1. Introduction
Emotion regulation (ER) – which involves the internal or external process of monitoring, evaluating, and modulating emotional experiences [
1,
2] – is a key psychological resource and has a major impact on mental health and social adjustment [
3,
4,
5]. Although the importance of ER is widely recognized [
6,
7,
8,
9], there is little consensus on the different dimensions of ER and their differential association with psychopathology [
10].
Based on the Self-Determination Theory (SDT), Roth et al [
11], introduced the concept of
Integrative Emotion Regulation (IER). SDT is a comprehensive motivational theory aimed at elucidating the relationship between psychological development and social adaptation [
12]. Within the SDT taxonomy of ER styles, IER is characterized by an inclination toward openness and receptivity to one's own emotional states. This approach involves the impartial processing of emotions and the deliberate cultivation of full emotional awareness. In contrast,
Suppressive Emotion Regulation (SER) represents a controlled form of ER that hinges on the avoidance of emotional experiences with the goal of mitigating the impact of emotional inputs. The third mode of ER is referred to as
Dysregulated Emotion Regulation (DER), wherein emotions are perceived as overwhelming, and the individual grapples with difficulties in effectively managing these emotional states [
13]. These distinctions within ER styles, particularly the introduction of IER, contribute significantly to our understanding of how individuals navigate their emotional landscapes and how these approaches may impact mental health and social adaptation.
ER has been considered to play a particularly salient role in the context of adolescence mental health [
14], a developmental phase distinguished by heightened emotional intensity and heightened moment-to-moment variability in emotional experiences [
15,
16]. Research has consistently documented the detrimental effect of maladaptive ER strategies (i.e., SER and DER) on the adolescents’ mental health [
17,
18,
19]. In contrast, IER has been shown to decrease the risk for developing psychopathology in adolescents [
20,
21,
22]. This emerging body of research highlights the importance of further exploring of the distinctive role of ER styles in adolescent psychopathology.
Among the most prevalent mental health problems among adolescence is non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI), which has been shown to be strongly related to difficulties in ER [
23,
24,
25]. NSSI is defined as "the deliberate, non-fatal self-infliction of body tissue damage without suicidal intent" [
26], and is associated with a range of adverse outcomes, including diminished academic performance, peer-related difficulties, and an elevated risk of suicidal behaviours [
27,
28,
29].
Theoretical frameworks propose that challenges in ER are also pivotal in the development of internalizing symptoms among adolescents [
30,
31,
32]. Building on this notion, research consistently indicates that difficulties in ER predict the emergence of both internalizing and externalizing symptoms in adolescents [
33,
34,
35]. Moreover, extending our understanding of adolescent mental health, previous studies have established a clear link between NSSI and internalizing and externalizing symptoms [
36,
37]. As proposed in the
Four-function Model by Nock and Prinstein [
38], NSSI is considered as a maladaptive means of coping with negative emotions and thoughts. Difficulty tolerating intense emotions can lead to a tendency to internalize emotional distress, which in turn increases susceptibility to non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI). Delving deeper into the dynamics of ER and its impact, it has been observed that the frequency of NSSI is closely associated with emotion dysregulation. Importantly, this relationship is assumed to be mediated by the presence of internalizing symptoms [
39]. However, the role of internalizing and externalizing symptoms as underlying mechanisms in the relationship between integrative, suppressive, and dysregulated ER with NSSI has not yet been examined. Furthermore, the vast majority of studies in this area are limited to Western adolescents.
Previous studies, distinct from the novel SDT taxonomy, have elucidated cultural disparities in ER and emotional expression [
40,
41]. Nevertheless, it is essential to highlight that, so far, only Benita et al.'s study [
20] has delved into the SDT taxonomy of ER, encompassing the examination of integrative, suppressive, and dysregulated ER as universal constructs. As emphasized in a meta-analysis review by Hu et al [
42], examining the relationship between ER and mental health, it is very important to consider culture as a moderator in this regard. It is noteworthy that Western cultures, in general, tend to exhibit a leaning toward individualism when compared to Eastern cultures, with Iran occupying an intermediary position between the individualistic and collectivist cultural spectrums [
43,
44]. Iranian families exhibit a profound cultural inclination toward the socialization of their offspring, instilling a deeply ingrained reverence for elder family members. Moreover, they emphasize the preservation of familial harmony by nurturing a strong sense of filial respect toward parents and other kin. Within this cultural context, children are subtly discouraged from openly expressing dissenting opinions or disagreements within the family dynamic [
45,
46].
The present study
The present study aims to investigate the cross-cultural relevance of ER styles in the context of adolescent psychopathology. To achieve this goal, we have formulated three main objectives: First, we examined the relationship between three different ER styles (IER, SER, and DER) and adolescent psychopathology (internalizing and externalizing problems and NSSI), as well as cultural differences in these relationships. Following Ryan and Deci's [
47] theoretical framework, we expected a significant negative relationship between IER and psychological symptoms and a significant positive relationship between DER and SER and psychological symptoms in the two cultural groups. Moreover, given previous research (e.g., [
42]) indicating a stronger correlation between expressive suppression and negative mental health indicators in Western cultural groups, we expect potential cross-cultural differences in the correlation between SER and psychological symptoms. Second, we examined the mediating role of internalizing and externalizing problems in the association between ER styles and NSSI. Based on the study by Kranzler et al. [
39], we hypothesized that internalizing and externalizing problems would mediate the relationship between ER styles and NSSI in both cultural groups. Third, we examined the role of ER styles in predicting internalizing and externalizing problems and NSSI, controlling for age, gender, and country. In light of previous research describing age-related differences in ER (or a review, see [
48]), increased incidence of internalizing symptoms in women (for a review, see [
50]) and cultural differences in ER and adolescent psychopathology [
50,
51,
52], we considered the potential effects of age, gender, and country on the relationship between ER styles and psychopathology.
