1. Introduction
Aging is associated with structural and functional changes that increase risks of dis- eases and death [
1,
2,
3]. Multiple factors contribute to aging. Accumulation of damage and dysfunction may happen due to genetic mutations, epigenetic modifications, oxida- tive stress and inflammation [
4,
5]. Physiological or biological age (BA) is defined as the current state of the individual as a biological system. A combination of detectable lifetime- dependent biological parameters characterizes the system state. These parameters include the current profile of genomic DNA methylation, age-associated structural changes in the brain, brain functional reserve, etc.
In normal aging, BA is equal to chronological age. If the process of getting older is accelerated, BA exceeds the chronological age. In decelerated aging, BA becomes lower than the official age [
6,
7,
8]. Accelerated aging (AA) shares common features with the normal one, but protein aggregation and excitotoxicity are specific to AA [
9,
10,
11]. Understanding mechanisms of aging opens opportunities for targeted treatment of the diseases that occur late in life [
9].
AA is the area of research with unresolved issues such as non-standardized terminol- ogy [
12] and understudied mechanisms [
13]. Researchers have not reached an agreement on whether neurodegeneration (ND) is a type of AA [
14,
15] or its outcome [
14,
16,
17,
18]. The latter view argues that certain biomarkers (BMs) are ND-specific and they do not detect AA [
17].
Different theories were proposed to explain AA pathogenesis [
19,
20,
21]. The genetic theory assumes that accumulation of DNA mutations and/or gene dysregulation are the major causes of AA [
19,
22]. The theory considers random DNA changes but ignores chro- mosomal, multifactorial and monogenic alterations [
13,
23,
24]. The multi-proteinopathies theory describes aggregation of misfolded proteins as a leading cause of cell dysfunction in age-related diseases [
20,
25]. The free radical theory postulates that the primary accelerator of aging is oxidative damage of DNA and proteins [
15,
26,
27,
28]. But the concept fails to explain difference between normal and abnormal levels of reactive oxygen species [
21,
29]. In practice, no diagnostic BMs can identify and prognosticate AA reliably [
20,
30].
2. Biomolecular Aspects of Aging
Neurocentric and neurovascular hypotheses describe aetiology of ND at sub-, cellular, and supra-cellular levels. Initially, research efforts were focused mainly on neurons. Then, investigators recognized the importance of non-neural cells in higher brain functions. Neu- rovascular (NV) view refers to a neurovascular unit (NVU) which is a dynamic multicellular structure mediating functional interactions between brain tissues per se and blood vessels. The NVU includes astrocytes, microglia, oligodendrocytes, precursor cells, endothelial cells, pericytes, excitatory and inhibitory neurons [
31]. The NV hypothesis proposes that neural cells in the NVU and circulating immune cells secrete proinflammatory mediators contributing to age-related neuroinflammation [
32], cell degeneration [
33,
34] and endothe- lial impairment [
34,
35]. These changes disrupt molecular networks, induce damage to the blood–brain barrier [
36,
37] and lead to NVU dysfunction which is a major cause of ND [
38]. But the exact role of an NVU in ND remains unclear [
39]. A research for ND-associated BMs is difficult due to high complexity and molecular heterogeneity of the NVU network. It requires whole genome studies, e.g., global transcriptome analysis followed by hierarchical data clustering [
40] or single-cell/single-nucleus transcriptomics [
41,
42].
Molecular biomarkers (MBMs) are biomolecules, their components, fragments or modifications with the associated measurable parameters that serve as a tool to diagnose pathologies and monitor biological processes. MBMs can be used to evaluate aging, partic- ularly to estimate the rate of its progression [
43]. Aging MBMs include mRNA transcripts, proteins [
44], telomere length, serum markers of DNA damage [
45], DNA methylation profiles [
46,
47], histone modifications [
48,
49,
50,
51,
52,
53,
54,
55,
56,
57,
58,
59], differentially expressed genes [
42,
60], non- coding RNAs [
61,
62,
63,
64] and other biomolecules (
Figure 1).
