Preprint
Article

Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire (CFPQ) With Portuguese Caregivers of 2-to-8-Year-Olds

Altmetrics

Downloads

98

Views

36

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

07 November 2023

Posted:

08 November 2023

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Recent findings have demonstrated an increase in the prevalence of childhood obesity and overweight in Portugal, urging the need to study modifiable risk factors such as parental feeding practices. The Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire (CFPQ) is an important self-report measure assessing a broad range of responsive and non-responsive feeding practices. However, the CFPQ has not yet been validated in Portugal. Therefore, the present study aimed to test the validity of this measure with Portuguese parents of 2- to 8-year-old children. A sample of 409 parents completed a Portuguese-adapted version of the CFPQ and the already validated Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ). Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA), Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA), and psychometric analysis were conducted. CFA demonstrated the original 12-factor structure did not fit the sample. EFA identified an 8-factor structure, comprising 29 items: Monitoring, Modeling, Environment, Involvement, Emotion Regulation, Restriction for Weight Control, Restriction for Health, and Pressure. Findings suggest that parental feeding practices are sensitive to parents’ background culture and children’s developmental period.
Keywords: 
Subject: Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Introduction

Obesity has reached epidemic proportions in Europe and is still escalating, posing an increasing health challenge. According to the WHO Regional Office for Europe [1], obesity and overweight affect 7,9% of children under the age of five (4.4 million children) and one in three school-aged children. Portugal is one of the countries in Europe with the highest prevalence of childhood obesity and overweight, with 31.6% and 13.5% of children being overweight and obese, respectively [2,3]. Overweight and obese children tend to remain so in adulthood [4,5,6] and are more likely to develop non-transmissible diseases, such as cardiovascular diseases, diabetes, musculoskeletal disorders, and cancer [7,8,9]. Thus, it is of great interest to investigate modifiable risk factors for obesity in infancy and childhood.
In this sense, a large body of evidence shows that parental feeding practices influence children’s eating behaviors and weight status [10,11,12,13,14]. Children are born with the ability to self-regulate their energy intake [15,16,17]. Nevertheless, as children get older, this ability decreases due to external influences [18,19,20,21,22], namely parental feeding practices [23,24]. Therefore, feeding practices can either support or undermine children’s ability to self-regulate their internal hunger and satiety cues, depending on whether they are responsive or non-responsive to these cues [25]. Responsive feeding practices refer to parents’ capacity to correctly recognize and respond to these cues, thus positively influencing children’s self-regulation of energy intake. On the contrary, non-responsive feeding practices refer to the use of excessive controlling and coercive feeding practices that teach children to eat for reasons unrelated to their appetite and ignore their hunger and satiety cues [23,26,27,28,29,30]. These practices can compromise children’s inborn capacity to regulate their energy intake and contribute to poor diet quality and excessive weight gain [12,26,27,29,31,32,33].
Most studies have focused on non-responsive feeding practices, such as pressure to eat and restriction [34,35,36,37,38,39], with responsive feeding practices being less studied. As such, the Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ) [40] has been one of the most widely used instruments to measure parental feeding practices; however, this instrument assesses only three feeding practices: monitoring, pressure to eat, and restriction (with the two last ones non-responsive practices). Therefore, researchers have highlighted the importance of including other less controlling and coercive feeding practices, especially those associated with healthy outcomes [33,41]. With the development of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire (CFPQ) [42], the CFQ and other previous measures were expanded. The CFPQ goes beyond controlling feeding practices, including a broader range of feeding practices, namely those that are responsive such as modeling healthy eating, teaching about nutrition, and encouraging balance and variety [42]. Thus, the CFPQ allows for a better description of parental feeding practices.
Given the high prevalence of obesity and overweight among Portuguese children [2], it is important to study and understand parental feeding practices. Even so, there is a lack of valid measures for the Portuguese population that assess feeding practices. To our knowledge, only two measures were validated in Portugal – the CFQ [43,44] and the Parental Feeding Style Questionnaire [45] – which focus more on controlling feeding practices, while one – the Children’s Intake Self-Regulation Feeding Practices Scale [46] – was created and developed in Portugal. The CFPQ has been validated in other countries (e.g., [47,48,49,50,51]), but not yet among the Portuguese population, which limits its generalizability and use in other cultures. Since feeding practices can be influenced by ethnicity [52,53], it is necessary to validate measures for different populations.
Thus, the purpose of this study is to validate the CFPQ with Portuguese parents of 2- to 8-year-old children using a confirmatory factor analysis and examine its psychometric properties. If the original CFPQ factor structure is not confirmed, we will conduct an exploratory factor analysis to identify an alternative factor structure.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The initial sample included 505 caregivers, and although none of the variables presented more than 2.2% missing, the final sample respects only completed data (n = 409, 80.99% of the cases). Caregivers were mostly mothers (98%) of children between 2 and 8 years (M = 49.51 months; SD = 13.04). Most children were boys (n = 224, 54.8% and girls 45.2%), first-born (n = 280, 68.5%), and had siblings (n = 262, 64.1%). Mothers’ age ranged between 22 and 50 years (M = 35.91; SD = 5.36) and fathers between 22 and 59 years (M = 37.98; SD = 6.17). Mothers’ education level varied between 8 and 21 years (M = 15.99; SD = 2.93) and fathers between 5 and 21 years (M = 14.33; SD = 3.41). Most caregivers were married or cohabiting (86.6%) and worked full-time (79.3% mothers; 93.8% fathers).

