School bullying is one of the major social problems affecting children and adolescents in all parts of the world [
1]. This subtype of aggressive, intentional behavior carried out by a group or an individual repeatedly and over time against a victim who cannot easily defend him or herself [
2] has been commonly recognized as a widespread expression of violence in the peer context [
3]. A recent report published by the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization [
4] revealed that more than 30% of the world’s students have been victims of bullying, bringing deleterious, immediate, mid- and long-term consequences both on educational and mental health domains [
3]. From a developmental perspective, such behaviors peak during middle school years (i.e., 12–15 years), and tends to decrease by the end of high school [
5] assuming different forms across age groups [
3]. During adolescence school bullying is viewed as a deliberate strategy used to re-establish social status and dominance when youth enter a new peer group [
6].
Taking into account this framework [
7], this study builds on a previous study [
10], now including in the research design an additional cohort of adolescents, new measures and a longer period of assessment. More specifically, we investigated, over four years, the longitudinal and simultaneous pathways linking youth’s violence exposure across different contexts, specifically within the more proximal and distal family and neighborhood/community microsystems, respectively, and across multiple forms (through direct victimization and witnessing), individual pro-violence moral cognitions (here represented by self-serving cognitive distortions—hereinafter CDs) considered in Gibbs’ model [
11], with school bullying perpetration.
In this regard, while there is a strong evidence that both exposure to domestic (e.g., [
12,
13,
14,
15]) as well as community violence (e.g., [
16,
17,
18]) and self-serving CDs [
19,
20,
21] are associated with externalizing behaviors (i.e., aggression, conduct problems, delinquency), only a few studies, most of them cross-sectionals, have systematically examined how bullying behavior (as a specific subtype of aggression) is influenced by experiences of domestic (e.g., [
22,
23,
24,
25,
26]) as well as community violence exposure (e.g., [
10,
27,
28,
29,
30,
31]). Moreover, the associations between violence exposure and moral CDs [
10], according to the Gibbs’ model, as well as how such pro-violence moral cognitions are related to the involvement in bullying behavior [
32] have been poorly investigated.
1.1. Exposure to Violence within the Family and Community as a Social-Environmental Risk Factor for School Bullying Perpetration
According to the ecological model applied to bullying [
41,
42,
43], understanding the factors that promote bullying behaviors requires a close examination of experiences youth daily live in environments they inhabit outside of school, as the more immediate or proximal (i.e., family), and distal (i.e., neighborhood/community) microsystems, with whom youths have direct interactions [
1].
During adolescence, violence exposure is one of the experiences that reach a dramatic peak [
44,
45,
46]. This exposure can take place across multiple, sometimes overlapping, contexts or settings (i.e., the concept of “poly-victimization”; [
47]) that refer to the multiple ecological levels of the microsystem. Also, it can assume multiple forms, including direct victimization and witnessing without being directly involved.
Some studies suggested a sort of equifinality of violence exposure across different contexts, which seems to eventuate in the same outcomes, whereas other studies suggested a differential effect of violence exposure on developmental outcomes in function of the involved microsystem. This may be due to the differential proximity to the child within the hierarchically ordered social ecology [
48]. More specifically, some studies [
35,
49,
50] suggested a stronger impact of violence exposure in the family than in other settings, revealing that domestic violence exposure was a more robust predictor of adjustment problems (e.g., anxiety, depression, aggression, and antisocial conducts) than exposure to community violence. However, other studies [
12,
16,
51] found that community violence has a unique role in predicting several externalizing behaviors accounting for other sources of violence exposure.
Another key issue concerns the differential effects of witnessing or victimization experiences that appear to affect child development differently [
52]. Overall, witnessing violence has been shown to be linked to externalizing behaviors (e.g., [
53]) mainly through a social learning process that leads to the acquisition of deviant social information patterns, which, in turn, increases the likelihood of aggressive behavior (e.g., [
30,
51,
54,
55]). Conversely, direct victimization appeared to be more strongly associated with the development of internalizing symptoms (e.g., [
56]) through impairments in emotional self-regulation that compromise the more general ability of individual’s adaptive behavior [
30]. Other studies have found no significant differences in the specific outcomes associated with witnessing and victimization experiences [
57].
Referring to school bullying, although most of the research highlighted that violence exposure within the family and community places youths at great risk for negative peer experiences such as bullying perpetration, the findings vary considerably across studies.