Despite a large number of suggested MBMs of ND, only few of them are validated. In most studies, sample sizes were too small to justify the accuracy and reproducibility of MBM data. For example, a recent study aimed at determining whether age affects different cell types in the NVU. The study resulted in a model discriminating patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) from healthy controls (HCs). The model revealed 15 genes related to AA (AAG): IGF1R, MXI1, RB1, PPARA, NFE2L2, STAT5B, FOS, PRKCD, YWHAZ, HTT, MAPK9,
HSPA9, SDHC, PRKDC and
PDPK1 [
42]. Differential expression of IGF1R, MXI1, PPARA,
YWHAZ and
MAPK9 correlated with ND progression. Therefore, they may function as facilitators or inhibitors of AD. But the research neither demonstrated cell-specific roles of the discovered AAGs nor identified their contribution to AD pathogenesis and interactions in the NVU. Moreover, the study failed to justify the AAGs as MBMs since it had an insufficient sample size of only 11 AD patients and 7 HCs [
42]. ND results from multiple structural changes at different genetic loci over a period of time [
65,
66]. AD accounts for 90% of ND cases. High risk of developing AD is associated with alterations in 15 genes that predispose to ND (NDG):
GBA1, APP, PSEN1, MAPT, GRN, SETX, SPAST, CSF1R, C9orf72 [
67],
TET2 [
68],
TBK1 [
69],
TOMM40, APOC1 [
70],
APOE [
70,
71] and
TREM2 [
72,
73,
74,
75,
76,
77,
78]. Surprisingly, the gene sets do not overlap across the studies on AAGs and NDGs. Researchers found that the
APOE e4 allele and mutation spectrum for
TREM2 gene are risk factors for developing dementia with Lewy bodies, multi-cognitive decline and corticobasal degeneration [
78,
79,
80,
81,
82,
83,
84,
85,
86,
87]. DNA methylation level reflects rate of aging. Approximately 1.5% of genomic DNA contains 5-methylcytosine (5-mC), and the level decreases during ontogenesis [
88]. The level of 5-mC is the highest in embryos, and then it reduces gradually across life [
89,
90]. In aging, global genomic DNA hypomethylation proceeds along with hypermethylation of CpG islands. These changes in DNA methylation patterns are called “epigenetic drift” [
91,
92]. In the mammalian genome, 60% of the CpG islands are associated with gene promoters and regulate gene transcription [
91].
In normal aging, age-predictive models demonstrate gradual linear changes in the DNA methylation profile, but environmental or genetic risk factors can accelerate the process of getting older [
93]. In monozygotic twins, the divergence of the methylome increases at different rates [
94]. Change of the DNA methylation profile was proposed as a mechanism of an epigenetic clock [
95,
96,
97] by analogy with a biological clock [
98,
99]. Monitoring deviation between biological and chronological age helps to study development and aging across the lifespan [
100]. Horvath [
101], Hannum [
93] and PhenoAge [
102] epigenetic clocks serve as markers of ND [
102,
103,
104,
105,
106], with the first of these showing the strongest correlation between epigenetic and chronological age [
107].
Histone modifications can serve as potential MBMs of aging, however, heterogeneity of animal models used to develop the biomarkers limits their applicability. For example, a drop in highly abundant transcription activation mark H3K4me3 [
48] correlated with an extended lifespan in
Caenorhabditis elegans [
49]. Contrarily, an increase in the H3K4me3 level was linked with AA in
Drosophila melanogaster [
50]. The level of heterochromatin- associated histone transcription repression mark H3K9me3 gradually decreases during aging in haematopoietic stem cells of humans and mice [
51]. In
C. elegans and other models of senescance, the most significant loss of H3K9me3 occurs in repressive regions [
52,
53]. H3K27me3 is associated with transcriptional silencing in aging [
54]. The role of H3K27me3 is controversial, as studies showed its bidirectional lifelong changes [
55,
56,
57,
58,
59].
Increased levels of H4K20me3 and H3K4me3 and decreased levels of H3K9me1 and H3K27me3 are common age-associated epigenetic marks [
108,
109,
110]. Research showed an increase in H3K4me3 promoter methylation in a CK-p25 tauopathy mouse model and hippocampus of AD patients [
111,
112]. The following histone methylation marks can also be found in an Alzheimer brain: H4K20me2, H3K4me2, H3K27me3, H3K79me1, H3K79me2, H3K36me2, H4K20me3, H3K27me1 and H3K56me1 [
113,
114]. Besides, histone acetylation marks H3K9ac, H3K14ac and H4K16ac are associated with normal and accelerated aging [
110,
111,
113,
114,
115]. Histone phosphorylation marks H4S47p and H3S10p and histone ubiquitination mark H2BK120ub are observed in AD [
114,
116,
117]. Further systematic research should elucidate regulatory mechanisms of histone modifications, their interaction, and interplay between histone marks and other factors.
