1. Introduction
The Trail Making Test (TMT) is one of the most commonly administered tests in clinical and research neuropsychological settings. The two parts of the test [part A (TMT-A) and part B (TMT-B)] enable the evaluation of visuoperceptual tracking and processing speed (TMT-A), as well as divided attention, set-shifting and cognitive flexibility (TMT-B) [
1,
2]. Time to complete TMT-A and TMT-B is the most commonly used measure of TMT [
1,
2]; slowed TMT-B performance compared to TMT-A performance can be a sign of impaired capacity to modify a plan of action and to simultaneously keep two streams of thought [
3]. In addition to these direct TMT measures, derived scores have become popular in the last decades as sensitive measures of cognitive flexibility and executive dysfunction [
4,
5,
6]. The most popular derived TMT measures are the difference score [TMT-B – TMT-B; TMT-(B-A)], the ratio score [TMT-B / TMT-A; TMT-(B/A)], and the proportional score [(TMT-B – TMT-A) / TMT-A; TMT-(B-A/A). Specifically, it has been suggested that the TMT-(B-A) difference score removes the speed component, minimizes the visuoperceptual and motor demands and better assesses the executive control processes [
7] while the TMT-(B/A) ratio score diminishes the influence of psychomotor demands and controls for factors related to intrasubject variability [
5]. Apart from these derived measures, the examination of different error rates in TMT-A and mostly in TMT-B, i.e. sequential and perseverative errors, yield valuable information regarding the cognitive mechanisms of processing speed, attention and cognitive flexibility [
2]. Even though a variety of brain pathologies may influence TMT performance [
1,
2], TMT is widely used to evaluate executive dysfunction and prefrontal pathology [
8] while recent neuroimaging studies highlight the importance of other brain areas as well, such as the parietal cortex [
9]. Of note, the executive network includes the frontal lobe, the lateral parietal lobe, subcortical structures (e.g. anterior thalamus, caudate nucleus) and the cerebellum [
10,
11,
12,
13]. All these areas are connected through white matter tracts and thus, damage to these structures or their white matter connections could result in slow processing speed and executive dysfunction [
14,
15,
16].
Cognitive impairment is a common finding among patients with stroke. The prevalence of cognitive impairment is estimated to be between two-thirds and three-quarters of stroke survivors, depending on the methods applied to assess cognition as well as the timing of cognitive assessment, i.e. acute, sub-acute or chronic phase [
17,
18,
19]. Post-stroke cognitive impairment is associated with early and enduring changes in patients’ daily living activities and quality of life and this association is still present despite methodological differences in the study design factors (e.g. sample size, patients’ age, follow-up period) [
20]. The main cognitive processes that are assessed using TMT, i.e. processing speed, divided attention and cognitive flexibility (i.e. one of the executive processes), are often affected in patients with stroke. Of note, patients with stroke due to frontal lesions show greater executive dysfunction than patients with lesions in other regions [
21]. Decreased processing speed is an underlying cause of post-stroke cognitive dysfunction [
22] while executive dysfunction occurs in the majority of stroke patients [
23], posing critical obstacles for the quality of life of these patients [
20]. Processing speed and attention deficits remain in the chronic phase [
24] whereas the presence of executive dysfunction early after the stroke significantly predicts a poor functional outcome one year post-stroke [
25]. Previous studies have also demonstrated an association between executive dysfunction and motor function in stroke [
26], including walking and balance ability, postural control and gait [
27,
28,
29,
30]. On the other hand, processing speed and attention are associated with on-road driving performance [
31] and driving simulator data [
32] while executive dysfunction has a significant impact on driving performance since it diminishes individual resources and risk awareness [
33]. Such an impact has also been reported in stroke survivors [
34].
Considering the wide use of TMT in research and clinical settings since its introduction in neuropsychological practice, the purpose of our review was to provide a comprehensive review on the use of TMT in stroke patients in an attempt to mainly identify its role for the evaluation of post-stroke cognitive dysfunction and progression over time, the identification of the underlying neuroanatomical pathology related to impaired TMT performance, and the association with other stroke outcomes, such motor function, driving ability and quality of life.
2. TMT in Stroke
2.1. TMT as Part of Executive Function Testing
The ΤΜΤ has been used in a variety of research protocols as part of a broader study design of executive testing, due to the fact that post-stroke executive dysfunction is a significant and independent predictor of functional outcome [
35,
36,
37]. Demographic factors, such as gender and educational level, have been correlated with the increased risk of executive dysfunction after stroke [
38,
39], while greater cognitive reserve through leisure activity engagement across life can mitigate the negative effects of stroke [
40]. Planning [
41] and goal setting [
42] after stroke are complex processes and require a thorough examination with a series of assessments to give a more complete picture, taking into account the structural damage caused by the disease. Along the same lines Cardoso and colleagues (2015) emphasize on the multidimensionality of executive functions and proposed a degree of independence between logic-based and emotion-based executive processes, which should be more thoroughly investigated [
43]. Another finding that has not been thoroughly studied is the significant negative effect of binge drinking on the performance of TMT [
44].
2.2. TMT and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living
The utilization of the TMT-B has proven valuable in evaluating functional autonomy, extending its applicability beyond the post-acute phase. Notably, it has demonstrated a modest yet significant ability to anticipate potential constraints in cognitively rooted Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADLs), thus playing a pivotal role in shaping post-discharge treatment strategies, as demonstrated by Minor and colleagues [
45]. Similarly, Lipskaya-Velikovsky and colleagues embarked on a parallel investigation to assess intricate routine activities that encompassed tasks demanding advanced planning and vigilant monitoring [
36]. The direct TMT measures (time-to-complete TMT-A and TMT-B) didn't consistently yield dependable prognosticators of functional proficiency. Ghaffari and colleagues, however, brought to light a noteworthy alternative by highlighting the TMT (B-A) difference score as the singularly reliable predictor for achieving autonomy in IADL performance [
46]. The collective insights from these studies illuminate the multifaceted utility of TMT-B, not only in delineating functional autonomy and limitations but also in guiding therapeutic pathways and interventions tailored to enhance cognitive and practical independence.
2.3. TMT and Driving Ability
Neuropsychological test performance on tests that measure cognitive/psychomotor speed (TMT A) [
47] and executive functioning (TMT B) [
34] are the most suitable metrics for predicting driving test outcomes. Some researchers provide optimal cut-off points for TMT-A and TMT-B that can predict post-stroke unsafe driving, i.e. 32 seconds for TMT-A and 79 seconds for TMT-B [
48]. The combination of TMT with other neuropsychological tests, such as the Snellgrove Maze Task [
49], the Useful Field of View test for lane maintenance [
50], the Symbol Digit Modalities Test [
51], the Rapid Pace Walk test [
52] and the Key Search Test of the BADS [
34], provides additional data for the prediction of driving ability. Another variant of the TMT is the version in the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS; [
53]). It has additional tasks on visual search, processing speed and motor speed, requiring higher levels of visual exploration and is considered to be a significant predictor of off-line motor learning [
54]. Another reliable variant of TMT-B presented by Lee and colleagues, i.e. driving TMT-B (DTMT-B), used a driving simulator in three-dimensional spaces to test the executive functions of drivers [
55].
