1. Introduction
Physical activity in childhood is associated with numerous health benefits [
1]. However, globally children are failing to meet the recommendations for physical activity [
2]. These low levels of physical activity are reflected in Wales with low levels of engagement reported in the school sport survey [
3] and the Active Healthy Kids, Wales Report card [
4].
Study into the relationship between motor development and lifelong physical activity is not new, with early work by Seedfeldt (1980) proposing the notion of a proficiency barrier in middle childhood if young children fail to achieve adequate levels of skill in their fundamental motor skills [
5]. Clark and Metcalfe’s 2002 model highlights the need for a broad range of movement skills that they suggest form the base camp needed to climb a variety of mountains of motor development [
6]. Further to this, the complex relationship between motor competence, perceived competence, fitness, physical activity and health was pro-posed by Stodden and colleagues in 2008, suggesting that early childhood is a critical window of opportunity to develop motor competence and support engagement in physical activity and entering a positive health trajectory [
7]. Numerous studies have since examined this relationship and demonstrated the importance of motor competence in driving physical activity engagement [
8,
9]. Research has also identified associations between motor competence and a range of broader health outcomes, including healthy weight status [
10,
11] and cardiorespiratory fitness [
12]. Studies have also linked motor competence to psychological constructs such as higher self-esteem [
13] and perceived physical competence [
14,
15]. Jones et al., (2021) suggested associations between motor competence, cognitive development and school readiness, with Veldman et al., (2019) identifying gross motor skills as positively associated with cognitive development in toddlers [
16,
17]. Research with an existing proven motor development programme, Successful Kinaesthetic Instruction for Pre-schoolers (SKIP) highlighted those children in the SKIP intervention developed better gross motor skills and executive functions [
18].
What is clear in developmental models and the existing research is the recognition of early childhood as a key window of opportunity for developing the foundational skills that will drive later physical activity [
7,
8,
19]. As such numerous motor skill interventions globally have focused on the development of motor skills in early childhood and have been shown to have significant impact on the development of motor competence. One such evidence-based motor development programme is SKIP which was developed to address developmental delay in motor skills with children in areas of deprivation [
20]. SKIP combines principles of motor development theory and physical education pedagogy and multiple research studies across a variety of contexts have examined the impact of the programme. Studies have measured the impact of SKIP on motor skills [20-22], teachers and practitioners’ ability to deliver SKIP in the USA [
23,
24], in Wales as SKIP Cymru [
25,
26] and in Indonesia as INDO-SKIP [
27,
28]. Most recently the ability to engage parents has been evaluated in SKIPing with PALS [
29].
The need for successful motor interventions such as SKIP is clear with levels of motor competence shown to be at historically low levels [30-34]. In Wales this has been particularly true with 100% of Welsh children demonstrating evidence of significant delays in motor competence [
35]. With the clear links between motor competence and physical activity, multiple programmes have been put in place to address this through professional development in schools, pre-schools, community settings and the home [
23,
24,
25,
26,
29].
However, a key gap in this literature is the lack of engagement of parents who are the first teachers of their children. Parents are key influencers of children’s physical development and studies have shown that with support parents can improve their children’s fundamental motor skills (FMS) [
36]. The important role that parents play in supporting children’s development has been acknowledged in both research and policy with the Welsh Government launching their ‘Education begins at home’ campaign in 2014 and further developing support materials for parent engagement highlighting that parent engagement is a powerful influence on school improvement. Drawing on the research of Desforges and Abouchaar (2003) the government guidance suggests that the degree of family engagement can have “six times more influence over the child’s educational attainment than the quality of the school” [
37,
38] (p. 5). Further guidance for organisations to support working with parents was published in 2017, also highlighting the important role that parents play in their children’s development [
39]. However, in all this guidance there is scarcely any mention of physical development or how parents can support their children to develop the vital motor skills that underpin their physical development other than a brief mention of physical activity in the context of weight management [
39] (p. 154).