Non-coding RNAs (ncRNAs) are used as aging MBMs [
118,
119,
120,
121].
Long non-coding RNAs (lncRNAs) are presented with the growth-arrest-specific transcript 5 (
GAS5) which plays a significant role in cell proliferation and apoptosis [
122,
123,
124]. Its down-regulation leads to phosphorylation of the tau protein in ND [
125,
126]. Long intergenic brain cytoplasmic RNA 1 (
BCYRN1) expressed in the dendritic domains of neurons is down-regulated in aging [
127].
MicroRNAs (miRNAs) impact neuronal plasticity, influence tau protein metabolism and mediate brain aging through regulation of gene expression [
128,
129,
130,
131,
132,
133,
134,
135,
136,
137]. Regulation of miR-145a and miR-375 depends on age in mouse brains [
138,
139,
140]. The MIR29 family, MIR339-5p, MIR195 and MIR107 modulate expression of beta-secretase 1 involved in cleaving the amyloid precursor protein [
141,
142,
143,
144,
145,
146]. Interestingly, miR-34 plays a protective role in Drosophila [
147] and MIR144/MIR451 regulates ADAM metallopeptidase domain 10 in AD [
148]. Hypothalamic stem cells secret over 20 miRNAs into the cerebrospinal fluid. These miRNAs control aging rate in mice [
149], which should also be relevant to human brain [
150]. Future studies should verify miRNA MBMs in humans [
151].
Circular RNAs (circRNAs) are abundant in the brain, and their expression changes with age in skeletal muscles [
152,
153]. CircRNAs contribute to ND through interaction with miRNAs. For example, ciRS-7 potentially functions as a sponge for MIR7-1 [
154] and its level is reduced dramatically in an AD brain [
155]. Cerebral circRNAs are linked with neurotransmitter function, synaptic activities and neuronal maturation. They target the expression and availability of specific age-related mRNAs in the brain. At least four circRNAs are involved in postoperative neurocognitive disorders [
156]. Another study revealed nearly 1200 cerebral circRNAs in a rat model of aging [
157]. Various biomarker candidates including circRNAs await validation in the clinical arena.
3. Aging of Organs and Systems beyond Neurodegeneration
Aging affects organs and systems with different rates of change; therefore, the AA concept needs to be adjusted when applied to individual organs. For example, ovarian aging implies a loss of follicle numbers and decreased oocyte viability. Typically, an accelerated decline in fertility begins around the age of 38 years and continues until the climacteric [
158]; however, a non-uniform decrease in follicle numbers results in a large variation in menopause onset. BA of the male reproduction system can also be assessed by fertility, but the arrest of reproductive capacity is reversible in older men, with lifestyle and disease factors prevailing over other determinants of aging [
159]. In mice, oxidative stress, inflammation, DNA damage and de novo mutations accelerate testicular aging [
160,
161,
162,
163], while growth differentiation factor 11 enhances antioxidant enzyme activity and protects the testes [
164]. A progressive age-related drop in Leydig and Sertoli cell function [
165], testicular size [
166] or testosterone level was demonstrated in older men [
167]; but no decrease in testicle size or the levels of testosterone was observed in a cohort of older men with healthy lifestyle and affordable healthcare service [
168,
169].
The cumulative effect of a disease rather than age may account for changes in male fertility throughout life. Chronological age inaccurately reflects reproductive BA. Therefore, the AA concept cannot be adopted to the reproductive system. This illustrates a challenge in assessing BA at the organ and system levels.
Sex hormones that affect fertility are a part of the endocrine system. Susceptibility to aging differ among endocrine glands. In men, hypothalamic–pituitary–testicular axis does not undergo dramatic chronobiological changes: only 35–50% of men over 80 have reduced testosterone levels [
170,
171]. Diabetes mellitus and obesity predispose to accelerated adipose tissue dysfunction affecting telomere length [
172,
173]. Adrenal and thyroid functions undergo less prominent age-related transformations than their hypothalamic regulation [
174]; therefore, BA assessment from hormonal findings is challenging. With aging, hormone activity decreases and endocrine alterations are established [
175]. BA is affected by the level of glycosylated haemoglobin, glucose, triglycerides, low-density lipoproteins and total cholesterol [
176,
177,
178]. The modulation of these parameters, lifestyle and environmental factors can prevent or contribute to AA [
179]. Effectiveness of hormone replacement therapy for aging reversal is questionable though [
180].