2.4. TMT and Gait Assessment
Numerous studies highlight the interplay between attention, processing speed and cognitive flexibility, as assessed by the TMT, and gait assessment in stroke patients. Executive dysfunction, a common post-stroke sequel, is often linked to compromised gait performance [
27,
56]. Poorer performance on complex gait tests is frequently associated with worse scores on the TMT, reflecting a shared vulnerability in cognitive and motor domains [
57,
58]. Dual-task training interventions targeting both cognitive tasks and gait tasks have consistently demonstrated positive effects on executive function and balance, reinforcing the connection between cognitive demands and gait control in stroke survivors [
59,
60]. Despite the reported significant associations between TMT scores and gait performance which highlight the importance of processing speed and executive functions in walking abilities [
61,
62], there are negative findings as well supporting that gait performance is not universally linked to cognitive function, as evidenced by studies reporting no significant correlation between community ambulation and executive function [
63]. This divergence underscores the complexity of their interaction, hinting at potential modulating factors that determine the extent of post-stroke cognitive-motor interdependence [
64,
65,
66].
2.5. TMT and Speech Abilities
A recent study highlighted the usefulness of the TMT processing speed component in assessing the underlying processes related to verbal fluency [
67]. Of interest, the association between TMT processing speed component and verbal fluency remained significant even after controlling for motor deficits and dysarthria in stroke patients [
67].. The study identified shared cognitive processes contributing to fluency tasks. Lesion analysis highlighted the role of left lesions involving deep hemispheric structures and specific brain areas in verbal fluency tasks [
67]. In the study of Rajtar-Zembaty and colleagues, patients with aphasia demonstrated a notably higher number of errors in TMT-B compared to non-aphasia and dysarthria groups. This deficit in cognitive flexibility within the aphasia group was associated with dysfunction in the prefrontal cortex, which plays a role in both language skills and components of executive functions [
68]. Another study indicated that TMT performance, particularly in TMT-B, was below normal for all groups, with only a subset of aphasics successfully completing the test. The study suggested that TMT might not be the most suitable tool for evaluating left hemisphere (LH) damage and aphasic patients due to their specific impairments [
69]. Moreover, Niessen and colleagues (Niessen et al., 2020) suggested that despite clinically relevant cognitive deficits, including aphasia and apraxia, behavioral impairments related to performance monitoring and error processing in LH stroke patients, as measured by the TMT, were not evident; executive dysfunction was present based on TMT scores, but this did not directly translate into observed behavioral impairments in performance monitoring and error processing [
70].
The aforementioned studies collectively suggest that the relationship between the TMT and speech abilities, particularly in aphasic patients, involves cognitive flexibility, prefrontal cortex dysfunction, and common neuroanatomical circuits associated with language skills and executive functions. The suitability of the TMT as an evaluation tool for aphasic patients varies, and despite observed executive dysfunction, it may not always translate to significant behavioral impairments in certain tasks.
2.6. TMT and Mood Status
The relationship between the TMT and mood status has been explored through various studies, revealing insights into cognitive function and emotional well-being. Donnellan and colleagues did not find any significant correlation between the TMT-(B-A) difference score and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [
71]. The absence of correlations suggests that the TMT difference score might not be directly linked to mood disturbances as assessed by the HADS. Another study examined the relationship between diabetes, depressive symptoms, stroke severity, and TMT performance [
72]. The comorbidity index of diabetes and depressive symptoms was associated with poorer performance on the TMT-B (cognitive flexibility and task-switching abilities). However, no significant association was observed between the comorbidity index and TMT-A (visual attention and processing speed). Stroke severity and time since stroke were additional predictors of both TMT-A and TMT-B scores, suggesting complex interactions between medical and mood-related factors in determining cognitive performance [
72]. Patients with post-stroke apathy experienced more pronounced cognitive impairments and deficits in attention and executive functions; apathy scale scores were correlated with TMT-A scores at admission, and both TMT-A and TMT-B scores, along with other cognitive tests, at discharge [
73]. Overall, these studies contribute to a deeper understanding of how TMT performance is influenced by and may influence mood-related factors in various contexts.
2.7. TMT in Interventional Studies
The body of research encompassing interventional studies employing the TMT to assess cognitive function in stroke patients presents a compelling narrative of diverse approaches yielding positive outcomes. These studies collectively underscore the potential of targeted interventions to enhance cognitive abilities, with the TMT serving as a robust indicator of progress [
74,
75,
76,
77,
78,
79,
80,
81,
82,
83,
84,
85,
86]. Another study emphasizes the significance of comprehensive training interventions, revealing significant improvements in various measures, including TMT performance, grip strength, and motor function [
87]. Another investigation into virtual reality interventions highlights the role of sensory-motor stimulation in fostering improvements in TMT-A and TMT-B scores [
88]. Moreover, Gjellesvik and colleagues explored the enduring benefits of interventions by reporting sustained improvements in TMT-B completion time, potentially attributed to heightened arousal and cerebral blood volume changes [
89]. These findings are buttressed by the results from a recent study, underscoring the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in enhancing cognitive measures, including TMT-A and TMT-B [
90]. The significance of intervention adherence surfaces in Ihle-Hansen study, offering an essential aspect to consider in designing effective cognitive enhancement programs [
40]. Interestingly, the advent of technology, such as virtual reality and interactive video games, introduces a novel dimension to cognitive rehabilitation [
91]. Such interventions not only foster improvements in TMT performance but also underscore the potential for technology-driven approaches to positively impact executive function. While the majority of studies point to positive outcomes, a few studies highlight the complexities of intervention effects [
92]. The potential ceiling effect observed in control groups raises questions about the nuances of experimental design and participant characteristics that warrant further investigation.
The TMT emerges as a pivotal tool within interventional studies targeting cognitive function in stroke patients. Its consistent inclusion across diverse interventions underscores its significance as a sensitive measure for assessing cognitive improvements. The TMT's ability to capture changes in processing speed, attention, and executive function provides researchers with a standardized and quantifiable metric to gauge the efficacy of interventions. Its wide applicability, as demonstrated in studies ranging from cognitive rehabilitation to technology-driven approaches, highlights the test's versatility in evaluating various intervention modalities. Consequently, the TMT not only serves as an objective marker of progress but also enables comparisons across studies, facilitating a comprehensive understanding of the nuanced impacts of interventions on cognitive recovery post-stroke.