Whilst there is recognition of the important role parents play in supporting their children’s development, including some research involving motor development [
29,
36], Agard and colleagues highlight concerning gaps between theory and practice in relation to parents’ understanding of early years physical activity [
40] and there are limited interventions that target the home environment [
41]. The Brian et al., (2023) SKIPing with PALS motor intervention with parents demonstrated increased competence for the children for parents who engaged in more sessions. Considering the global concerns surrounding childhood inactivity [
2], poor motor skills [
42] and overweight and obesity [
43], there is a growing need for action to address these issues beyond school and pre-school and empower parents to support their children’s motor development at home and in their communities. Flynn et al., (2023) in a recent review of parent engagement studies found that parental involvement in interventions significantly improved the FMS of children [
44]. However, parents reported many barriers to supporting their children’s physical development, including environmental, time and energy constraints [
45]. Brian et al (2023) highlight the potential of mobile applications (APP) to support parent engagement with their child to promote physical development. They suggest that developmentally appropriate APPs should be designed, and future research needs to examine the use of this type of technology for supporting motor development and physical activity [
29]. Such recommendations were also highlighted in the Flynn et al. (2023) review, where studies using mobile APP technology to support parents saw significant improvements in both locomotor and object control skills [
44,
46,
47]. The use of mobile APPs offers the potential to empower parents and support them to promote their children’s physical development in their own home and local community. The affordances and experiences of the home are significantly related to children motor competence [
48], therefore supporting parents to enhance the affordances and activities in their home is an opportune way to support motor skills in children. The use of technology to provide developmentally appropriate activities for parents and children in an easily accessible and instructional format could support parents globally. Such an approach could have wide-spread impact on enhancing children’s physical development before entering the school system and serve as a foundation for work done in schools and pre-schools.
As a result, the MiniMovers (MM) APP (MiniMovers.org) was developed to address global concerns about children’s developmental delays in motor skills and the gap between theory and practice for parents. The MM APP draws on over 30 years of motor development evidence from the SKIP motor skill programme and studies to design activities that are at an appropriate level for the children engaging in the activities with their parents. Combining motor development expertise with creativity expertise, the MM APP provides resources to parents to support their children’s motor competence with developmentally appropriate activities and equipment. The MM APP has three levels representing Mini, Mighty and Mega movers that represent different levels of a child’s motor competence. Built into the APP motor development experts designed an algorithm that asks parents about their child’s age and questions on their child’s motor competence. Resulting from these data, the MM APP then places the child into one of the three levels (Mini, Mighty, Mega). At each level there are four creative worlds (farm, sea life, jungle, space) with eight activities that are develop-mentally appropriate for that level of motor competence. This ensures the children are able to engage with activities at the appropriate level for their stage of physical development and as such it aims to ensure the children are engaged, experience success and make progress in their motor competence. For each activity there is a video of a parent and child performing the activity with music for motivation and animated characters relative to the world. In addition, the APP provides information to parents on how to set up the activity, the equipment needed, cues to support their child and how the activity is supporting child development.
This study was the first exploratory step in a line of future MM research to examine whether parents could utilise the MM activities at home and how parents and children responded to these activities. Additionally, to inform future research it was important to explore a variety of lab- and field-based assessment measures to determine what measures best captured changes in child motor skill outcomes as a result of the MM programme. Thus, the overall purpose of the study was to examine the feasibility and effectiveness of an 8-week MM programme on children’s motor competence. This initial pilot study addressed the following research questions: 1) What is the feasibility and experiences of families using the MM programme? 2) What is the impact of the 8-week MM programme on children’s motor skill performance? 3) What assessment methods best discriminate changes in motor performance as a result of the MM programme?
4. Discussion
There is paucity of literature around parent’s ability to promote their child’s motor competence, yet we know that parents are the primary role models and gatekeepers of young children’s physical development. This initial feasibility and pilot study demonstrates the promise of the MM APP programme to empower parents to support their children’s physical development. The overall purpose of the study was to examine the feasibility and effectiveness of an 8-week MM programme on children’s motor competence. There were three research questions.
