1. Introduction
The ability to acquire a motor skill is expressed by a significant reduction in reaction time (RT) or error rate through practice [
1]. Reduced RT after training is related to neuroplasticity in different areas of the frontoparietal cortex (FPC) [
2,
3,
4]. Shorter RT in response to expected visual stimuli has been mainly associated with increased activation of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) [
5]. The PPC is strongly associated with sensorimotor integration for perception and action [
6]. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) has been activated for inhibition of unrelated stimuli to produce the best response to stimuli in difficult task demands [
7]. The primary motor cortex (M1) is a key motor area that is mainly activated in the process of acquiring a motor skill through the sustained learning of complex movements [
8,
9,
10,
11]. Although neuroimaging studies reveal important insights into brain areas involved in motor timing, further research is needed to determine the essential role of different areas of the brain in RT as one of the most important temporal variables during motor learning.
Non-invasive brain stimulation methods like transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) can be used to determine the specific role of different brain areas involved in the temporal processing of a certain task. TDCS is the application of a weak direct electrical current via the scalp to modulate cortical excitability in the human brain [
12]. The application of anodal tDCS (a-tDCS) over cortical target areas depolarizes the resting membrane potentials of the neurons, which may cause increased excitability [
13,
14]. This may lead to the formation of stronger and more effective synaptic connections between activated neurons during the learning process [
15,
16,
17]. Changes in physical performance following the application of a-tDCS over M1 have been reported in sequenced learning tasks such as a serial reaction time task (SRTT) [
18,
19,
20,
21,
22] and sequential visual isometric pinch task (SVIPT) [
23,
24,
25]. Even though there are some studies on the effects of a-tDCS on RT in SRTT, little is known about the effects of brain stimulation on SVIPT. SVIPT is a force control sequenced task with greater motor demands compared to SRTT, which is a key pressing task in which participants focus on cognitive functions rather than motor functions.
Both human and animal studies have demonstrated that the sequential knowledge acquired in one hand transfers to the other hand [
26,
27,
28]. Such a phenomenon is called “intermanual transfer” and it reflects how unilateral hand practice affects the performance of the other hand [
29,
30,
31,
32,
33]. Neuroimaging studies revealed that training with one hand led to excitatory or inhibitory activity in both hemispheres [
34,
35]. It is well-known that the corpus callosum is the main neural pathway that connects left and right cortical areas, including the prefrontal, motor, somatosensory, parietal, and occipital areas on either hemisphere [
36]. Indeed, the corpus callosum enables the transfer of motor skills from one hand to the other hand. For example, bilateral M1 activation has been reported when participants performed SRTT training with one hand [
37,
38]. The transfer learning is not only observed from one hand to another hand but also seen from one task to another task. Although some studies confirmed the presence of intermanual transfer of learning in SRTT, little is known about the effects of brain stimulation on the transfer of learning in SVIPT. Therefore, in the current study, we aimed to investigate whether a-tDCS over three stimulation sites (DLPFC, M1 or PPC) could differentially affect RT during SVIPT. We also aimed to explore whether these effects are transferred to the untrained hand.
4. Discussion
Our findings showed that participants who received the left M1 stimulation showed a significant increase in the RT ratio for some target forces compared to the sham group, while DLPFC and PPC stimulation did not modify RT within the SVIPT. The observed elongations in the ratio of RT after M1-a-tDCS were transferred into the untrained hand in sequence blocks of SVIPT but not random blocks. In the current study, we aimed to assess whether RT during SVIPT was differentially affected by stimulation of three different areas of the FPC. No improvement in RT was observed after a single session of a-tDCS concurrent with SVIPT training. Unlike DLPFC and PPC a-tDCS, which resulted in no effects on RTs, significant impairments were observed in this variable after M1 stimulation.