2.8. TMT and Clinical Features
The TMT stands as a cornerstone in neuropsychological assessment, providing valuable insights into cognitive function, particularly executive functions and attention. Recent studies have delved into the intricate interplay between TMT performance and an array of clinical features, unveiling a diverse landscape of connections in various patient populations.
In a study by Einstad and colleagues, completion time exceeding 167 seconds on TMT-B emerged as a key indicator of impaired executive function, shedding light on a threshold for executive dysfunction in a significant proportion of patients [
26]. Mobility and grip strength were intriguingly linked to global cognitive impairments, executive dysfunction, and memory impairment whereas a higher dual-task cost specifically correlated with executive dysfunction assessed by TMT-B [
26]. Bian and colleagues brought cerebrovascular health into focus, showcasing how the breath-holding index could serve as an insightful parameter for evaluating cerebrovascular reserve impairment, particularly in individuals with leukoaraiosis [
93]. This finding adds depth to our understanding of the cognitive consequences of cerebrovascular health. Jo and colleagues added a new layer of complexity by demonstrating the interdependence of cognitive function and post-stroke dysphagia severity [
94]. Visual attention and executive functions were pinpointed as influential factors in the oral phase of swallowing, emphasizing the intricate connection between cognitive and motor processes.
The predictive power of cognitive assessment tools took center stage in another study where brief screening instruments exhibited consistent utility, unlike more domain-specific cognitive tests [
95]. This highlights the importance of selecting appropriate tools for accurate cognitive assessment in different clinical contexts. Sörös and colleagues underscored the limitations of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) as an independent cognitive screening tool for transient ischemic attack (TIA) and minor stroke patients [
96]. The prevalence of executive dysfunction, particularly in TMT Part A and Part B, further emphasized the need for comprehensive cognitive assessment tools. Pedersen and colleagues connected fibrinogen concentrations to cognitive outcomes, emphasizing the role of this factor in younger stroke patients' cognitive performance [
97]. Interestingly, the association was specific to TMT-A, indicating distinct patterns of cognitive impairment related to different cognitive tasks [
97].
In the realm of treatment outcomes, Lattanzi and colleagues demonstrated the potential benefits of endovascular treatment combined with intravenous thrombolysis on cognitive performance, hinting at promising avenues for enhancing cognitive outcomes in ischemic stroke patients [
98]. Different perspectives opened doors to understanding several factors driving TMT performance post-stroke. Two recent studies underscored genetic and clinical factors' impact on cognitive outcomes [
99,
100]. Genetic polymorphisms and clinical conditions like atrial fibrillation were tied to executive dysfunction and cognitive performance, providing a holistic view of the factors shaping cognitive outcomes. The links between blood pressure and cognitive performance were also examined, revealing a more refined relationship independent of sociodemographic and clinical factors, further contributing to our understanding of the complex relationship between blood pressure and cognitive health [
101].
According to potential biomarkers, Shaheen and colleagues explored the correlation between serum levels of IL-8 and executive functions in early acute ischemic stroke patients, opening new avenues for understanding the biological underpinnings of cognitive impairment [
102]. Rosenbaum Halevi and colleagues hinted at the potential impact of treatment strategies, suggesting a potential cognitive improvement in some cognitive tests at 90 days post-stroke, highlighting the dynamic nature of cognitive recovery [
103]. The intricate relationship between cerebral blood flow and cognitive function was demonstrated by Altmann and colleagues, indicating that transcranial Doppler ultrasonography could serve as an early diagnostic tool for cognitive impairment post-stroke [
104]. Another study ventured into pharmacological interventions, showcasing how fluoxetine administration might influence cognitive function and serum levels of neurotrophic factors in patients with vascular cognitive impairment [
105]. The longitudinal perspective was provided by Ling and colleagues who highlighted the predictive power of systolic blood pressure and lacunes in assessing cognitive outcomes over time in a specific patient population [
106]. Finally, Kotlega and colleagues delved into fatty acid metabolism's role in cognitive outcomes, underscoring the multifaceted interplay between metabolic cascades and stroke-related cognitive impairment [
107].
The collective findings of these studies underscore the significance of the TMT in unraveling the intricate relationships between cognitive function and various clinical factors in stroke population. The TMT serves not only as an assessment tool but as a window into the intricate web of cognitive health, offering insights that pave the way for tailored interventions and a deeper understanding of cognitive outcomes post-stroke.
2.9. TMT and Neuroanatomical Features
The interplay between cognitive functions and neuroanatomical features has become a focal point for researchers seeking to comprehend the underlying mechanisms of cognitive impairments resulting from brain lesions.
An intriguing trajectory of TMT-A performance improvement from acute to subacute phases following stroke is suggested by Dacosta-Aguayo and colleagues [
108]. Notably, better TMT-A performance aligns with higher fractional anisotropy (FA) in specific white matter tracts, offering insights into the relationship between white matter integrity and cognitive recovery. Another research unveiled the association between TMT-A completion time and medial temporal lobe atrophy, as well as global cortical atrophy and lower education levels [
109]. These findings highlight the potential of structural brain changes and educational background as rapid indicators of cognitive impairment following transient ischemic attacks and mild ischemic strokes. The study by Cipolotti and colleagues challenges conventional categorizations by demonstrating no significant differences in frontal executive tasks among different etiology subgroups [
110]. Instead, strong effects of premorbid IQ and age on cognitive tasks suggest the influence of broader cognitive factors on executive function. The intricate relationship between executive function, white matter integrity, stroke characteristics, and cerebrovascular risk is investigated by Veldsman and colleagues who emphasize the mediating role of white matter integrity, highlighting its significance in explaining executive dysfunction and incident stroke, which are both manifestations of cerebrovascular risk factors [
111]. Hagberg and colleagues focus on the relationship between TMT-A and amyloid deposition and identify that cortical amyloid deposition does not significantly correlate with neurodegeneration or cognition in stroke survivors with cognitive impairment, emphasizing the complexity of post-stroke cognitive decline and the need to explore additional factors beyond amyloid pathology [
112]. A more recent study highlights the potential for brain compensatory mechanisms following stroke [
113]. Increased degree centrality values in the right parahippocampal gyrus correlate positively with TMT-A and TMT-B scores, suggesting the brain adaptive capacity to promote cognitive recovery [
113].