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire (CFPQ) [42]

The CFPQ is a self-report instrument containing 49 items measuring parental feeding practices for which parents of 2- to 8-year-old children respond on a 5-point Likert scale, indicating their degree of agreement (1 = disagree to 5 = agree; items 1–13) or their frequency of use a specific feeding approach (1 = never to 5 = always; items 14–49). The items are distributed over 12 subscales, seven of these subscales reflect more positive or responsive feeding practices: (1) Monitoring (four items, i.e., keeping track of child’s intake of unhealthy foods), (2) Modeling (four items, i.e., being an active and enthusiastic model of healthy eating for the child”), (3) Teaching about Nutrition (three items, i.e., encouraging the child’s intake of healthy foods through didactic techniques), (4) Encourage Balance and Variety (four items, i.e., promoting healthy and varied food consumption), (5) Environment (four items, i.e., providing healthy foods), (6) Involvement (three items, i.e., encouraging child’s involvement in food preparation and in meal planning), (7) Child Control (five items, i.e., allowing the child to control their feeding interactions and own eating behaviors); the other five subscales reflect non-responsive feeding practices (8) Emotion Regulation (three items, i.e., using food to regulate the child’s emotions), (9) Food as Reward (three items, i.e., using food as a reward for the child’s behavior), (10) Restriction for Weight Control (eight items, i.e., controlling the child’s intake to maintain or decrease the child’s weight), (11) Restriction for Health (four items, i.e., controlling the child’s intake to limit unhealthy foods), (12) Pressure (four items, i.e., encouraging the child to eat more food at meals, ignoring the child’s satiety/hunger cues). Items 16, 37, and 42 are reverse coded. An already translated and adapted Portuguese version was used [54].

2.2.2. Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ) [40]

The CFQ consists of 31 items assessing parental feeding practices and parents’ perceptions of parental and children’s weight status and their concerns about it. The CFQ was designed for use with parents of 2- to 11-year-old children. Items are answered on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never/disagree) to 5 (always/agree). For this study, only the three subscales measuring feeding practices were used: (1) Monitoring (three items, e.g., “How much do you keep track of the high-fat foods that the child eats?”), (2) Pressure to Eat (four items, e.g., “If my child says ‘I’m not hungry’, I try to get him/her to eat anyway”), (3) Restriction (eight items, e.g., “I have to be sure that my child does not eat too many high-fat foods”).

2.3. Procedures

This study is part of the research project “ChildObesity—Child obesity risk: the Role of Attachment, child's Temperament and Self-regulation", approved by the Ethics Committee of the ISPA—Instituto Universitário (I/038/06/2020).
Participants were recruited through convenience sampling, guaranteeing the confidentiality of data collecting process. Part of the sample was recruited via private and public schools in the Lisbon Metropolitan Area (26.5%). Schools’ boards were contacted to present the study and to ask their permission for data collection. Informed consent and questionnaires were sent to caregivers through their children’s teachers and returned in a closed envelope after completion. The other part of the sample (63.5%) was recruited online through social network posts (e.g., Facebook, Instagram). The questionnaires were inserted on Qualtrics web platform, and the resulting hyperlink was published in social network sites. When accessing the hyperlink, participants were informed about the aims of the study and about their rights (e.g., confidentiality, withdrawal by simply closing the web browser window). After providing informed consent by clicking on the “I agree” option, participants were redirected to the questionnaires. Following recommendations for good practices in online data collection, data collected via Qualtrics were subjected to quality control by checking whether there was any duplicated internet protocol (IP) address to prevent more than one response from the same IP [55].