Environmental and endocrinological factors affect BA of connective tissue. Status of the skeletal system reflects an individual endocrine profile and micronutrient balance [
181,
182,
183] as well as environmental and occupational attainments [
184]. For example, bone resorption in astronauts prevails over its formation due to microgravity; however, bone density normalises after the flight [
185,
186]. Skin elasticity serves as a marker of aging which rate can be modified due to estrogen deficiency, metabolic alterations and exogenous factors (burns) [
187,
188,
189,
190,
191]. Fibroblasts constitute a natural cell stock that allows skin rejuvenation, repair and decelerated aging [
192]. In connective tissue, a combinatory effect of internal and external factors determines BA more accurately than the chronological one [
193,
194]. Therefore, an inability to account for the decreased aging rate reveals a weakness of the AA concept.
Studies in other systems have also reported reversibility of age-related changes in them. For example, physical training can rejuvenate the respiratory system by expanding the alveolar space. However, studies on these issues did not comprehensively evaluate BA of the lung since the impact of muscle atrophy on results in the spirometry test was not considered [
195,
196,
197]. Lifestyle changes (e.g., calorie restriction and physical activity) could also reverse aging in patients with early stages of chronic kidney disease [
198]. Another example is shown by the discovered potential to rejuvenate kidneys with up-regulation of the
Klotho gene [
199]. These evidences speak for a limited generalisation of the AA concept. AA affects various systems and cross-organ communication. The interaction between systems can impede the atrophy of an organ through compensatory mechanisms in other organs. Several studies have demonstrated the role of the central nervous system in reversing the aging of other systems and organs [
200,
201,
202]. Endocrine and cardiovascular diseases promote renal aging [
203]. Conversely, kidney transplantation can revive other parts of the body [
204,
205]. The characterisation of organ- and system-specific aging processes is challenging and it will require combinatory approaches that are largely missing in the AA concept.
4. Limitations of the AA Concept
The AA concept should be considered within the context of individual capacities and personalised structure–functional reserve mechanisms (
Figure 2). A generalized term “structure-functional reserves” is introduced to denote an observed variability in multiple structural and functional parameters at different levels in a population: expression of genetically and epigenetically regulated genes, number, viability and functionality of cells, amount of synapses and intercellular contacts, secretion of cytokines, potency of physiological responses, etc. The term definition could be further developed and linked to statistical distributions of measurable biological parameters in the population and to a norm of reaction.
Physiological reserves reflect the remaining capacity of an organ to perform its function. Aging and diseases lead to atrophy due to a reduction in the number of cells and supracellular structures [
206,
207]. In the context of brain aging, physiological cognitive reserve reflects the level of education, occupational and environmental attainments and performance in cognitive tests [
206]. Reversible forms of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia represent clinical examples of restoring individual reserve potential. The examples are not aligned with AA theory [
208,
209]. Neural compensation in the elderly leads to formation of secondary brain networks [
210], which decelerate the aging of the brain [
206,
211]. In elderly patients, reversion of MCI results from specific lifestyle activities and cognitive stimulation throughout life [
212,
213].
Age assessment requires an accurate estimation of individual reserves that account for biological and chronological age differences. In neuroscience, machine learning models predicted total years lived in good health from brain-imaging data with error of 2.1–4.9 years [
214,
215]. Individual brain age can also be calculated as a difference between chronological age and the predicted BA [
216]. In obstetrics, the evaluation of gynecological status takes into account both reproductive health and potential fertility. Overall BA depends on reserve capacities of individual systems and organs [
217,
218].
Variance in reserve capacities complicates precise BA assessment. Criteria of AA of the brain are unclear since indicators of normal aging are still missing [
219]. Methods for BA assessment are not standardized, they do not take into account individual reserve potential, and reference curves for brain changes have not yet been created. Methodological discrepancies lead to contradictory findings in different studies. For example, AD adds 1.5 years to brain age, MCI adds 1 year, multiple sclerosis—0.41 years, Parkinson’s disease (PD)—3 years and schizophrenia—5.5 years. The last two pathologies impact cognition in a milder and slower way than AD [
220,
221,
222]. Another research on AD revealed the added brain age between 6 and 9 years [
223].