The neural underpinnings of TMT performance are explored by Singh and colleagues, highlighting the intricate connection between spatial planning, working memory, and visual search processes [
114]. A recent longitudinal exploration adds a temporal dimension, demonstrating how incident lacunes are independently associated with incident stroke and changes in TMT Part B performance [
117]. This longitudinal perspective underscores the long-lasting consequences of structural changes and their impact on cognitive trajectories. Varjačić and colleagues focus on executive set-switching and underscore the critical role of the left insular cortex [
115]. By identifying the association between lesions in this region and poorer executive set-switching, the study provides valuable insights into the neural underpinnings of attentional flexibility. Furthermore, the persistence of the lesion effect even after accounting for lower-level cognitive processes suggests the insula involvement in higher-order regulatory functions. Another research delves into mental flexibility deficits and their relationship with damaged neural connections [
116]. The study unveils the intricate connectivity patterns linking various cortical and subcortical structures implicated in cognitive control and attention networks. An investigation into bimanual grasp coordination following hemispheric strokes underscores the intricate relationship between cognitive and motor functions [
118]. The study describes how lesion side and cognitive processes interact to influence motor coordination, providing insights into the interplay between perception, action planning, and lesion site. Jankowska and colleagues study emphasizes the wide-ranging effects of stroke locations on executive dysfunction. Contrary to expectations, executive dysfunction is not confined to anterior stroke locations; even posterior and subcortical lesions contribute to the impairment [
119]. This underlines the necessity for tailored treatments based on lesion locations. Shin and colleagues examine cerebellar strokes and reinforce the role of cerebellar sites in neuropsychological functioning [
120]. The presence of lesions in the right posterior intermediate lobe of the cerebellum correlates with poorer performance in subtests evaluating executive function, such as TMT, shedding light on the specific cognitive consequences of cerebellar lesions. The identification of correlations with the TMT performance underscores the importance of considering connectomics in lesion-symptom mapping, reinforcing the interdependence of regional structures in shaping cognitive outcomes.
In a recent study, Ferris and colleagues introduce DTI metrics of the anterior thalamic radiation as potential imaging biomarkers of post-stroke cognitive impairment [
121]. The association between ATR microstructure and processing speed and executive function performance underscores the value of lesion location-specific metrics in predicting cognitive outcomes. Muir’s findings reveal associations between larger infarcts, CHIPS severity, and various metrics of set shifting and processing speed. Moreover, the association between left superior longitudinal fasciculus damage and TMT-(B-A) score further elucidates the role of specific white matter tracts in cognitive performance [
122]. Another study utilizes cluster analysis to classify TMT-B performance groups based on derived measures and identifies brain sites associated with different performance levels, emphasizing the role of specific neural structures in shaping cognitive outcomes during TMT-B completion [
123]. In addition, Kopp and colleagues shed light on the patterns of errors in TMT-A and TMT-B tasks [
124] in association to lesion patterns. They identify that the number of errors, but not completion time on the TMT-B, is associated with right hemispheric frontal lesions while the prevalence of type B shifting errors suggests a failure to switch between numbers and letters, indicating the complex nature of cognitive processes involved in these tasks [
124].
In conclusion, the diverse array of studies examining the relationship between the TMT and neuroanatomical features has provided a multifaceted perspective on the intricate interplay between brain lesions and cognitive functions. These investigations have underscored the localized nature of cognitive deficits, revealed the significance of specific brain regions such as the insular cortex and parahippocampal gyrus, and illuminated the dynamic role of white matter integrity in shaping post-stroke cognitive outcomes. Additionally, the varying impact of lesion side, cognitive domains, and anatomical sites has challenged conventional categorizations and emphasized the need for tailored interventions. As our understanding of the complex relationships between brain structure and cognitive function continues to deepen, these findings offer promising avenues for refining rehabilitation strategies, personalized treatment plans, and imaging biomarkers to mitigate the cognitive consequences of brain lesions.
3. Discussion
Τhe present literature review attempts to describe and analyze the use and application of the TMT in the clinical field of stroke. To the best of our knowledge, no such an attempt has previously occurred despite the widespread use of TMT in stroke studies. Through a comprehensive review of the literature in the last decade, we provide a conceptual framework of the available findings, further highlighting potential gaps and challenges of the application of TMT in clinical and research settings (
Figure 1).
The association between TMT performance and IADLs underscores the clinical relevance of TMT in predicting individuals' capacity to navigate daily life tasks independently, making it pertinent for rehabilitation centers and long-term care facilities. Moreover, its role in assessing driving ability reveals its potential for safeguarding not only the patient but also the broader community, indicating its importance in assessing fitness to drive and informing licensing authorities.
The integration of TMT into executive function testing illuminates its pivotal role in revealing deficits in higher-order cognitive processes, aiding clinicians in formulating tailored rehabilitation strategies for stroke survivors seeking to regain cognitive and functional independence. Its linkage to gait assessment emphasizes the intricate interplay between cognitive and motor functions, offering novel insights into post-stroke mobility issues, thereby finding application in physical therapy and geriatric care settings.
The correlation between TMT outcomes and speech abilities underscores its sensitivity to language-related impairments post-stroke, making it a crucial tool in speech therapy interventions. Equally noteworthy is the potential of TMT to capture mood status, facilitating a more comprehensive understanding of the emotional toll of stroke and aiding in holistic patient management, which is relevant in both clinical and psychological support context.
The incorporation of TMT within interventional studies holds promise for refining therapeutic approaches and measuring treatment efficacy, contributing to the advancement of stroke rehabilitation protocols in research institutions and clinical trials. Its robustness in capturing clinical and neuroanatomical features further underscores its potential as a biomarker for stroke severity and lesion localization, which has implications for both academic research and diagnostic settings.
Therefore, our comprehensive review underscores that the TMT stands as an invaluable asset in the stroke assessment toolkit, contributing nuanced insights into diverse cognitive, functional, and emotional dimensions. As research progresses, continued exploration of the TMT potential across these domains is encouraged, fostering a deeper comprehension of post-stroke dynamics and enhancing patient-centered care across hospitals, rehabilitation centers, research institutions, and community health settings. Its integration into both research and clinical practice reaffirms TMT status as an indispensable instrument in stroke-related evaluations, enabling holistic insights that extend beyond traditional neurological assessments.
Strengths of this comprehensive review include a rigorous and comprehensive database search, covering a wide array of topics, ranging from TMT associations with cognitive and functional outcomes to its implications in various domains such as driving ability, executive functions, mood status, and more. This breadth of coverage ensures a holistic understanding of TMT multifaceted utility within the context of stroke assessment. Secondly, the incorporation of diverse sources of evidence, including RCTs, original studies, clinical trials, and theoretical frameworks, lends depth to the analysis and strengthens the conclusions drawn. Additionally, the emphasis on the applicability of TMT findings in clinical and research frameworks underscores the practical relevance of the review's insights. The present review has some limitations that need to be addressed. The review's reliance on existing literature up to a certain knowledge cutoff date could potentially omit relevant studies published afterward. The diversity of stroke populations, varying degrees of stroke severity, and individual patient characteristics might introduce heterogeneity in the results across studies, potentially influencing the generalizability of the findings. The diversity and the number of the surveys included did not allow quality assessment of the included studies nor a meta-analysis of the results.