4. Discussion
The primary objective of this study was to illuminate cultural differences in the relationship between distinct ER modes and adolescent psychopathology based on the SDT framework and to examine the mediating role of internalizing and externalizing problems in the association between ER modes and NSSI in two different cultural groups. To achieve this goal, we compared two distinct samples: Iranian adolescents, serving as a representative sample of a more collectivist culture, and Belgian adolescents, as proxies for a more individualistic cultural context.
In line with the insights from Mesquita [
62], highlighting the pivotal role of cultural values, norms, and ideas in the development of psychological symptoms, emotion regulation (ER), and normative behavior, our study brought to light distinct patterns within the Iranian and Belgian samples. Notably, the Iranian cohort exhibited higher occurrences of internalizing symptoms, coupled with a prevalent use of Suppressive Emotion Regulation (SER) modes. This finding echoes prior research by Dadkhah and Shirinbayan [
63], indicating a substantial incidence of emotional suppression among Iranian adolescents. The prevalence of SER in this population underscores the robust influence of cultural norms on emotion regulation, particularly in collectivistic societies.
Conversely, the Belgian sample displayed higher frequencies of externalizing symptoms and a greater inclination toward Intrusive Emotion Regulation (IER). This aligns with the broader cultural context in Belgium, characterized by individualistic tendencies, as identified by Hofstede insights country comparison tool [
64]. The observed higher prevalence of IER among Belgians reflects the cultural emphasis on self-expression, self-reliance, and the pursuit of individual goals. In contrast to the collectivistic approach, the deliberate engagement with one's emotional experiences, seeking to integrate them into personal aspirations and values, aligns harmoniously with the individualistic cultural framework in Belgium.
Our findings further corroborate these cultural variations, revealing that Iranian adolescents scored significantly higher in suppressive regulation, consistent with prior studies [
41,
65,
66] a higher prevalence of emotional suppression in collectivist/non-Western cultures. This resonates with the broader pattern observed in Asian/Eastern cultures, where greater control over emotions is encouraged, potentially contributing to a higher incidence of internalizing problems [
41]. Tahmouresi et al.'s [
67] cross-cultural study, comparing Iranian and German children, similarly found a greater tendency toward emotional suppression in the Iranian context, highlighting the enduring influence of culture on emotion regulation strategies and their impact on psychological well-being.
These associations underscores the interplay between cultural values and ER strategies, contributing to a deeper understanding of the observed variations between the Belgian and Iranian samples in our study. Additionally, our findings underscore the notion that emotion dysregulation appears to exert adverse effects on adolescent mental health across diverse cultural contexts. This is evidenced by the strong associations identified between DER and internalizing and externalizing symptoms, as well as NSSI in both cultural groups.
Next, The results of the hierarchical regression indicated that DER positively predicted NSSI and internalizing and externalizing problems above and beyond age, gender, and culture. This finding further supports our bivariate correlation results indicating robust correlation between DER and psychological symptoms in both countries and little or no difference in the correlations.
Emotion dysregulation is linked to an increased reliance on negative coping mechanisms, commonly observed in both internalizing and externalizing disorder [
68]. Our findings support the conceptualization proposed by Clapham & Brausch [
69], indicating that adolescents may manifest internalizing symptoms in response to insufficient emotion regulation.
This finding is aligns with the overarching consensus that emotion dysregulation serves as a risk factor for adolescent psychopathology, as highlighted in previous research such as McLaughlin et al [
70]. Also consistent with a recent study- drawing upon data derived from 49 distinct sources- showing that people who struggle to regulate their emotions are more likely to engage in self-harm throughout their lives and emotion dysregulation is strongest predictor of NSSI [
71]. Nevertheless, our findings revealed that even when accounting for gender and age and country differences, emotion dysregulation remained correlated with NSSI. This supports the findings of Wolf et al [
71], indicating that heightened challenges in emotion regulation are connected to an increased probability of engaging in NSSI across individuals, regardless of age or gender. These results differ from studies such as Chen & Chun [
72] and Thomas & Bonnaire [
73], which have suggested gender differences in this association. The present study stands as a distinctive contribution by demonstrating the association between emotion dysregulation and psychopathology across diverse countries.
Mediation analysis revealed that both internalizing and externalizing problems served as mediating factors in the association between DER and NSSI in the Belgian sample, with internalizing problems demonstrated a more pronounced influence. In the Iranian model, only externalizing problems acted as a mediator in the relationship between DER and NSSI. This finding aligns with the results of Kranzler et al.'s study [
39], which evidenced the mediating role of internalizing problems in the relationship between emotion dysregulation and NSSI. Furthermore, SER was indirectly related to NSSI primarily through internalizing problems in the Belgian sample. While, in the Iranian sample, the association between SER and NSSI was only mediated by externalizing problems. These outcomes are consistent with previous research demonstrating links between emotion dysregulation and internalizing problems [
70,
74], as well as between internalizing problems and NSSI (e.g., [
75]). The observation that NSSI was correlated with heightened SER is particularly noteworthy in the context of the Belgian adolescents. This finding aligns with prevailing Western theories which consider NSSI as a means to evade or suppress intense emotions and thoughts, as evidenced by Hooley and Franklin [
76]. However, intriguingly, this correlation between NSSI and SER was not observed in the Iranian sample, which may suggest cultural disparities in the expressions of NSSI, as previously indicated by Gholamrezaei et al [
77]. Their research revealed lower incidence rates of NSSI in an Iranian sample and limited support for NSSI as an ER strategy, particularly among men, when compared to Western samples.