2.4. Data analysis

Data analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 29.0, and Jamovi, version 2.3.28 (SEM 0.9.5 package). The psychometric sensitivity of the items was assessed with skewness (sk) and kurtosis (ku), with absolute values of sk smaller than 3 and ku smaller than 10 considered adequate [56]. Construct validity was assessed through factorial and subsequent convergent and discriminant validity. The factorial validity was analyzed using a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) of the original factor structure suggested by Musher-Eizenman and Holub [42], and the following CFA relative and absolute goodness of fit indexes were assessed: the relative chi-square fit index (χ2/df), the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis fit index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). The model’s fit was considered adequate when CFI and TLI ≥ 0.90-0.95, RMSEA ≤ 0.08-0.06, and SRMR ≤ 0.10-0.08 [57,58]. Items that performed poorly in the CFA (i.e., factor loading < 0.40, and/or negative factor loading, or commonalities < 0.30) were analyzed and removed to improve model fit. The reliability of the final model subscales of the final model was examined using Cronbach's alpha (> 0.60) [59]. Measurement invariance was also tested [60]. Configural invariance was tested by constraining the basic factor model to equality across the considered groups. If the model fits reasonably well configural invariance was achieved. To test for metric invariance equality constraints on the factor loadings were specifying if ΔCFI ≤ 0.01, ΔRMSEA ≤ 0.015, and ΔSRMR ≤ 0.03 weak was achieved. To test for scalar invariance, intercepts were constraint to equality across groups, and a strong invariance was achieved if ΔCFI ≤ 0.01, ΔRMSEA ≤ 0.015, and ΔSRMR ≤ 0.01. Finally for strict invariance, residuals were also constraint to equality across groups. Using a smaller sample (n = 315) the associations between CFPQ and CFQ were analyzed to test for convergent and discriminant validity.
Since several studies have found different factor structures for the CFPQ on different datasets, an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) was also performed to identify the appropriate number and structure of factors to fit the data without imposing an a priori hypothesis on the data was also conducted. Factors were expected to correlate, so principal factor analysis and oblique rotation methods were specified. Items that performed poorly (i.e., factor loading < 0.40, and/or negative factor loading) were analyzed and removed. Items cross-loading ≥ 0.40 on multiple factors were also dropped. Kaiser’s Measure of Sampling Adequacy (MSA) was expected to be ≥ 0.60 for the overall measure, and each individual item. Factors were dropped if internal reliability (Cronbach’s α) was lower than 0.60.

3. Results

Almost all items showed adequate psychometric sensitivity (|Sk| = 0.05 to 2.42; |Ku| = 0.02 to 7.38), except for item 45 (“I often put my child on a diet to control his/her weight” with |Sk|= 5.52 and |Ku|= 33.01; M = 1.09, SD = 0.45).

3.1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The CFA of the original 12-factor model did not present an acceptable fit for all the considered indexes (CFI = 0.816, TLI = 0.796, RMSE = 0.050, and SRMR = 0.069). To improve the model fit, four items presenting factor loadings lower than .40 were removed (items 18 and 45 from Restriction for Weight Control subscale, λ = 0.21 and λ = 0.36 respectively; item 44 from Modeling, λ = 0.30; and item 12 from Child Control, λ = 0.36). Although the model fit did show an improvement, it was still not acceptable (CFI = 0.853, TLI = 0.834, RMSEA = 0.047, and SRMR = 0.063). Four items with low communality (< 0.30) were gradually removed: item 10 from Child Control (h2 = 0.19 and λ = 0.44); item 43 from Restriction for Health (h2 = 0.20 and λ = 0.43); item 13 from Balance and Variety (h2 = 0.21 and λ = 0.46); item 17 from Pressure (h2 = 0.20 and λ = 0.45); items 31 and 42 from Teaching about Nutrition (h2 = 0.21, λ = 0.47 and h2 = 0.21, λ = 0.47 respectively), finally item 11 from Child Control (h2 = 0.23 and λ = 0.49) and residuals of item 34 and 35 were correlated (MI = 83.4). Most of the considered model fit indexes were acceptable (CFI = 0.913, TLI = 0.898, RMSEA = 0.041, and SRMR = 0.054). Internal consistency reliability of the subscales was examined using 12-factor final model. However, Teaching about Nutrition subscale was reduced to one item and was removed. All the subscales showed acceptable reliability with 𝛼 ≥ 0.60 (from 0.63 to 0.85), except for encouragement for Child Control (𝛼 = 0.46) and Food as Reward (𝛼 = 0.59) (see Table 1).
Using the final 11-factor model with 37 items, measurement invariance across sex was tested. Results suggest configural invariance as the model fits reasonably well (CFI = 0.892, RMSEA = 0.047, and SRMR = 0.065). Strict invariance across groups was achieved, more specifically loading (ΔCFI = 0.002, ΔRMSEA = 0.000, and ΔSRMR = 0.001), intercepts (ΔCFI = 0.002, ΔRMSEA = 0.001, and ΔSRMR = 0.001), and residual invariance (ΔCFI = 0.007, ΔRMSEA = 0.001, and ΔSRMR = 0.000) as achieved.

3.2. Exploratory Factor Analysis

EFA was also explored. In the first EFA, a 9-factor solution was identified. MSA was adequate for the overall measure, and for each item (overall MSA = 0.79, for the items MSA range from 0.64 to 0.89). However, fifteen items performed poorly and were removed (5, 6, 10, 11, 13, 16, 18, 19, 22, 24, 26, 36, 38, 40, 45). The second EFA, five items performed poorly and were removed (12, 23, 31, 37, 42). On the third EFA, an 8-factor structure was identified, items 26 and 37 presented low factor loadings and were removed. Final EFA presented an 8-factor structure (see Table 2). MSA was adequate for the overall measure and for each individual item (overall MSA = 0.74, for the items MSA range from 0.63 to 0.84).