As regards the family context, a previous review by Nocentini et al. [
24] provided empirical evidence about the role of contextual (i.e., parental mental health and domestic violence) and relational family processes (i.e., child abuse and neglect, maladaptive parenting, communication, parental involvement and support) in affecting the risk of bullying others or being victimized. However, although the authors found that domestic violence exposure has a more consistent and stable role as predictor of bullying across studies, to date, the studies specifically focused on the relation between violence exposure within the family and bullying perpetration are still rather scarce.
Based on a review by Voisin and Hong [
26], some studies provide empirical evidence that youths who are exposed to domestic violence are more likely to perpetrate school bullying, as well as become victims of bullying (e.g., [
22,
58,
59,
60,
61,
62,
63]). Otherwise, other studies (e.g., [
23]) have found an association of domestic violence exposure with other types of problematic behaviors (i.e., externalizing behavior or physical aggression and internalizing behaviors), rather than relational bullying behaviors (e.g., social exclusion, spreading rumors). More specifically, Baldry [
22] documented that, among a large sample of Italian adolescents and preadolescents, about 1/6 had witnessed domestic violence, and bullies were more likely than not to have witnessed violence in the home, over and above age, gender, and child abuse. Other cross-sectional studies reported similar findings revealing that domestic violence witnessing represents a risk factor for bullying peers [
59,
60,
61,
62,
63]. Otherwise, in a cross-sectional and retrospective study, Sanders and Jenkins [
25] found that domestic violence witnessing significantly predicted the frequency of bullying victimization and presence of relational bullying victimization but not of bullying perpetration.
Conversely, among longitudinal studies, Bowes et al. [
58], using a nationally representative community-based sample of children and distinguishing the differential effects of direct victimization (i.e., child maltreatment) and exposure to domestic violence, found that over and above other socio-environmental factors and children’s behavior problems, youths who were victims of parents’ maltreatment were at increased risk for bullying victimization, whereas those who witnessed domestic violence were at increased risk for bullying perpetration.
Finally, a longitudinal study [
23] using a community-based sample of children found that witnessing domestic violence was related to several problematic behaviors (i.e., externalizing behavior or physical aggression and internalizing behaviors) but not to child-reported relational bullying behaviors or victimization by peers.
To date, relatively few studies [
64,
65] have investigated how school bullying is influenced by experiences in environments outside of more immediate or proximal settings (e.g., school and family), and by the perceptions of the neighborhood/community where youths live such as community violence exposure [
10]. In this regard, some studies, without distinguishing the differential effects of community violence witnessing and victimization, found a significant association between violence exposure within the community and bullying perpetration [
27,
31] over and above other socio-environmental factors, such as poverty, inequality, and political violence [
28]. A seminal cross-sectional study by Schwartz and Proctor [
30] found that children who had been witnesses to or victims of community violence were more likely to bully their classmates.
Furthermore, using a latent transition mixture analysis, Davis et al. [
29] reported that the largest proportion (25%) of youths who experienced heightened levels of community violence as witnesses were more likely to be perpetrators of school bullying. Nonetheless, Andershed et al. [
66] found that bullying others in school was related to a heightened risk of being violently victimized when out on the streets among both boys and girls. In a longitudinal study [
10], it was found that being exposed to community violence as a witness but not as a victim promoted the perpetration of bullying over time.
From a social-ecological systems perspective and consistent with a transactional developmental model of conduct problems [
67], both family and community violence exposure and their effects on child development can be understood as resulting from reciprocal interactions between the individual and environment that are continuously influenced by experiences and conditions across multiple interrelated systems over time [
68]. In this regard, there is some empirical support for causal, bidirectional influences between violence exposure and externalizing problems such that the more youths are exposed to violent contexts, the more likely they learn pro-violence behavioral models and,
vice-versa, the more youths engage in aggressive and delinquent behaviors, the more likely they put themselves in high-risk situations in which they could become witnesses or victims of violence [
17,
18].
However, the causal nature of the relationships between exposure to violence, both in the family and neighborhood/community, and bullying remains unclear [
43], given the limited number of studies that have examined the mechanisms through which violence exposure could affect involvement in bullying behavior.
1.2. Self-Serving Cognitive Distortions as Individual Social-Cognitive Risk Factors for School Bullying Perpetration
Consistent with the social-cognitive approach (e.g., [
69,
70]), according to which people act upon their interpretation of social events, previous research claimed that moral cognitions represent a key factor for motivating moral or immoral acts such as bullying behaviors [
20,
71]. In terms of moral cognitive processes, the self-serving CDs defined as “inaccurate or biased ways of attending to or conferring meaning upon experiences” [
72] (p. 1) represent one of the common limitations characterizing antisocial youth’s social cognitions [
11,
73,
74].