Future research endeavors in the realm of the TMT and stroke assessment hold great promise for refining clinical practice and advancing our knowledge. Longitudinal studies tracking TMT performance over time can illuminate cognitive recovery trajectories post-stroke, while investigations into the predictive validity of TMT scores can establish its role as a prognostic tool. Exploring neuroplasticity and cognitive training interventions in relation to TMT performance can uncover avenues for enhancing recovery. Integrating TMT with neuroimaging can deepen our understanding of its neural correlates, and accounting for cultural and linguistic factors can lead to culturally sensitive assessments. The feasibility of technology integration, the role of TMT in special populations, and combining it with other assessments all offer avenues for comprehensive stroke evaluations. Continual standardization efforts and updated norms ensure TMT consistent and reliable use. Overall, these research directions can enrich stroke rehabilitation strategies, bolster patient outcomes, and advance our comprehension of cognitive function and post-stroke recovery dynamics.
4. Conclusions
In conclusion, the TMT emerges as a versatile and valuable neuropsychological tool with significant implications for stroke-related assessments. Our comprehensive review of its applications within the stroke population highlights its multifaceted utility in evaluating various domains of cognitive and functional abilities, spanning both acute and chronic phases of stroke recovery.
Author Contributions
A.T. and F.C. reviewed the literature, screened the abstracts of the reference list, and assessed the articles; D.T. and P.V. solved any disagreement regarding screening or selection process; A.T. and F.C. wrote the first manuscript; S.K., P.B., C.K., A.S., N.A., K.V. reviewed the draft and provided valuable feedback. The corrected version was discussed collegially. K.T., A.T. and F.C. updated and wrote the final version. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was funded by the project “Study of the interrelationships between neuroimaging, neurophysiological and biomechanical biomarkers in stroke rehabilitation (NEURO-BIO-MECH in stroke rehab)” (MIS 5047286), which was implemented under the action “Support for Regional Excellence” funded by the operational program “Competitiveness, Entrepreneurship and Innovation” (NSRFm2014-2020) and co-financed by Greece and the European Union (the European Regional Development Fund).
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary, 3rd ed. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 2006.
- Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, et al. Neuropsychological assessment, 5th ed. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 2012.
- Arbuthnott K, Frank J. Trail Making Test, Part B as a measure of executive control: Validation using a set-switching paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 2000; 22: 518–528. [CrossRef]
- Lamberty GJ, Axelrod BN. Derived adult Trail Making Test indices. In: The quantified process approach to neuropsychological assessment. Philadelphia, PA, US: Taylor & Francis; 2006: 161–171.
- Lamberty GJ, Putnam SH, Chatel DM, et al. Derived Trail Making Test indices: A preliminary report. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology 1994; 7: 230–234.
- Christidi F, Kararizou E, Triantafyllou N, et al. Derived Trail Making Test indices: demographics and cognitive background variables across the adult life span. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2015; 22: 667–678. [CrossRef]
- Sánchez-Cubillo I, Periáñez JA, Adrover-Roig D, et al. Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009; 15: 438–450. [CrossRef]
- Demakis GJ. Frontal lobe damage and tests of executive processing: a meta-analysis of the category test, stroop test, and trail-making test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004; 26: 441–450. [CrossRef]
- Varjacic A, Mantini D, Demeyere N, et al. Neural signatures of Trail Making Test performance: Evidence from lesion-mapping and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 2018; 115: 78–87. [CrossRef]
- Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci 2010; 14: 277–290. [CrossRef]
- Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 2349–2356. [CrossRef]
- Beuriat P-A, Cristofori I, Gordon B, et al. The shifting role of the cerebellum in executive, emotional and social processing across the lifespan. Behavioral and Brain Functions 2022; 18: 6. [CrossRef]
- Bellebaum C, Daum I. Cerebellar involvement in executive control. Cerebellum 2007; 6: 184–192. [CrossRef]
- Turken AU, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Bammer R, et al. COGNITIVE PROCESSING SPEED AND THE STRUCTURE OF WHITE MATTER PATHWAYS: CONVERGENT EVIDENCE FROM NORMAL VARIATION AND LESION STUDIES. Neuroimage 2008; 42: 1032–1044. [CrossRef]
- Filley CM. Cognitive Dysfunction in White Matter Disorders: New Perspectives in Treatment and Recovery. JNP 2021; 33: 349–355. [CrossRef]
- Filley CM, Fields RD. White matter and cognition: making the connection. J Neurophysiol 2016; 116: 2093–2104. [CrossRef]
- Jaillard A, Naegele B, Trabucco-Miguel S, et al. Hidden dysfunctioning in subacute stroke. Stroke 2009; 40: 2473–2479. [CrossRef]
- Jokinen H, Melkas S, Ylikoski R, et al. Post-stroke cognitive impairment is common even after successful clinical recovery. Eur J Neurol 2015; 22: 1288–1294. [CrossRef]
- Renjen PN, Gauba C, Chaudhari D. Cognitive Impairment After Stroke. Cureus 2015; 7: e335. [CrossRef]
- Stolwyk RJ, Mihaljcic T, Wong DK, et al. Poststroke Cognitive Impairment Negatively Impacts Activity and Participation Outcomes. Stroke 2021; 52: 748–760. [CrossRef]
- Han M, Kim D-Y, Leigh J-H, et al. Value of the Frontal Assessment Battery Tool for Assessing the Frontal Lobe Function in Stroke Patients. Ann Rehabil Med 2020; 44: 261–272. [CrossRef]
- Su C-Y, Wuang Y-P, Lin Y-H, et al. The Role of Processing Speed in Post-Stroke Cognitive Dysfunction. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2015; 30: 148–160. [CrossRef]
- Povroznik JM, Ozga JE, Haar CV, et al. Executive (dys)Function after Stroke: Special Considerations for Behavioral Pharmacology. Behav Pharmacol 2018; 29: 638–653. [CrossRef]
- Turunen KEA, Laari SPK, Kauranen TV, et al. Domain-Specific Cognitive Recovery after First-Ever Stroke: A 2-Year Follow-Up. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 2018; 24: 117–127. [CrossRef]
- Leśniak M, Bak T, Czepiel W, et al. Frequency and prognostic value of cognitive disorders in stroke patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008; 26: 356–363. [CrossRef]
- Einstad MS, Saltvedt I, Lydersen S, et al. Associations between post-stroke motor and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr 2021; 21: 103. [CrossRef]
- Hayes S, Donnellan C, Stokes E. Associations between executive function and physical function poststroke: a pilot study. Physiotherapy 2013; 99: 165–171. [CrossRef]
- Hayes S, Donnellan C, Stokes E. Executive dysfunction and balance function post-stroke: A cross-sectional study. Physiotherapy 2016; 102: 64–70. [CrossRef]
- Liu-Ambrose T, Pang MYC, Eng JJ. Executive function is independently associated with performances of balance and mobility in community-dwelling older adults after mild stroke: implications for falls prevention. Cerebrovasc Dis 2007; 23: 203–210. [CrossRef]
- Yu H, Zhang Q, Liu S, et al. Effect of Executive Dysfunction on Posture Control and Gait after Stroke. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 2021; 2021: e3051750. [CrossRef]
- Wadley VG, Bull TP, Zhang Y, et al. Cognitive Processing Speed Is Strongly Related to Driving Skills, Financial Abilities, and Other Instrumental Activities of Daily Living in Persons With Mild Cognitive Impairment and Mild Dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2020; 76: 1829–1838. [CrossRef]
- Beaulieu-Bonneau S, Fortier-Brochu É, Ivers H, et al. Attention following traumatic brain injury: Neuropsychological and driving simulator data, and association with sleep, sleepiness, and fatigue. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 2017; 27: 216–238.