3.3. Associations between modified CFPQ subscales and CFQ subscales

Relation to subscales from the CFQ was also explored (see Table 3). The majority of the associations found between the modified CFPQ subscales and the CFQ subscales were in expected directions, except for the positive association between CFPQ Modeling and CFQ Restriction (r = 0.25, p < 0.001), the negative association between CFPQ Environment and CFQ Monitoring (r = –0.14, p < 0.05), and the positive association between CFPQ Restriction for Health and the CFQ Monitoring (r = 0.22, p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

This study aimed to examine the original factor structure proposed by Musher-Eizenman and Holub [42] of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire in a Portuguese sample of caregivers of children between 2 and 8 years old. Confirmatory factor analyses showed the lack of fit of the original 12-factor and 49-item model in our sample, which is in line with previous findings from other validation studies in other cultures (e.g., [47,48,50,51,61,62,63]). These studies proposed different structures for the CFPQ. For example, Melbye et al. [64] study with a sample of parents of 10- to 12-year-olds in Norway suggested a 10-factor model, including 42 items. In New Zealand, Haszard et al. [48] identified a 5-factor model with 32 items in a sample of parents of 4- to 6-year-olds. Shohaimi et al. [51] study with Malaysian mothers of 7- to 9-year-olds identified a 12-factor model with 39 items. The study of Mais et al. [50] with Brazilian parents of 5- to 9-year-olds proposed a 6-factor model, including 42 items, which was confirmed in another sample of Brazilian parents of 2- to 5-year-olds [63]. In Jordan, Al-Qerem et al. [47] identified a 11-factor model with 43 items in a sample of mothers of 6- to 12-year-olds. In our study, exploratory factor analysis suggested that the most suitable structure was 29 items distributed over 8 factors: Monitoring, Emotion Regulation, Restriction for Weight Control, Modeling, Pressure, Restriction for Health, Environment, and Involvement.
The different CFPQ structures identified over several validation studies could be explained by cultural and social differences but also methodological differences. Indeed, these studies were conducted with parents of children of different group ages, which could influence the final models of the CFPQ. As demonstrated by Saltzman et al. [65], some feeding practices could be more relevant to parents at different developmental periods. Thus, their reliance on particular feeding practices may change as children grow. In their study, Saltzman et al. [65] analyzed the factor structure of the CFPQ across two-time points. At Time 1, when children were, on average, 37 months of age, a 7-factor model was deemed the most appropriate fit. At Time 2, at 57 months of age, a 5-factor model was the most suitable structure. For instance, when children were about 37 months of age, the Emotion Regulation and Food as Reward subscales were found, but not at 57 months. In this sense, these feeding practices could be more appropriate for parents of toddlers, and their use may decrease as children age [48,65]. On the other hand, the Restriction for Health subscale emerged only at Time 2, suggesting that this feeding practice could be more appropriate for parents of preschoolers [65]. Other feeding practices, including, Teaching about Nutrition, Encourage Balance and Variety, and Child Control, could be more suitable for parents of school-aged children [47,48,65]. This could explain why, in the present study, these last three feeding practices were not found since most of our sample was composed of 3- to 5-year-olds (86.8%). In this sense, it is unsurprising that we also didn’t find the Food as Reward subscale, as the proportion of toddlers in our sample was small.
Compared to the original CFPQ 12-factor model [42], in our 8-factor model most of the items loaded in their respective factor. The subscales Monitoring, Emotion Regulation, and Pressure had the same composition as the original factor. The subscales Restriction for Weight Control and Restriction for Health lost two (Item 18 “I have to be sure that my child does not eat too many high-fat foods”; Item 45 “I often put my child on a diet to control his/her weight”) and one item (Item 40 “I have to be sure that my child does not eat too much of his/her favorite foods”), respectively.
The Modeling subscale lost one item (Item 44 “I model healthy eating for my child by eating healthy foods myself”) that loaded in the Environment subscale. This result was also found in the study of Melbye et al. [64]. As explained by Melbye et al. [64], the other three items in the Modeling subscale appear to reflect a more active form of modeling (e.g., “I try to eat healthy foods in front of my child...”; “I show my child how much I enjoy eating healthy foods”), whereas this particular item reflects a more passive form of modeling. Furthermore, we could postulate that if caregivers practice healthy eating, the probability of healthy foods being available at home is high [64]. To elucidate, Melbye et al. [64] proposed that “healthy eating practices among parents might be more related to the availability of healthy foods in the home environment than to 'active' modeling of healthy eating” (p. 8).