According to Gibbs and colleagues’ “three
Ds” formulation [
75], the self-serving CDs are conceived as potentially preceding antisocial action, i.e., the
primary CDs, as well as following behavior, i.e., the
secondary CDs. More specifically, while the
primary distortions (the category
Self-centered) reflect more immature moral judgment stages [
76] and serve as main motivators or “pretexts” of aggressive behaviors, the
secondary distortions (the categories
Blaming others,
Minimizing/Mislabeling, and
Assuming the worst) support the self-centered attitudes [
11] and have been characterized as pre- or post-transgression rationalizations or “excuses” for facilitating aggressive behaviors.
Despite an increasing number of studies [
19,
20,
21] found a link between self-serving CDs and externalizing behaviors, only a few studies [
10,
32] examined the association between CDs and bullying at school. The study by Owens et al. [
32] carried out with Australian adolescents found that bullies and bully-victims showed a higher tendency than victims and not-involved persons in assuming the worst, exhibiting minimizing-mislabeling and self-centered CDs, whereas only bullies were higher in blaming others. Similarly, Dragone et al. [
10] showed that the development of CDs promoted the perpetration of bullying over time.
However, a transactional developmental model [
67] would be better equipped to explain the emergence of chronic antisocial behavior over time. This model suggests the possibility of a multidirectional causality between individual cognitions and behaviors, providing support to Gibbs’ conceptualization of secondary CDs as a form of post-rationalizations or “excuses” serving to emotionally and cognitively overcome dissonance between individual moral standards and behavioral transgressions and neutralize potential feelings of guilt or empathy towards the victim, thus avoid damage to one’s self-image when engaging in antisocial conducts [
77,
78].
Based on these considerations, disentangling the temporal order is needed to provide key evidence on the causal direction of the link between moral cognitions and aggressive behaviors [
79]. However, the experimental or longitudinal designs suited to test assumptions of temporal order and to clarify a possible causal link between aggression and moral cognitions [
80] (p. 45) are still rather scarce. Specifically, Aquilar et al. [
81] found reciprocal influences over time among values, moral judgment- considered similarly to CDs as a moral motivator [
82] of externalizing behaviors-, and antisocial behaviors. Moreover, Ribeaud and Eisner [
79] conducted two-period path models to test the relationship between moral neutralization and aggressive behavior in early adolescence, and found that moral neutralization might be envisaged as facilitating aggressive behavior by providing
ex ante justifications. In contrast, aggressive behavior would, in turn, induce
ex post legitimizations that allow a smooth integration of norm-breaking behavior into an apparently intact moral self-concept.
1.3. Examining how the Cognitive Desensitization Process Could Link Exposure to Violent Contexts, Self-Serving Cognitive Distortions, and School Bullying Perpetration
According to social learning theory [
83], children chronically exposed to violence in their daily life environment may learn, via an observational process, that violence itself is a socially acceptable means for conflict resolution [
84]. Such process resulting from growing up in violent contexts and facilitating more approving violent beliefs, more positive moral evaluations of aggressive acts, and more justification for inappropriate behavior inconsistent with social and individual’s moral norms has been defined by Huesmann and Kirwil [
34] “cognitive desensitization to violence”. Consistently, Ng-Mak and colleagues [
36] formulated a “pathologic adaptation” model according to which repeated exposure to high levels of violence in inner-city urban neighborhoods leads to cognitions that normalize violence through mechanisms of neutralization of moral standards which, in turn, facilitate the engagement in future episodes of violence, thus, perpetuating the cycle of violence.
The depiction of moral cognitive processes as mediators of life experiences and as proximal mechanisms for externalizing behaviors is consistent with the biopsychosocial perspective on the development of adolescent conduct problems [
67]. According to this perspective, it is assumed that as a function of the aggressogenic life experiences, such as the repeated experience of being a witness or a victim of violence, children develop idiosyncratic social knowledge about their world and social-information processing patterns that justify the appropriateness of behaving aggressively in problematic social situations.