- Bekiaris E, Amditis A, Panou M. DRIVABILITY: a new concept for modelling driving performance. Cogn Tech Work 2003; 5: 152–161. [CrossRef]
- Motta K, Lee H, Falkmer T. Post-stroke driving: examining the effect of executive dysfunction. J Safety Res 2014; 49: 33–38. [CrossRef]
- Kim H, Lee Y-N, Jo E-M, et al. Reliability and Validity of Culturally Adapted Executive Function Performance Test for Koreans with Stroke. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 2017; 26: 1033–1040. [CrossRef]
- Lipskaya-Velikovsky L, Zeilig G, Weingarden H, et al. Executive functioning and daily living of individuals with chronic stroke: measurement and implications. Int J Rehabil Res 2018; 41: 122–127. [CrossRef]
- Park SH, Sohn MK, Jee S, et al. The Characteristics of Cognitive Impairment and Their Effects on Functional Outcome After Inpatient Rehabilitation in Subacute Stroke Patients. Ann Rehabil Med 2017; 41: 734–742. [CrossRef]
- Akbar M, Tammasse J, Bintang AK, et al. The frequency and determinants of executive dysfunction among patients with subacute phase of ischemic stroke in West Nusa Tenggara, Indonesia. Bali Medical Journal 2023; 12: 249–254.
- Arsic S, Konstantinovic L, Eminovic F, et al. Correlation between the quality of attention and cognitive competence with motor action in stroke patients. BioMed Research International 2015; 2015.
- Ihle-Hansen H, Langhammer B, Lydersen S, et al. A physical activity intervention to prevent cognitive decline after stroke: Secondary results from the Life After STroke study, an 18-month randomized controlled trial. J Rehabil Med 2019; 51: 646–651. [CrossRef]
- Andrews G, Halford GS, Chappell M, et al. Planning following stroke: a relational complexity approach using the tower of london. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8: 1032. [CrossRef]
- Slavin SJ, McCune-Richardson L, Moore J, et al. Cognitive Testing During Mild Acute Ischemic Stroke Predicts Long-Term Return to Work. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases 2022; 31.
- Cardoso CO, Branco LD, Cotrena C, et al. Correlational analysis of performance in executive function tasks after stroke. Psychology and Neuroscience 2015; 8: 56–65.
- Laari SPK, Kauranen TV, Turunen KEA, et al. Executive Dysfunction Related to Binge Drinking in Ischemic Stroke. Cogn Behav Neurol 2020; 33: 23–32. [CrossRef]
- Minor M, Jaywant A, Toglia J, et al. Discharge Rehabilitation Measures Predict Activity Limitations in Patients With Stroke 6 Months After Inpatient Rehabilitation. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2022; 101: 761–767. [CrossRef]
- Ghaffari A, Rostami HR, Akbarfahimi M. Predictors of Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Performance in Patients with Stroke. Occup Ther Int 2021; 2021: 6675680. [CrossRef]
- Aslaksen PM, Ørbo M, Elvestad R, et al. Prediction of on-road driving ability after traumatic brain injury and stroke. Eur J Neurol 2013; 20: 1227–1233. [CrossRef]
- Choi SY, Lee JS, Oh YJ. Cut-off point for the trail making test to predict unsafe driving after stroke. J Phys Ther Sci 2016; 28: 2110–2113. [CrossRef]
- Barco PP, Wallendorf MJ, Snellgrove CA, et al. Predicting road test performance in drivers with stroke. Am J Occup Ther 2014; 68: 221–229. [CrossRef]
- Hird MA, Vesely KA, Tasneem T, et al. A Case-Control Study Investigating Simulated Driving Errors in Ischemic Stroke and Subarachnoid Hemorrhage. Front Neurol 2018; 9: 54. [CrossRef]
- Kobayashi Y, Omokute Y, Mitsuyama A, et al. Predictors of track test performance in drivers with stroke. Turkish Neurosurgery 2017; 27: 530–536.
- Tiu J, Harmon AC, Stowe JD, et al. Feasibility and validity of a low-cost racing simulator in driving assessment after stroke. Geriatrics (Switzerland) 2020; 5.
- Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. Im Internet: https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft15082-000; Stand: 05.09.2023.
- Al-Dughmi M, Al-Sharman A, Stevens S, et al. Executive Function Is Associated With Off-Line Motor Learning in People With Chronic Stroke. J Neurol Phys Ther 2017; 41: 101–106. [CrossRef]
- Lee S, Lee JA, Choi H. Driving Trail Making Test part B: a variant of the TMT-B. J Phys Ther Sci 2016; 28: 148–153. [CrossRef]
- Singh T, Perry CM, Fritz SL, et al. Eye Movements Interfere With Limb Motor Control in Stroke Survivors. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2018; 32: 724–734. [CrossRef]
- Hamre C, Fure B, Helbostad JL, et al. Impairments in spatial navigation during walking in patients 70 years or younger with mild stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil 2020; 27: 601–609. [CrossRef]
- Park M-O, Lee S-H. Effects of cognitive-motor dual-task training combined with auditory motor synchronization training on cognitive functioning in individuals with chronic stroke: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Medicine (Baltimore) 2018; 97: e10910. [CrossRef]
- Kim J, Yim J. Effects of an exercise protocol for improving handgrip strength and walking speed on cognitive function in patients with chronic stroke. Medical Science Monitor 2017; 23: 5402–5409.
- Park M-O, Lee S-H. Effect of a dual-task program with different cognitive tasks applied to stroke patients: A pilot randomized controlled trial. NeuroRehabilitation 2019; 44: 239–249. [CrossRef]
- Sakai K, Hosoi Y, Harada Y. Walking Ability Associated with Executive Dysfunction in Patients with Stroke: A Cross-Sectional Study. Brain Sci 2023; 13. [CrossRef]
- Uwa-Agbonikhena IF, Gryb VA, Gerasymchuk VR. ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN THE UPPER EXTREMITY FUNCTION AND COGNITION IN POST-STROKE PATIENTS. Wiad Lek 2021; 74: 1917–1920.