Several methodologic limitations demonstrate the need for caution when assessing research findings. For example, studies on age-related brain atrophy commonly have a cross-sectional design that is less accurate compared to the longitudinal one [
224]. Many studies are based on small non-representative cohorts [
225,
226,
227]; therefore, applicability of the designed mathematical models is low. Certain studies of brain aging focus on the middle-aged and elderly population and they fail to report individual prenatal pathologies and childhood trauma affecting brain health and BA of study participants [
228]. Application of the concept of AA to localised degeneration presents a challenge since different brain parts become older unevenly [
229]. For example, in localised ND, BA assessment reflects the level of damage to the most vulnerable brain parts (e.g., substancia nigra and ruber nuclei in PD) [
230,
231,
232]; however, one should also consider the brain resources that can minimise the atrophy effects [
233]. In systemic ND, the brain ages faster than in localised ND [
234,
235], and the difference in the speed of atrophic changes is apparent [
236].
ND has polyetiological nature, and contemporary neuroscience lacks a clear explanation of cooperation between different causal factors. It is still unclear whether chronic diseases lead to or result from ND [
237,
238] since the genetic, environmental and lifestyle factors interact in an undefined way [
18,
239,
240]. Several articles have revealed a misalignment between dementia risk, cognitive performance and MBM levels [
241,
242]. Another disadvantage of AA studies is an inability to report an impact of medications on study results [
243]. Last but not least, AA represents a diagnostic but not pathognomonic signature in ND and in psychiatric diseases such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder [
222,
244,
245]. The entire range of symptoms observed in these patients cannot be explained by brain aging only [
222,
246,
247].
Drug therapy could extend reserves. For instance, antidepressants can help to re- verse MCI [
248]. Certain cognitive disorders demonstrated a reversible pattern in cognitive performance upon treatment [
248,
249]. Sex hormone replacement in ND reduces risk of cognitive impairment, delays symptom onset and slows the progression [
250]. Antioxidant-based therapy also alleviates severity of the disease [
251,
252]. Recent ND studies described a number of novel therapeutic options targeting mitophagy, protein aggregation and cellular senescence. These options include specific antibodies, inducers of cell proliferation and NAD+ supplementations [
9,
18,
253]. The observed treatment effects contradict the idea of irreversibility of changes claimed by the AA concept.
7. Afterword: Aging Science History and Theories
Several theories have been postulated to explain a possible biological meaning or evolutionary role of aging: evolutionary advantage of species (1890s, Weisman), accumulated mutations (1952, Medawar), antagonistic pleiotropy (1957, Williams), replicative senescence (1965, Hayflick), and the disposable soma theory (1972, Kirkwood) [
306]. Theories about what causes aging commonly fall into either of two categories: genetic or stochastic. The genetic, or programmed, group refers to endocrine, immunological and programmed longevity theories. They suggest that aging is predetermined through genetics and that organisms have a built-in clock which dictates life expectancy. Stochastic, or damage, theories propose that random errors and damage accumulate over time and limit longevity. This group includes wear-and-tear, rate of living, cross-linking, free radical and somatic DNA damage hypotheses [
307].
Theories of aging can also be classified by biological level. Gene regulation, codon restriction, error catastrophe, somatic mutation and dysdifferentiation theories describe molecular-level processes. Cellular senescence–telomere, free radical, wear-and-tear and apoptosis theories focus on the cellular level. Neuroendocrine, immunologic and rate of living theories conceptualise changes at the system level [
308].
In 1920s, Laboratories of the Rockefeller Institute for Medical Research conducted experiments on AA and published findings. The author applied the terms “normal aging” and “aging rate” to effects of light on Drosophila inbred in the dark [
309,
310]. Since then, the numbers of references on “aging”, “aging rate” and “AA” has reached 614,132; 56,088 and 21,401, respectively [
311].
In 1928, a Professor of Neurology of the Columbia University, Frederick Tilney, published the work “The aging of the human brain”, where an AD patient brain was compared to the normally aged one in the diagnostic context of the number of plaques. In this work, he also claimed an abundance of senile plaques in all human brains after the age of 90 years, influence of unfavourable factors and diseases on the brain. Recorded a century ago, his words are worth repeating today: “It is amazing how little general or particular interest man has shown in the most important organ of his body and life. Up to the present time he has devoted relatively little attention and much less capital to the understanding of that part of his machinery which is the secret of his success and the only hope for his future progress, if not his actual salvation. . . The ridiculous stupidity of annually consecrating appalling sums of money to the savage purposes of destruction should in time shock human intelligence out of patronizing such futilities and into wiser realizations. Certainly, one liberally supported and effective brain institute would prove an incomparably more profitable investment for civilization than the most powerful battle fleet that ever sailed the seas.” (Tilney, 1928 [
312]).