hippocampal areas CA1 and CA3 of control rats (Cntr), post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated rats (TLE), and post-SE rats treated with anakinra (TLE+A), lamotrigine (TLE+L), or their combination (TLE+A+L). Markers indicate individual values per rat. Columns show mean values and error bars show standard error of the mean. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 (post-hoc Games-Howell test for CA1; post-hoc Tukey's test for CA3).
Figure 3.
Statistical data on the number of neurons per 100 µm length of cell layer in hippocampal areas CA1 and CA3 of control rats (Cntr), post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated rats (TLE), and post-SE rats treated with anakinra (TLE+A), lamotrigine (TLE+L), or their combination (TLE+A+L). Markers indicate individual values per rat. Columns show mean values and error bars show standard error of the mean. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 (post-hoc Games-Howell test for CA1; post-hoc Tukey's test for CA3).
Figure 4.
Behavior of rats in the Open field test. (a) Representative examples of tracks in the open field of control rat (Cntr), post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated rat (TLE), and post-SE rats treated with anakinra (TLE+A), lamotrigine (TLE+L), or their combination (TLE+A+L). Statistical data on total distance traveled by rats in the open field (b), time of locomotion (c); number of acts (d); number of climbs (e); time of hole exploration (f); time of grooming (g); time in the center of the field (h). Data are presented as mean and standard error of the mean (b-f, h) or median and interquartile range (g). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001,**** p < 0.0001, Tukey's post hoc test (normally distributed data) or Dunn's multiple comparison test (non-normally distributed data). Each point represents the value of a different animal.
Figure 4.
Behavior of rats in the Open field test. (a) Representative examples of tracks in the open field of control rat (Cntr), post-SE lithium-pilocarpine untreated rat (TLE), and post-SE rats treated with anakinra (TLE+A), lamotrigine (TLE+L), or their combination (TLE+A+L). Statistical data on total distance traveled by rats in the open field (b), time of locomotion (c); number of acts (d); number of climbs (e); time of hole exploration (f); time of grooming (g); time in the center of the field (h). Data are presented as mean and standard error of the mean (b-f, h) or median and interquartile range (g). * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001,**** p < 0.0001, Tukey's post hoc test (normally distributed data) or Dunn's multiple comparison test (non-normally distributed data). Each point represents the value of a different animal.
Figure 5.
The behavior of the rats in the Social test. (a) Time of communication. (b) Percentage of time of different types of communicative behaviors. (c) Time of aggressive behavior. (d) Number of acts of aggressive behavior. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001,**** p < 0.0001; Tukey's post hoc test (normally distributed data) or Dunn's multiple comparison test (non-normally distributed data). Data are presented as mean and standard error of the mean (a) or median and interquartile range (c, d). Each point represents the value of a different animal.
Figure 5.
The behavior of the rats in the Social test. (a) Time of communication. (b) Percentage of time of different types of communicative behaviors. (c) Time of aggressive behavior. (d) Number of acts of aggressive behavior. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001,**** p < 0.0001; Tukey's post hoc test (normally distributed data) or Dunn's multiple comparison test (non-normally distributed data). Data are presented as mean and standard error of the mean (a) or median and interquartile range (c, d). Each point represents the value of a different animal.
Figure 6.
Behavior of rats in the Fear conditioning test. (a) Time of freezing in cage A. (b) Time of freezing during adaptation to an unfamiliar cage B. (c) Time of freezing in the new cage B in response to a tone, a pain-associated stimulus. (d) Time of freezing in the novel cage B after cessation of the pain-associated stimulus. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001; Dunn's multiple comparisons test. The data are presented as median and interquartile range (a-b) or as mean and standard error of the mean (c-d).
Figure 6.
Behavior of rats in the Fear conditioning test. (a) Time of freezing in cage A. (b) Time of freezing during adaptation to an unfamiliar cage B. (c) Time of freezing in the new cage B in response to a tone, a pain-associated stimulus. (d) Time of freezing in the novel cage B after cessation of the pain-associated stimulus. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001; Dunn's multiple comparisons test. The data are presented as median and interquartile range (a-b) or as mean and standard error of the mean (c-d).
Figure 7.
Spatial learning and memory of rats in the Morris water maze. (a) Training dynamics (distance traveled to find the platform) over four training days. (b) Distance traveled before finding the platform in the third trial of the first training day. (c) Distance traveled before finding the platform on the first attempt of the second training day. (d) Total distance traveled before finding the platform on all trials for 4 training days. (e-g) Long-term spatial memory test on the fifth experimental day, time spent in the target area where the platform was previously located: (e) first 30 seconds; (f) for the entire test (90 seconds). (g) Examples of tracks during platform retrieval on test day. Data are shown for the entire test (90 seconds). The area where the platform was previously located is highlighted. *-p<0.05; **p<0.01, Tukey's or Games-Howell's post hoc test. Data are presented as mean and standard error of the mean. Each point represents the value of a different animal.
Figure 7.
Spatial learning and memory of rats in the Morris water maze. (a) Training dynamics (distance traveled to find the platform) over four training days. (b) Distance traveled before finding the platform in the third trial of the first training day. (c) Distance traveled before finding the platform on the first attempt of the second training day. (d) Total distance traveled before finding the platform on all trials for 4 training days. (e-g) Long-term spatial memory test on the fifth experimental day, time spent in the target area where the platform was previously located: (e) first 30 seconds; (f) for the entire test (90 seconds). (g) Examples of tracks during platform retrieval on test day. Data are shown for the entire test (90 seconds). The area where the platform was previously located is highlighted. *-p<0.05; **p<0.01, Tukey's or Games-Howell's post hoc test. Data are presented as mean and standard error of the mean. Each point represents the value of a different animal.
Figure 8.
The experimental design.
Figure 8.
The experimental design.
Figure 9.
Fear Conditioning Test Scheme.
Figure 9.
Fear Conditioning Test Scheme.