- Durcan S, Flavin E, Horgan F. Factors associated with community ambulation in chronic stroke. Disabil Rehabil 2016; 38: 245–249. [CrossRef]
- Baek CY, Yoon HS, Kim HD, et al. The effect of the degree of dual-task interference on gait, dual-task cost, cognitive ability, balance, and fall efficacy in people with stroke: A cross-sectional study. Medicine (Baltimore) 2021; 100: e26275. [CrossRef]
- Tanabe J, Amimoto K, Sakai K, et al. Effects and Adaptation of Visual-Motor Illusion Using Different Visual Stimuli on Improving Ankle Joint Paralysis of Stroke Survivors—A Randomized Crossover Controlled Trial. Brain Sciences 2022; 12.
- Teuschl Y, Ihle-Hansen H, Matz K, et al. Multidomain intervention for the prevention of cognitive decline after stroke - a pooled patient-level data analysis. Eur J Neurol 2018; 25: 1182–1188. [CrossRef]
- Godefroy O, Aarabi A, Dorchies F, et al. Functional architecture of executive processes: Evidence from verbal fluency and lesion mapping in stroke patients. Cortex 2023; 164: 129–143. [CrossRef]
- Rajtar-Zembaty A, Przewoźnik D, Bober-Płonka B, et al. Application of the Trail Making Test in the assessment of cognitive flexibility in patients with speech disorders after ischaemic cerebral stroke. Aktualnosci Neurologiczne 2015; 15: 11–17.
- Bonini MV, Radanovic M. Cognitive deficits in post-stroke aphasia. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2015; 73: 840–847. [CrossRef]
- Niessen E, Ant JM, Bode S, et al. Preserved performance monitoring and error detection in left hemisphere stroke. Neuroimage Clin 2020; 27: 102307. [CrossRef]
- Donnellan C, Al Banna M, Redha N, et al. Association Between Metacognition and Mood Symptoms Poststroke. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2016; 29: 212–220. [CrossRef]
- Swardfager W, MacIntosh BJ. Depression, Type 2 Diabetes, and Poststroke Cognitive Impairment. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2017; 31: 48–55. [CrossRef]
- Kawasaki M, Hoshiyama M. Apathy and depression during the recovery stage after stroke. International Journal of Therapy and Rehabilitation 2020; 27: 1–12.
- Bo W, Lei M, Tao S, et al. Effects of combined intervention of physical exercise and cognitive training on cognitive function in stroke survivors with vascular cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Clinical rehabilitation 2019; 33: 54–63.
- Haire CM, Vuong V, Tremblay L, et al. Effects of therapeutic instrumental music performance and motor imagery on chronic post-stroke cognition and affect: A randomized controlled trial. NeuroRehabilitation 2021; 48: 195–208. [CrossRef]
- Hara T, Abo M, Kakita K, et al. Does a combined intervention program of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation and intensive occupational therapy affect cognitive function in patients with post-stroke upper limb hemiparesis? Neural Regen Res 2016; 11: 1932–1939. [CrossRef]
- Huber SK, Held JPO, de Bruin ED, et al. Personalized Motor-Cognitive Exergame Training in Chronic Stroke Patients-A Feasibility Study. Front Aging Neurosci 2021; 13: 730801. [CrossRef]
- Kim T-., Bae S-., Kim K-. Effects of dual-task training with different intensity of aerobic exercise on cognitive function and neurotrophic factors in chronic stroke patients. Research Journal of Pharmacy and Technology 2019; 12: 693–698.
- Bonkhoff AK, Schirmer MD, Bretzner M, et al. Abnormal dynamic functional connectivity is linked to recovery after acute ischemic stroke. Hum Brain Mapp 2021; 42: 2278–2291. [CrossRef]
- Liu Y, Yin M, Luo J, et al. Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on the performance of the activities of daily living and attention function after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil 2020; 34: 1465–1473. [CrossRef]
- Nilsson MKL, Johansson B, Carlsson ML, et al. Effect of the monoaminergic stabiliser (-)-OSU6162 on mental fatigue following stroke or traumatic brain injury. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2020; 32: 303–312. [CrossRef]
- Rozental-Iluz C, Zeilig G, Weingarden H, et al. Improving executive function deficits by playing interactive video-games: Secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial for individuals with chronic stroke. European Journal of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine 2016; 52: 508–515.
- Tanikaga M, Muzuno J, Tanaka M, et al. Assessment of attention function recovery in patients after stroke using sequential desk-top tasks. International Journal of Therapy and Rehabilitation 2018; 25: 665–674.
- Torrisi M, Maresca G, De Cola MC, et al. Using telerehabilitation to improve cognitive function in post-stroke survivors: is this the time for the continuity of care? Int J Rehabil Res 2019; 42: 344–351. [CrossRef]
- Wang Y, Ye M, Tong Y, et al. Effects of robot-assisted therapy on upper limb and cognitive function in patients with stroke: study protocol of a randomized controlled study. Trials 2022; 23: 538. [CrossRef]
- Wolf TJ, Polatajko H, Baum C, et al. Combined Cognitive-Strategy and Task-Specific Training Affects Cognition and Upper-Extremity Function in Subacute Stroke: An Exploratory Randomized Controlled Trial. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy: Official Publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association 2016; 70: p1-7002290010.
- Lee H-S, Lim J-H, Jeon B-H, et al. Non-immersive Virtual Reality Rehabilitation Applied to a Task-oriented Approach for Stroke Patients: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2020; 38: 165–172. [CrossRef]
- Kim D, Ko J, Woo Y. Effects of dual task training with visual restriction and an unstable base on the balance and attention of stroke patients. Journal of Physical Therapy Science 2013; 25: 1579–1582.
- Gjellesvik TI, Becker F, Tjønna AE, et al. Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training After Stroke (The HIIT Stroke Study) on Physical and Cognitive Function: A Multicenter Randomized Controlled Trial. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2021; 102: 1683–1691. [CrossRef]
- Jung H, Jeong J-G, Cheong Y-S, et al. The Effectiveness of Computer-Assisted Cognitive Rehabilitation and the Degree of Recovery in Patients with Traumatic Brain Injury and Stroke. J Clin Med 2021; 10. [CrossRef]
- Liu Z, He Z, Yuan J, et al. Application of Immersive Virtual-Reality-Based Puzzle Games in Elderly Patients with Post-Stroke Cognitive Impairment: A Pilot Study. Brain Sci 2022; 13. [CrossRef]
- Pallesen H, Bjerk M, Pedersen AR, et al. The Effects of High-Intensity Aerobic Exercise on Cognitive Performance After Stroke: A Pilot Randomised Controlled Trial. J Cent Nerv Syst Dis 2019; 11: 1179573519843493. [CrossRef]
- Bian Y, Wang J-C, Sun F, et al. Assessment of cerebrovascular reserve impairment using the breath-holding index in patients with leukoaraiosis. Neural Regen Res 2019; 14: 1412–1418. [CrossRef]
- Jo SY, Hwang J-., Pyun S-. Relationship between cognitive function and dysphagia after stroke. Annals of Rehabilitation Medicine 2017; 41: 564–572.