However, to date, relatively few studies have investigated whether the moral cognitive processes could mediate the effect of early life experiences, such as violence exposure within the family and community, on later behavioral problems, such as bullying, also disentangling the potential differential effects due to victimization and witnessing violence (e.g., [
30]). Specifically, the transmission of violence among adolescents who directly suffer maltreatment or are indirectly exposed to domestic violence could be cognitively mediated [
85]. In this regard, some studies (e.g., [
86,
87,
88]) found that while experiencing witnessing was more likely associated with the schemas of justification of violence which, in turn, lead to aggressive behavior, direct victimization was linked with less aggressiveness and more depression, through the schema of mistrust [
87] as well as with later bullying victimization in the school through the development of maladaptive schemas of rejection [
89].
Nevertheless, it was found that also direct victimization experiences within the family, such as childhood maltreatment and physical abuse, might contribute to the development of cognitive structures or schemas in the victims, which, in turn, would influence their subsequent behavior. Such schemas assume the form of normative beliefs about the social appropriateness of aggression [
90], including the idea that the use of the aggression is justified (e.g., because the other deserves it) and leads to positive outcomes for the individual (e.g., because it serves to obtain respect from others). Furthermore, when exposed to childhood maltreatment and physical abuse, these socio-cognitive biases could also take the form of hostile attributional bias when faced with the ambiguous intentions of others in social situations [
91] or of acceptance of violence as normative in adult relationships [
92].
Also, with regard to the link between violence exposure within the neighborhood/community and social-cognitive processes, research findings have been mixed. For instance, Wilkinson and Carr [
93] using qualitative data from male violent offenders, found that individuals respond to exposure to violence, without distinguishing between violence witnessing and victimization, in many ways, some of which would be consistent with traditional concepts of moral disengagement. In the same direction were the results of Hyde et al. [
40], who found a positive association between neighborhood impoverishment and moral disengagement.
Nonetheless, several studies have found significant associations between community violence and acceptance of violent cognitions or bias of social-information processing (see, for example, [
37,
38]), whereas Bacchini et al. [
94] showed that higher levels of exposure to community violence as a witness, along with the perception of higher levels of deviancy among peers, reduced the strength of moral criteria for judging moral violations. Among the very few longitudinal studies, Esposito et al. [
95] showed that a high frequency of exposure to community violence was a significant risk factor for being in the class with higher and tendentially stable CDs, relative to the moderate and decreasing class. Similarly, Dragone et al. [
10] found a longitudinal relationship between community violence witnessing and the development of CDs.
Other studies [
30] have shown distinct mediational pathways linking each form of violence exposure to social difficulties with peers suggesting that the impact of victimization on aggressive behavior took place through impairments in emotion regulation, whereas witnessing influences aggressive behavior through social-cognitive biases about aggression when involved in processing social situations. Consistently, Esposito et al. [
96] found that a moderate exposure to community violence, as a witness and as a victim, as well as only as a witness, were linked to increased bullying perpetration through greater moral disengagement. Interestingly, high levels of exposure as both a witness and a victim did not show significant associations with either moral disengagement or bullying perpetration.
Taking this evidence as a starting point, further longitudinal research is needed to clarify whether both experiences of violence exposure, as a victim and/or as a witness, are associated with constructs of moral cognitions such as CDs, which, in turn, could promote the involvement in aggressive behaviors as school bullying perpetration.
1.4. The Current Study
The main aim of this study was to investigate the mediating role of self-serving CDs in the relationship between violence exposure within the family and neighborhood/community, as a victim and as a witness, and school bullying perpetration over time. A four-wave cross-lagged panel mediation design was used, which allowed us to control for baseline values of all variables in each wave and to examine the transactional nature and likely causal direction of the pathways linking exposure to domestic and community violence, CDs, and school bullying perpetration. More specifically, we expected that: (i) being exposed to violence within the family and neighborhood/community increased the likelihood that adolescents would develop self-serving CDs and perpetrate bullying; and (ii) making use of CDs would promote engagement in future episodes of bullying perpetration. In addition, consistent with the transactional developmental model, we expected that the associations between both domestic and community violence exposure with CDs and bullying perpetration, as well as between CDs and bullying perpetration, were reciprocal over time.
As regards the specific hypotheses we made about the differential effects of direct victimization and witnessing, while for domestic violence exposure, no a priori hypotheses were formulated due to the limited and conflicting prior literature, with regard to community violence exposure, we hypothesized significant associations between violence witnessing and both CDs and bullying perpetration, whereas no a priori hypotheses were formulated for violence victimization, due to the limited prior literature.
Given that gender-based differences have been observed in violence exposure and its developmental effects [
22,
97] as well as in CDs [
32] and bullying behavior [
98], adolescent gender was included as control variable. Adolescents’ social desirability was also used as a control variable, given its potential confounding effect on all study variables [
99].