Contrary to our results, Waters-Metenier et al. (2014) observed an enhancement in both execution time and RT following a 4-day application of bihemespheric M1 a-tDCS with an intensity of 2 mA and an electrode size of 35 cm
2 during a piano-like key task [
42]. In contrast, Horvath et al. (2016) found no significant effects of anodal or cathodal M1 tDCS (2 mA or 1 mA with electrode size of 35 cm
2) on a simple motor reaction time task [
43]. They suggested that tDCS over M1, regardless of polarity, stimulation intensity, and electrode montage, might not have a positive effect on reaction time in a relatively lower-level motor behavioral task [
43]. In the current study, we applied a single session of a-tDCS with an intensity of 0.3 mA and a small electrode size of 3 cm
2 over M1 during a complex sequential motor task in which participants control their forces to reach different target forces appeared on the computer screen. We used a focal small electrode size of 3 cm
2 to selectively stimulate the M1 area, not nearby areas, such as the primary sensory area, premotor cortex, or supplementary motor area. It is probable in the current study, M1 representations of the specific muscles that are involved in the SVIPT task were not selectively stimulated by the 3-cm
2 tDCS. Nitsche et al. (2007) showed that reducing stimulation electrode size produces changes in M1 excitability only for the muscle representation covered by the small tDCS electrode, not for the muscle representation of the adjacent areas [
44]. In addition, focal stimulation of the M1 area with an electrode size of 3 cm
2 can decrease stimulation in nearby areas such as the premotor, or supplementary areas, which may influence M1 excitability. Boros et al. (2008) found that anodal stimulation of the premotor (0.1 mA, 3.5 cm
2, 13 min) modifies the intracortical excitability of the ipsilateral M1 [
45]. Elbert and co-workers observed that application of anodal tDCS (0.26 mA, 1.5 cm
2) at the vertex close to the supplementary motor area can improve RT in a tone-noise sequences task [
46]. Therefore, activity modulation of adjacent interconnected areas might increase the effects of M1 a-tDCS stimulation in order to improve RT processing. In our study, a small electrode size of 3 cm
2 over the left M1 area was used. The stimulation of nearby areas such as premotor or supplementary areas and their effects on RT during SVIPT should be explored in future studies.
In our study, we found no significant effects of left DLPFC stimulation on RTs within the SVIPT compared to sham stimulation. Marshall et al. (2005) showed that both anodal and cathodal stimulation (260 μA; 15 sec-on/15 sec-off; 8 mm diameter, 15 min) impaired reaction time processing in a working memory task [
47]. In contrast, enhancement effects in a stop-signal reaction time were observed following the right DLPFC a-tDCS stimulation (0.5 mA, 9 cm
2, 19 min) with extra cephalically montage on the contralateral deltoid [
48]. They found that a-tDCS over the right DLPFC can improve cognitive inhibition processes in a stop-signal reaction time by making fewer omission errors [
48]. Contrary to their results, we observed no positive effects on RTs within SVIPT in participants who received the left DLPFC a-tDCS compared to the sham group. These discrepancies can be explained by the different methodologies used in these studies. In our study, we stimulated left DLPFC with a contra-orbital montage in a constant, not intermittent, manner during a pinch-force sequential task. With regards to the positive effects observed in RT following the right DLPFC in a recognition reaction time task, it might be valuable in future studies to explore the effects of the right DLPFC tDCS on RTs within SVIPT.
In the current study, we also observed no significant effects of left PPC stimulation on RTs within SVIPT. However, the relevance of the left PPC as an anticipatory center for precise sensorimotor timing has been identified in the study by Krause et al. (2012). They showed that activity in the PPC is essential for precise execution of sensorimotor tasks, especially when quick adjustment of movements is required in response to external stimuli [
49]. In addition, Heinen et al. (2016) have shown that bilateral PPC stimulation, independent of electrode configuration, can enhance visual working memory precision more than unilateral PPC stimulation [
50]. They also found that cathodal but not anodal tDCS over the right PPC can improve general working memory precision [
50]. Although the SVIPT task used in the current study, was not similar to theirs, bilateral PPC stimulation or cathodal PPC stimulation within SVIPT should be explored in future studies.
In this study, we also aimed to assess the differential effects of brain stimulation over three different areas of the FPC on the transfer of learning within SVIP. No transfer learning was observed in the DLPFC and PPC stimulation groups. We also observed that the impairments in the ratio of the RTs in the M1 group were transferred to the left untrained hand. The present result is in line with a study by Keitel et al. (2018) showing that a-tDCS applied to the right M1 impairs implicit motor sequence learning of both hands [
51]. They applied a-tDCS (9 cm
2, 0.25 mA, 10 min) over right (ipsilateral) M1 during SRTT with the right trained hand [
51]. In the current study, we applied a-tDCS over left (contralateral) M1 during SVIPT training with the right hand. In both studies, participants were not aware of the underlying sequential pattern indicating implicit learning, which is mediated by a cortico-striatal-cerebellar network [
52]. The observed negative interannual transfer in M1 group showed that there is an interaction between bilateral M1, which support the hypothesis of interhemispheric rivalry. Therefore, the inhibitory effect of left M1 a-tDCS on implicit motor sequence learning was seen in both hands. Regarding the fact that the a-tDCS technique used in this study showed no significant improvement on RTs, further research is needed to investigate the impact of different stimulus conditions of tDCS in terms of electrode montage, current intensity, or electrode size on RT in SVIPT.