- Bertolin M, Van Patten R, Greif T, et al. Predicting cognitive functioning, activities of daily living, and participation 6 months after mild to moderate stroke. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2018; 33: 562–576.
- Sörös P, Harnadek M, Blake T, et al. Executive dysfunction in patients with transient ischemic attack and minor stroke. J Neurol Sci 2015; 354: 17–20. [CrossRef]
- Pedersen A, Stanne TM, Redfors P, et al. Fibrinogen concentrations predict long-term cognitive outcome in young ischemic stroke patients. Res Pract Thromb Haemost 2018; 2: 339–346. [CrossRef]
- Lattanzi S, Coccia M, Pulcini A, et al. Endovascular treatment and cognitive outcome after anterior circulation ischemic stroke. Sci Rep 2020; 10: 18524. [CrossRef]
- Harahap HS, Akbar M, Bintang AK, et al. Methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) C677T polymorphism rather than homocysteine increase the risk of ischemic stroke-associated executive dysfunction. Bali Medical Journal 2022; 11: 443–450.
- Cappellari M, Forlivesi S, Zucchella C, et al. Factors influencing cognitive performance after 1-year treatment with direct oral anticoagulant in patients with atrial fibrillation and previous ischemic stroke: a pilot study. Journal of thrombosis and thrombolysis 2021; 51: 767–778.
- Levine DA, Galecki AT, Okullo D, et al. Association of Blood Pressure and Cognition after Stroke. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases 2020; 29.
- Shaheen HA, Daker LI, Abbass MM, et al. Post-stroke executive dysfunction and verbal fluency negatively correlated to IL8. Egyptian Journal of Neurology, Psychiatry and Neurosurgery 2019; 55.
- Rosenbaum Halevi D, Bursaw AW, Karamchandani RR, et al. Cognitive deficits in acute mild ischemic stroke and TIA and effects of rt-PA. Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology 2019; 6: 466–474.
- Altmann M, Thommessen B, Rønning OM, et al. Middle Cerebral Artery Pulsatility Index is Associated with Cognitive Impairment in Lacunar Stroke. J Neuroimaging 2016; 26: 431–435. [CrossRef]
- Zhang L, Liu X, Li T, et al. Fluoxetine May Enhance VEGF, BDNF and Cognition in Patients with Vascular Cognitive Impairment No Dementia: An Open-Label Randomized Clinical Study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2021; 17: 3819–3825. [CrossRef]
- Ling Y, De Guio F, Duering M, et al. Predictors and Clinical Impact of Incident Lacunes in Cerebral Autosomal Dominant Arteriopathy With Subcortical Infarcts and Leukoencephalopathy. Stroke 2017; 48: 283–289. [CrossRef]
- Kotlęga D, Peda B, Palma J, et al. Free Fatty Acids Are Associated with the Cognitive Functions in Stroke Survivors. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2021; 18. [CrossRef]
- Dacosta-Aguayo R, Graña M, Fernández-Andújar M, et al. Structural integrity of the contralesional hemisphere predicts cognitive impairment in ischemic stroke at three months. PLoS One 2014; 9: e86119. [CrossRef]
- Yang Y, Ye L, Lin R, et al. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging assessments of early cognitive impairment in patients after mild ischemic stroke and transient ischemic attack. J Integr Neurosci 2020; 19: 313–319. [CrossRef]
- Cipolotti L, Healy C, Chan E, et al. The impact of different aetiologies on the cognitive performance of frontal patients. Neuropsychologia 2015; 68: 21–30. [CrossRef]
- Veldsman M, Werden E, Egorova N, et al. Microstructural degeneration and cerebrovascular risk burden underlying executive dysfunction after stroke. Sci Rep 2020; 10: 17911. [CrossRef]
- Hagberg G, Ihle-Hansen H, Fure B, et al. No evidence for amyloid pathology as a key mediator of neurodegeneration post-stroke - a seven-year follow-up study. BMC Neurol 2020; 20: 174. [CrossRef]
- Min Y, Liu C, Zuo L, et al. The relationship between altered degree centrality and cognitive function in mild subcortical stroke: A resting-state fMRI study. Brain Res 2023; 1798: 148125. [CrossRef]
- Singh T, Fridriksson J, Perry CM, et al. A novel computational model to probe visual search deficits during motor performance. J Neurophysiol 2017; 117: 79–92. [CrossRef]
- Varjačić A, Mantini D, Levenstein J, et al. The role of left insula in executive set-switching: Lesion evidence from an acute stroke cohort. Cortex 2018; 107: 92–101. [CrossRef]
- Anziano M, Mouthon M, Thoeny H, et al. Mental flexibility depends on a largely distributed white matter network: Causal evidence from connectome-based lesion-symptom mapping. Cortex 2023; 165: 38–56. [CrossRef]
- Ling Y, De Guio F, Jouvent E, et al. Clinical correlates of longitudinal MRI changes in CADASIL. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2019; 39: 1299–1305. [CrossRef]
- Johnson T, Ridgeway G, Luchmee D, et al. Bimanual coordination during reach-to-grasp actions is sensitive to task goal with distinctions between left- and right-hemispheric stroke. Exp Brain Res 2022; 240: 2359–2373. [CrossRef]
- Jankowska AM, Klimkiewicz R, Kubsik A, et al. Location of the ischemic focus in rehabilitated stroke patients with impairment of executive functions. Adv Clin Exp Med 2017; 26: 767–776. [CrossRef]
- Shin MA, Park OT, Shin J-H. Anatomical Correlates of Neuropsychological Deficits Among Patients With the Cerebellar Stroke. Ann Rehabil Med 2017; 41: 924–934. [CrossRef]
- Ferris J, Greeley B, Yeganeh NM, et al. Exploring biomarkers of processing speed and executive function: The role of the anterior thalamic radiations. Neuroimage Clin 2022; 36: 103174. [CrossRef]
- Muir RT, Lam B, Honjo K, et al. Trail Making Test Elucidates Neural Substrates of Specific Poststroke Executive Dysfunctions. Stroke 2015; 46: 2755–2761. [CrossRef]
- Nishimura A, Sutoko S, Kiguchi M, et al. Projection of Damaged Visual and Language Regions on Low Trail Making Test Part-B Performance in Stroke Patients. Front Neurol 2022; 13: 853942. [CrossRef]
- Kopp B, Rösser N, Tabeling S, et al. Errors on the Trail Making Test Are Associated with Right Hemispheric Frontal Lobe Damage in Stroke Patients. Behav Neurol 2015; 2015: 309235. [CrossRef]
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).