4.1. Feasibility and Experiences of Families Using the MM Programme
Parents were consistently playing the activities with an average of 4.91 times per week over the 8-week period reported in the questionnaires. Whilst we have frequency of engagement, we do not have the dose as the parents did not report how long they played the activities. The data does show number of times of engagement across the 8 weeks ranging from 4.63 to 5.5 times per week. This is encouraging as it demonstrates that the engagement was consistent throughout. Of particular interest was that the highest mean engagement was in the last two weeks when we may have expected the families to be getting programme fatigue. This may be explained by the qualitative findings which highlighted enjoyment as a key theme. Parents highlighted in both the questionnaire feedback and the interviews how both they and their children had enjoyed the APP activities. Comments noted enjoyment of specific activities with one mother stating “The obstacle course was fab. I enjoyed setting it to challenge my kids” (M4) and another echoing this “The obstacle course was great fun and easy to set up in the house” (M5). The parents noted the ease of setting up and playing the activities “Easy to set up, I like the fact I can say, shall we do MiniMovers and we have the stuff there and ready to go and we are playing within minutes” (M6), “I’ll go, right we're going to do like mini movers and it's just easy to set up because I've got the videos to show them” (M4). The ability to set and play the activities easily removed the barriers highlighted by Bentley et al (2012) [
45]. Parents also noted their children’s enjoyment of playing games with them, “she loved it! Especially when she was moving the spots further away from each other to make it harder for her dad to hop between” (M3) and they spoke of their own enjoyment of playing with their children “really fun and engaging for both me and the kids” (M4). Parents noted that it was a good way for more social interaction with their children, “she wants to do it together” (M3) and of being less sedentary “we kind of we get in the habit of putting the telly on in the evenings, but it's a good way of not doing that” (M3). It is paramount that families enjoy the APP activities if they are going to continue to engage and, as Flynn et al. [
44] suggest, improve their children’s motor skills.
Another key theme highlighted in the data was independence. Parents noted how quickly children were able to learn the activities and then set them up independently “He would also play it without me initiating” (M4) “they'll just help themselves, and they'll set things up” (M4). The videos meant children were able to learn the activities and have more control in their playing “having the videos there makes it easier for her and means she's got a bit more control over things herself” (M5), “She put them out in different places” (M1) and “she did decide to add extra obstacles in such as cushions and the sofa” (M5). Parents also reported children adapting the games and creating their own versions “he made the river with a scarf and did little stepping stones in the river that he did all that by himself.” (M2). This ability for the children to learn activities and then go onto to practice them gives more opportunities for physical activity to drive motor competence [
7] which in turn will drive later physical activity [
8,
9]. This is of particular importance for children of this age as this is a critical window of opportunity for laying foundations for lifelong physical activity, health and psychological outcomes [7-11,13].
The final theme to emerge was parent knowledge. It was clear that parents did not know that they needed to support their children to develop motor competence prior to engaging in the study and they valued the increased understanding of how they could support their children “For like a minute we'll just practise catching, which is something I wouldn't necessarily have done before because I didn't realise how important it was” (M4). The parents were spending more time observing their children and were able to note improvements in their ability “her hopping and control/balance really improved through the game” (M3) and were getting learning to alter the equipment “It was maybe a bit hard but on advice from the research team I tried a softer ball” (M4), and they were altering the activities as children progressed such as “lots of minor adaptations to the obstacle course” (M3) and “we've used scrunched up paper as extra balls and lightly scrunched paper and stuff” (M5). As they progress through the 8-week programme parents were able to observe their children and note that changing the equipment impacted their children’s success “and then we try different balls and it's, I think really worked” (M4 week 8 feedback). These data suggest that parents’ knowledge of their children’s progress has enabled them to enhance the affordances and experiences in the home, which Flores et al., (2019) identify as being significantly related to motor competence [
48]. Parents were pleased to be able to identify when they felt their children had made progress with one M1 noting the impact of motor improvements on her daughters confidence and “these games have really benefited her cause her balance is so much better then her confidence like the other day, I caught her and she's been so risk averse I caught her trying to climb something and she was so chuffed because she could climb it.” and M2 noting “I think that he’s come on really well. Yeah, yeah, he's definitely improved.” These observations align to existing research which highlights the relationship between motor competence perceived competence [
14,
15] and self-esteem [
13]. With concerns about the lack of parents' knowledge in relation to early years physical activity [
40] and recommendations for using APP technology to support parents [
29], the MM APP offers some initial positive findings. Future research needs to explore in more depth the user experience of the APP drawing on theories of behaviour change, which was beyond the scope of this study.