In line with the results of Saltzman et al. [65], in the present study, the Involvement subscale included one item from the Teaching about Nutrition subscale (Item 25 “I discuss with my child why it’s important to eat healthy foods”). In previous studies, items from feeding practices such as Involvement, Teaching about Nutrition, Encourage Balance and Variety, and Modeling also loaded together. Moreover, in some studies, items from these subscales were included into one new single factor named Healthy Eating Guidance (48,50,61,63,65). This is expected because caregivers who use positive/responsive feeding practices don’t use them in isolation but in combination with others [47,48,50,66].
The modified CFPQ subscales also demonstrated good convergent and discriminant validity, which was indicated by the correlations between theoretically related constructs and a lack of correlations between unrelated constructs, respectively. In fact, the CFPQ Monitoring, Restriction for Weight Control, Restriction for Health, and Pressure subscales were strongly correlated with the corresponding CFQ subscales that measure the same constructs. Furthermore, distinct positive/responsive feeding practices such as Monitoring and Modeling were positively interrelated, and negative/non-responsive feeding practices such as Restriction, Emotion Regulation, and Pressure were also interrelated. Nonetheless, we found three apparently counterintuitive correlations. First, the CFPQ Environment subscale was negatively correlated with the CFQ Monitoring subscale. We believe it is possible that parents who keep less healthy foods or more unhealthy foods (e.g., salty snacks, candy, pastries) in the house feel the need to monitor their child’s intake of unhealthy foods. Second, the CFPQ Restriction for Health subscale was positively correlated with the CFQ Monitoring subscale. This correlation was also found in previous studies [40,65]. As suggested by Saltzman et al. [65], parents who monitor their child’s intake may be more likely to guide them to eat healthy foods and thus may also restrict more the child’s intake of unhealthy foods. Lastly, the CFPQ Modeling subscale was positively correlated with the CFQ Restriction subscale. This correlation was also reported in Saltzman et al. [65] study, which could suggest parents who demonstrate healthy eating for their child may be more likely to also guide and restrict child's intake of food to limit the consumption of unhealthy foods.
This study has limitations that should be addressed. First, participants were recruited using convenience sampling procedures, and the large majority of the sample was highly educated and consisted of mothers of, particularly, preschool children, thereby limiting the generalization of the findings. Future studies should test the CFPQ in more heterogeneous samples. Second, data are cross-sectional, limiting our ability to examine stability of factor scores across time. Third, since the CFPQ is a self-report instrument, responses may have been vulnerable to social desirability. Finally, other psychometric qualities should be tested, like predictive validity with other measures of child eating behaviors and weight outcomes.
These limitations notwithstanding, given the lack of valid measures assessing parental feeding practices for the Portuguese population, the present study provides a relevant contribution by validating an instrument that covers a wide variety of feeding practices. Moreover, reducing the questionnaire from 49 to 29 items could decrease the response burden. Our results also add to the parental feeding practices literature by supporting the role of cultural background and children’s age group in caregivers’ endorsement of feeding practices [23,33,67]. Indeed, as in previous validation studies, our study resulted in a modified version of the CFPQ. In this sense, the design and implementation of interventions targeting feeding practices to promote children’s development of healthy eating behaviors and prevent obesity must be culturally sensitive and appropriate to the child’s age. To make this possible, further large-scale studies that include culturally diverse samples are needed to facilitate cross-culture comparisons and understand cultural differences in parental feeding practices.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.V. and C.F.; methodology, all authors.; data collection, A.F.S., C.F. and M.F.; data analysis and drafting the manuscript, A.F.S., C.F. and M.F.; data interpretation and editing the manuscript, all authors. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (F.C.T.: PTDC/PSI-GER/29636/2017, UIDB/04810/2020, and SFRH/BD/06367/2020).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by ISPA Ethics Committee (Approval code: I/038/06/2020, Approval Date: 8 June 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Acknowledgments