4.2. Influence of the 8-Week MM Programme on Children’s Motor Skill Performance and Best Measures to Discriminate Child Outcomes
Fundamental motor skills (FMS) form the base camp to the mountain of motor development [
6] and are critical to future engagement in physical activity [
7,
9]. FMS consist of locomotor skills like running and jumping and object control skills like throwing and kicking [
60]. Early childhood is the window of opportunity in which to develop these skills [
6,
60]. The MM activities incorporate a wide variety of locomotor and object control activities in developmentally appropriate and engaging settings using music and parents as facilitators. The activities that are incorporated in the MM APP are founded on over 30 years of SKIP research across a wide variety of contexts and countries [20-29]. Thus, a strength of the MM APP is that the activities themselves have a strong evidence base. What is not clear from the research to date is whether parents can bring about meaningful changes to their children’s motor skill performance in their own homes. This study attempted to provide a very initial response to that question as both a feasibility and pilot study of the influence of the MM programme on children’s motor performance.
We used a large number of lab- (see
Table 2) and field-based (TGMD-3 and stages) measures of motor performance to examine the influence of the MM programme on the motor performance of the children. This study was exploratory in nature and the reason we under-took so many measures was to inform future research in this area. This work was partially funded by an Accelerate Wales programme that gave us access to the ATiC lab with a wide range of technologies such as an 8-camera motion capture system, GoPros and a force plate and thus the ability to undertake more sophisticated measures of movement. However, it was interesting that few of these measures resulted in significant pre-to post-test improvements in motor skills. Run time, underhand throw distance, jump distance, jump distance scaled by height, catch lag and catch duration were the only significant findings. Although we calculated these measures using the kinematic data, run, throw and jump could have easily been conducted using a stop watch and measuring tape. In all measures the small sample size and large standard deviations made it more challenging to find significance.
Overall, two locomotor skills, run and horizontal jump demonstrated significant improvements in multiple measures of the skill. For run, the TGMD-3, stage data and run duration demonstrated that the MM programme resulted in significant improvements to running process and product measures. Additionally, run velocity and step length neared significance. For horizontal jump, the TGMD-3 and jump distance plus jump distance scaled by height were significant. Children improved their jumping from 66% of their height to 77% which is quite substantial. Both skills require leg strength and multi-limb coordination and it was heartening to see that the MM activities were helping children develop in this area. All children struggled with the vertical jump and this was not considered to be a good measure for children this young. Overall, we concluded that run and horizontal jump (scaled by height) are two good measures for future research. Both are quite simple to measure. How-ever, it should be noted that the 0.5 sec decrease in run time may not be able to be detect-ed so well using a stop watch versus coding off the video.
For object control skills overhand throw, underhand throw, kick and catch were the skills that emerged as having significance. The only measure of overarm throw that was significant was the TGMD-3 going from 0.14 to 1.43 critical elements out of a possible 8 points (four critical elements by two trials). Interestingly the stage data was not significant with children going from 1.71 to 2.57. We suspect with a larger sample size we would find significance in this variable. For underarm throw both the TGMD-3 and throw distance significantly improved with children going from 2.43 critical elements to 4.71 out of a possible 8 points. It was good to see these throwing data as there were lots of activities in the MM programme that worked on throwing. For kicking both the TGMD-3 and stage data significantly improved. Children went from less than one critical element (0.71) to 4.71 points and from a stage 1-2 (M=1.57) to a stage 2-3 (M=2.86) meaning that children went from a stationary kick to a moving approach to kicking the ball. This is quite remarkable given the fairly small dose of the MM programme. Both parents and children noticed these improvements in kicking skills with one parent (M4) commenting that her child had noticed she got better and she was going to put her child in football as a result of the success and fun in the kicking activities. For catch, children significantly improved their stages going from 3.14 to 3.71. Surprisingly the TGMD-3 data did not reveal a significant improvement going from 3.86 to 5.14, probably in part due to the large standard deviations.
For catching, the trial duration was significantly longer in the post-test, with the hand more prolonged in its latency. This may have been due to the instructor who was different between pre-test and post-test so the distance, speed and trajectory were mostly likely different. Future research needs to think about the delivery of the ball in catching and try and standardise the toss to the participant. The eye tracking data revealed some interesting findings about why the children were failing to catch suggesting that sometimes their hands were late to intercept the ball flight and on other occasions they made contact with the ball but did not secure the ball in their hands dropping it to the floor. We suggest that in addition to catching success rate, future research also code if a child make contacts with the ball but drops it as this may be an early marker of child success in caching.
For those only interested in catching, the eye tracking is a very useful tool to monitor eye movement behaviour. In our tests, children over 4 years were fine to wear the Tobii Pro glasses for catching. It has been reported that the latency of eye movements such as saccade and vergence decreases with age (4.5-12 years) in children [
61]. Our results also showed that the eye latency decreased with in children of the Mega level compared to Mighty level. The Mega children were also able to track and pursue the ball when it was higher in the air, even with the head movement. However, the post-test showed slightly longer eye latency for 4 out of 5 children (except S8), as well as longer hand latency for everyone. This may be largely due to the throwing conditions not being well controlled such as differences in the instructors who threw the ball during pre- and post-tests. If a more comprehensive assessment of many FMSs is part of the research plan, we are not sure that the time and energy necessary to secure this data is worth collecting given the findings.
Overall, the MM programme resulted in significant improvements to children’s FMS. We recommend the use of the following measures in future research. For locomotor skills run and horizontal jump are good skills to evaluate from both a process (TGMD-3 and stages) and product (time, distance) stand point. Future research should consider what other locomotor skills like hopping might be of value to measure. For object control skills we recommend measuring overhand and underhand throw, kick and catch using both process (TGMD-3 and stages) and product (throw distance, catch success, catch touches of ball) measures of the skills. Additionally, the spider plots were good visual representations of change, especially when children have such large variability in their scores, and we would recommend using this approach in future research. The Mini (younger) children were quite challenging to test and we recommend that future research focus on the Mighty and Mega movers.
4.3. Limitations
The small sample size and large standard deviations are a major limitation of this paper. The standard deviations were particularly large in the Mini movers and this group of three children were quite challenging to test needing many breaks and refocusing across the testing period. However, the significant findings across multiple skills and measures of motor competence provide a robust measure of the impact of the MM programme. We recommend limiting testing to the Mighty and Mega children. Due to the small and initial nature of this study we had a very limited measure of feasibility only knowing how many days each week parents performed activities through the questionnaires. Future research needs to add additional measures of fidelity and feasibility by including what activities the children played, for how long, and how they modified them (if they did) in order to get a better measure of dose. It would also be valuable to know how the MM programme influenced behaviour change in children and parents and in what behaviours. The next steps in this line of work is to undertake a larger scale research to evaluate the MM’s APP as an ecologically valid and accessible approach to promoting motor competence in young children in a family environment.
Author Contributions
Conceptualisation, Nalda Wainwright; Data curation, Yajie Zhang, Nalda Wainwright, Jacqueline Goodway, Amanda John, Fatma Layas and Kate Piper; Formal analysis, Jacqueline Goodway and Anna Stevenson; Funding acquisition, Sean Jenkins; Investigation, Yajie Zhang, Nalda Wainwright, Amanda John, Anna Stevenson and Kirsty Thomas; Methodology, Yajie Zhang, Nalda Wainwright, Jacqueline Goodway, Amanda John and Sean Jenkins; Project administration, Amanda John, Kirsty Thomas and Sean Jenkins; Writing – original draft, Yajie Zhang, Nalda Wainwright and Jacqueline Goodway; Writing – review & editing, Yajie Zhang, Nalda Wainwright and Jacqueline Goodway.