Data collection and preparation of this article were supported in part by grants from the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (PTDC/PSI-GER/29636/2017, UIDB/04810/2020). We acknowledge the support of all colleagues who contributed to data collection.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe. WHO European Regional Obesity Report 2022. World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2022. Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/353747.
  2. Instituto Nacional de Saúde Doutor Ricardo Jorge. Infográfico - Obesidade Infantil 2022. Instituto Nacional de Saúde Doutor Ricardo Jorge, Lisbon, Portugal, 2023. Available online: http://repositorio.insa.pt/handle/10400.18/8631.
  3. Rito, A.; Mendes, S.; Figueira, I.; Faria, M.C.; Carvalho, R.; Santos, T.; Cardoso, S.; Feliciano, E.; Silvério, R.; Sancho, T.S.; Dinis, A.; Rascôa, C.L.; Batista, C.; Cruz, R.; Marques, C. Childhood Obesity Surveillance Initiative: COSI Portugal 2022. Instituto Nacional de Saúde Doutor Ricardo Jorge, Lisbon, Portugal, 2023. Available online: http://repositorio.insa.pt/handle/10400.18/8630.
  4. Lumeng, J.C.; Taveras, E.M.; Birch, L.; Yanovski, S. Z. Prevention of obesity in infancy and early childhood: A National Institutes of Health workshop. JAMA Pediatr. 2014, 169, 484–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Simmonds, M.; Llewellyn, A.; Owen, C.G.; Woolacott, N. Predicting adult obesity from childhood obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 2016, 17, 95–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ryder, J.R.; Jacobs, D.R., Jr.; Sinaiko, A.R.; Kornblum, A.P.; Steinberger, J. Longitudinal changes in weight status from childhood and adolescence to adulthood. J. Pediatr. 2019, 214, 187–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Horesh, A.; Tsur, A.M.; Bardugo, A.; Twig, G. Adolescent and childhood obesity and excess morbidity and mortality in young adulthood — A systematic review. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2021, 10, 301–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lauby-Secretan, B.; Scoccianti, C.; Loomis, D.; Grosse, Y.; Bianchini, F.; Straif, K.; International Agency for Research on Cancer Handbook Working Group. Body Fatness and Cancer — Viewpoint of the IARC Working Group. N. Engl. J. Med. 2016, 375, 794–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Skinner, A.C.; Perrin, E.M.; Moss, L.A.; Skelton, J.A. Cardiometabolic risks and severity of obesity in children and young adults. N. Engl. J. Med. 2015, 373, 1307–1317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Carnell, S.; Wardle, J. Associations between multiple measures of parental feeding and children’s adiposity in United Kingdom preschoolers. Obesity 2007, 15, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Faith, M.S.; Scanlon, K.S.; Birch, L.L.; Francis, L.A.; Sherry, B. Parent-child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obes. Res. 2004, 12, 1711–1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Larsen, J.K.; Hermans, R.C.; Sleddens, E.F.; Engels, R.C.; Fisher, J.O.; Kremers, S.S. How parental dietary behavior and food parenting practices affect children’s dietary behavior. Interacting sources of influence? Appetite 2015, 89, 246–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Peters, J.; Sinn, N.; Campbell, K.; Lynch, J. Parental influences on the diets of 2-5-year-old children: Systematic review of interventions. Early Child Dev. Care 2012, 182, 837–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ventura, A.K.; Birch, L.L. Does parenting affect children’s eating and weight status? Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2008, 5, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Birch, L.L.; Johnson, S.L.; Andresen, G.; Peters, J.C.; Schulte, M.C. The variability of young children’s energy intake. N. Engl. J. Med. 1991, 324, 232–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Birch, L.L.; Deysher, M. Calorie compensation and sensory specific satiety: Evidence for self regulation of food intake by young children. Appetite 1986, 7, 323–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fox, M.K.; Devaney, B.; Reidy, K.; Razafindrakoto, C.; Ziegler, P. Relationship between portion size and energy intake among infants and toddlers: Evidence of self-regulation. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2006, 106, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Brugaillères, P.; Issanchou, S.; Nicklaus, S.; Chabanet, C.; Schwartz, C. Caloric compensation in infants: developmental changes around the age of 1 year and associations with anthropometric measurements up to 2 years. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 109, 1344–1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Cecil, J.E.; Palmer, C.N.A.; Wrieden, W.; Murrie, I.; Bolton-Smith, C.; Watt, P.; Wallis, D.J.; Hetherington, M.M. Energy intakes of children after preloads: Adjustment, not compensation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2005, 82, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Johnson, S.L.; Taylor-Holloway, L.A. Non-Hispanic white and Hispanic elementary school children’s self-regulation of energy intake. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 83, 1276–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kral, T.V.; Stunkard, A.J.; Berkowitz, R.I.; Stallings, V.A.; Brown, D.D.; Faith, M.S. Daily food intake in relation to dietary energy density in the free-living environment: A prospective analysis of children born at different risk of obesity. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 86, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Smethers, A.D.; Roe, L.S.; Sanchez, C.E.; Zuraikat, F.M.; Keller, K.L.; Rolls, B.J. Both increases and decreases in energy density lead to sustained changes in preschool children’s energy intake over 5 days. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 204, 210–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Daniels, L.A. Feeding practices and parenting: A pathway to child health and family happiness. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2019, 74, 29–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Wood, C.; Blissett, J.M.; Brunstrom, J.M.; Carnell, S.; Faith, M.S.; Fisher, J.O.; Hayman, L.L.; Khalsa, A.S.; Hughes, S.O.; Miller, A.L.; Momin, S.R.; Welsh, J.A.; Woo, J.G.; Haycraft, E.; American Heart Association Council on Lifestyle and Cardiometabolic Health; Council on Epidemiology and Prevention; Council on Lifelong Congenital Heart Disease and Heart Health in the Young; Council on Cardiovascular and Stroke Nursing; Stroke Council. Caregiver influences on eating behaviors in young children. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2020, 9, e014520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Frankel, L.A.; Hughes, S.O.; O’Connor, T.M.; Power, T.G.; Fisher, J.O.; Hazen, N.L. Parental influences on children’s self-regulation of energy intake: Insights from developmental literature on emotion regulation. J. Obes. 2012, 2012, 327259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Black, M.M.; Aboud, F.E. Responsive feeding is embedded in a theoretical framework of responsive parenting. J. Nutr. 2011, 141, 490–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. DiSantis, K.I.; Hodges, E.A.; Johnson, S.L.; Fisher, J.O. The role of responsive feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: A systematic review. Int. J. Obes. 2011, 35, 480–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Hodges, E.A.; Johnson, S.L.; Hughes, S.O.; Hopkinson, J.M.; Butte, N.F.; Fisher, J.O. Development of the responsiveness to child feeding cues scale. Appetite 2013, 65, 210–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hurley, K.M.; Cross, M.B.; Hughes, S.O. A systematic review of responsive feeding and child obesity in high-income countries. J. Nutr. 2011, 141, 495–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Pérez-Escamilla, R.; Segura-Pérez, S.; Lott, M. Feeding guidelines for infants and young toddlers: A responsive parenting approach. Nutr. Today 2017, 52, 223–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Birch, L.L. Child feeding practices and the etiology of obesity. Obesity 2006, 14, 343–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Spill, M.K.; Callahan, E.H.; Shapiro, M.J.; Spahn, J.M.; Wong, Y.P.; Benjamin-Neelon, S.E.; Birch, L.; Black, M.M.; Cook, J.T.; Faith, M.S.; Mennella, J.A.; Casavale, K.O. Caregiver feeding practices and child weight outcomes: A systematic review. Am. J. Clin. 2019, 109, 990–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Vaughn, A.E.; Ward, D.S.; Fisher, J.O.; Faith, M.S.; Hughes, S.O.; Kremers, S.P.; Musher-Eizenman, D.R.; O’Connor, T.M.; Patrick, H.; Power, T.G. Fundamental constructs in food parenting practices: A content map to guide future research. Nutr. Rev. 2016, 74, 98–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Blisset, J. Relationships between parenting style, feeding style and feeding practices and fruit and vegetable consumption in early childhood. Appetite 2011, 57, 826–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Farrow, C.V.; Blissett, J. Controlling feeding practices: Cause or consequence of early child weight? Pediatrics 2008, 121, 164–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Fisher, J.O.; Birch, L.L. Restricting access to foods and children’s eating. Appetite 1999, 32, 405–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Galloway, A.T.; Fiorito, L.M.; Francis, L.A.; Birch, L.L. “Finish your soup”: Counterproductive effects of pressuring children to eat on intake and affect. Appetite 2006, 46, 318–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Mou, Y.; Jansen, P.W.; Raat, H.; Nguyen, A.N.; Voortman, T. Associations of family feeding and mealtime practices with children's overall diet quality: Results from a prospective population-based cohort. Appetite 2021, 160, 105083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Vaughn, A.E.; Tabak, R.G.; Bryant, M.J.; Ward, D.S. Measuring parent food practices: A systematic review of existing measures and examination of instruments. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2013, 10, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Birch, L.L.; Fisher, J.O.; Grimm-Thomas, K.; Markey, C.N.; Sawyer, R.; Johnson, S.L. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: A measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite 2001, 36, 201–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Loth, K.A.; Mohamed, N.; Trofholz, A.; Tate, A.; Berge, J.M. Associations between parental perception of- and concern about- child weight and use of specific food-related parenting practices. Appetite 2021, 160, 105068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Musher-Eizenman, D.; Holub, S. Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire: Validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2007, 32, 960–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Real, H.; Oliveira, A.; Severo, M.; Moreira, P.; Lopes, C. Combination and adaptation of two tools to assess parental feeding practices in preschool children. Eat. Behav. 2014, 15, 383–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Viana, V.; Franco, T.; Morais, C.; Almeida, P.; Silva, D.; Guerra, A. Controlo alimentar materno e estado ponderal: Resultados do Questionário Alimentar para Crianças. Psicol. Saúde Doenças 2012, 13, 298–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pimenta, F.; Garcia, I.Q.; Cristina, S.; Galvão, D.; Marôco, J.; Leal, I. Validation of the parental feeding style questionnaire in a Portuguese sample. Psicol. Saúde Doenças 2019, 20, 740–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Gomes, A.I.; Roberto, M.S.; Pereira, A.I.; Alves, C.; João, P.; Dias, A.R.; Veríssimo, J.; Barros, L. Development and psychometric characteristics of an instrument to assess parental feeding practices to promote young children’s eating self-regulation: Results with a Portuguese sample. Nutrients 2022, 14, 4953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Al-Qerem, W.A.; Ling, J.; AlBawab, A.Q. Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practice Questionnaire among school aged children in Jordan: A factor analysis study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Haszard, J.J.; Williams, S.M.; Dawson, A.M.; Skidmore, P.M.; Taylor, R.W. Factor analysis of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire in a large sample of children. Appetite 2013, 62, 110–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Lauzon-Guillain, B.; Musher-Eizenman, D.; Leporc, E.; Holub, S.; Charles, M.A. Parental feeding practices in the United States and in France: Relationships with child’s characteristics and parent's eating behavior. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2009, 109, 1064–1069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mais, L.A.; Warkentin, S.; Latorre, M.R.O.; Carnell, S.; Taddei, J.A.A.C. Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire among Brazilian families of school aged children. Front. Nutr. 2015, 2, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Shohaimi, S.; Wei, W.Y.; Shariff, Z.M. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Malay version Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire tested among mothers of primary school children in Malaysia. Sci. World J. 2014, 2014, 676174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Evans, A.; Seth, J.G.; Smith, S.; Harris, K.K.; Loyo, J.; Spaulding, C.; Van Eck, M.; Gottlieb, N. Parental feeding practices and concerns related to child underweight, picky eating, and using food to calm differ according to ethnicity/race, acculturation, and income. Matern. Child Health J. 2011, 15, 899–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Power, T.G.; O'Connor, T.M.; Fisher, J.O.; Hughes, S.O. Obesity risk in children: The role of acculturation in the feeding practices and styles of low-income Hispanic families. Child. Obes. 2015, 11, 715–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Francisco, B.F. Estilos, práticas e percepções e preocupações parentais relacionadas com os hábitos alimentares da criança pré-escolar. Master’s thesis, Faculdade de Psicologia, Universidade de Lisboa, Lisboa, 2016.
  55. Gosling, S.; Vazire, S.; Srivastava, S.; John, O. Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet questionnaires. Am. Psychol. 2004, 59, 93–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Brown, T.A. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research; Guilford Press: New York, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  57. Byrne, B.M. Structural equation modeling with Amos: Basic concepts, applications and programming; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  58. Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hair, J.F.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E. Multivariate data analysis, 7th ed.; Pearson: New York, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  60. Cheung, G.W.; Rensvold, R.B. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. 2002, 9, 233–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Arlinghaus, K.R.; Hernandez, D.C.; Eagleton, S.G.; Chen, T.; Power, T.G.; Hughes, S.O. Exploratory factor analysis of The Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire (CFPQ) in a low-income Hispanic sample of preschool aged children. Appetite 2019, 140, 82–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Minaie, M.; Mirzaei, K.; Heshmat, R.; Movahedi, A.; Motlagh, A.D.; Parsaeian, M.; Raghebi, S.S.; Djazayery, A. Revising the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire used in planning preventive overweight, obesity, and underweight programs for 2-5-year-old children. Int. J. Prev. Med. 2019, 10, 159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Warkentin, S.; Mais, L.A.; Latorre, M.R.D.O.; Carnell, S.; Taddei, J.A.A.C. Validation of the comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire in parents of preschool children in Brazil. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Melbye, E.L.; Ogaard, T.; Overby, N.C. Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire with parents of 10-to-12-year-olds. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2011, 11, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Saltzman, J.A.; Balantekin, K.N.; Musaad, S.; Bost, K.K.; Fiese, B.H.; STRONG Kids Team. Longitudinal factor analysis of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire among parents of preschool-aged children. Appetite 2018, 129, 94–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Carnell, S.; Wardle, J. Measuring behavioural susceptibility to obesity: Validation of the child eating behaviour questionnaire. Appetite 2007, 48, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Musher-Eizenman, D.; Lauzon-Guillain, B.; Holub, S.C.; Leporc, E.; Charles, M.L. Child and parent characteristics related to parental feeding practices. A cross-cultural examination in the US and France. Appetite 2009, 52, 89–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Subscales’ internal consistency reliability (𝛼) for the 12-factor CFPQ model.
Table 1. Subscales’ internal consistency reliability (𝛼) for the 12-factor CFPQ model.
Subscale 𝛼
Monitoring (4 items: 1,2,3,4) 0.85
Modeling (3 items: 46,47,48) 0.77
Teaching Nutrition (1 item: 25)
Balance and Variety (3 items: 24,26,38) 0.63
Environment (4 items: 14,16R,22,37R) 0.66
Involvement (3 items: 15,20,32) 0.69
Emotion Regulation (3 items: 7,8,9) 0.82
Food Reward (3 items: 19,23,36) 0.59
Child Control (2 items: 5,6) 0.46
Restriction Weight (6 items: 27,29,33,34,35,41) 0.81
Restriction Health (3 items: 21,28,40) 0.64
Pressure (3 items: 30,39,49) 0.74
Table 2. Factors, items, and loadings from Exploratory Factor Analysis.
Table 2. Factors, items, and loadings from Exploratory Factor Analysis.
Factors and Items Original Factors Factor Loading
Monitoring α = 0.85
1 Monitoring 0.81
2 Monitoring 0.91
3 Monitoring 0.64
4 Monitoring 0.73
Modeling α = 0.77
46 Modeling 0.59
47 Modeling 0.93
48 Modeling 0.78
Environment α = 0.59
14 Environment 0.58
44 Modeling 0.47
Involvement α = 0.69
15 Involvement 0.61
20 Involvement 0.76
25 Teaching Nutrition 0.41
32 Involvement 0.60
Emotion Regulation α= 0.82
7 Emotion Regulation 0.62
8 Emotion Regulation 0.88
9 Emotion Regulation 0.87
Restriction for Weight α = 0.81
27 Restriction for Weight 0.70
29 Restriction for Weight 0.77
33 Restriction for Weight 0.68
34 Restriction for Weight 0.51
35 Restriction for Weight 0.53
41 Restriction for Weight 0.52
Restriction for Health α = 0.63
21 Restriction for Health 0.69
28 Restriction for Health 0.59
43 Restriction for Health 0.45
Pressure α = 0.73
17 Pressure 0.45
30 Pressure 0.65
39 Pressure 0.72
49 Pressure 0.70
Table 3. Associations between the 8-factor CFPQ model subscales and CFQ susbscales.
Table 3. Associations between the 8-factor CFPQ model subscales and CFQ susbscales.
Title 1 Title 2 Title 3
Restriction Pressure to Eat Monitoring
Monitoring 0.02 -0.11 0.41***
Modeling 0.25*** -0.04 0.24***
Environment -0.20*** -0.14* -0.14*
Involvement -0.15* -0.13* 0.03
Emotion Regulation 0.19** 0.11 0.00
Restriction for Weight 0.31*** 0.10 0.12
Restriction for Health 0.65*** 0.27*** 0.22***
Pressure .32*** 0.77*** -0.02
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated