1. Introduction
Body odors are natural components of the environment in a person’s daily life. The past three decades have generated a suite of discoveries supporting on the one hand the idea that human olfaction has been underestimated during practically whole time of its investigation [
1] and, on the other hand, concerning the potential involvement of body odors in various aspects of interpersonal communication, such as familial bonding, mate selection and emotional state in human [
2,
3]. Besides, it has been shown that body odor may be processed differently in the brain than other odors [
4]. The mechanisms underlying such a communication are poorly documented to date.
Human body odors may change hormonal status of women in general, or regulate women’s menstrual cycles in particular [
5,
6,
7]. Apocrine glands in the axillae are currently considered to be the most likely source of human chemical signals, or at least axillary secretions contribute most to the appearance of the so-called “body odor” [
8]. Preti and colleagues have shown that extracts of male axillary secretions may directly affect LH-pulsing and mood of women in the follicular phase of menstrual cycle [
6]. It has been shown that extracts of male axillary secretions affect the length of the menstrual cycle in women with a history of irregular cycle length [
5,
9]. In premenopausal women effects of application of extracts of male axillary secretions are even more profound than in younger women. It was shown that idiopathic repeated pregnancy loss was associated with altered olfactory responses to body odor [
7]. At the same time, hormone changes across the women’s menstrual cycle may lead to changes in perception of chemical signals or other odors and hormonal responses to these cues [
10,
11].
The modulatory effect of male axillary secretions or their components has been addressed in several studies [
6,
12,
13]. The aim of the present study was to determine the role of menstrual cycle phase on the effects of the male axillary secretions in women.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
29 healthy heterosexual women with known medical history, not taking hormonal pills were enrolled in the protocol. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Women were divided into two groups: 18-45 y.o. later referred to as “reproductive age women” with mean age of 24.7 ± 5.9 years and with regular menstrual cycle 26-30 days long, and over 45 y.o., later referred to as “premenopausal women” with mean age of 46.4 ± 3.5 years. For each woman, we had information on the length and regularity of her menstrual cycles for 6 months preceding the experiment. Length of menstrual cycles was self-recorded by participants of the study; the beginning of menses was considered the first day of the menstrual cycle. Women in the follicular phase were tested on the 9th or 10th day of the menstrual cycle. Women in the luteal phase were tested 5.3 ± 3.3 days prior menses, which start was also documented.
2.2. Extract of Male Axillary Secretions (EMAS)
Axillary secretions were collected during four hours on sterile cotton pads (0.2 × 11 × 9 cm) from 6 healthy heterosexual men of 32.2 ± 4.6 years old using the standard technique [
14,
15]. For seven days prior to collection all odor donors refrained from using deodorant/antiperspirants and washed/bathed using only plain soap without fragrance. After collecting the secretions, the pads from all the donors were combined and soaked with 70% ethanol (15 ml per pad) (ethanol 96.3% purity, serial # 386710, Ferejn, Russia) for 1 hour; about 85% of ethanol extract was recovered. Finally, a masking fragrance (0.005% solution of 2-phenylethanol, CAS Number: 60-12-8, Sigma-Aldrich
® Cat. # 77861, ≥99.0% purity) was added to the ethanol extract [
15]. Clear 70% ethanol solution scented with the same fragrance was used as an odor-matched control sample. All solutions were stored at -40 °C in aliquots and brought to room temperature before testing. Female participants were unable to distinguish between scented ethanol extract of male axillary secretions (EMAS) and control solution by smell. Male donors had no contact and were not familiar with the female participants.
2.3. Evaluation of Emotional State of Test Subjects
We evaluated the current emotional state of test subjects using two questionnaires: Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [
16] adapted for the Russian population [
17] and Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) [
18,
19]. The questionnaires were printed to fill in by hand.
PANAS questionnaire consists of a list of 20 adjectives that describe different feelings and emotions. Subjects were asked to note on a five-point Likert scale how they feel with regard to each statement - from “ very slightly or not at all” (score 1) to “very much” (score 5). To evaluate total positive or negative affect we summed up the scores of 10 positive (interested, excited, strong, enthusiastic, proud, alert, inspired, determined, attentive, active) or, respectively, 10 negative (distressed, upset, guilty, scared, hostile, irritable, ashamed, nervous, jittery, afraid) adjectives.
VAS was presented for 8 adjectives describing the mood. They were: attuned to communication, sincere, relaxed, sad, focused, experiencing sexual arousal, energetic, nervous. Participants were instructed to place a mark on a 100 mm scale with anchors “not at all” and “extremely” at the ends in accordance with their current mood.
2.4. Procedure
A within-subject, double-blind, repeated-measures design was used in such a way that each participant underwent testing both with control solution and EMAS. EMAS or control solution (0.5 ml) were applied to a cotton pad and placed within 15 cm of the woman’s nose once every 2 hours either in the follicular or luteal phase of her menstrual cycle. Procedure continued for 8 hours (from 10 a.m. to 6 p.m.). Neither experimenter, nor participant were informed about the order in which solutions were applied. None of the women correctly guessed the true nature of the stimuli that were being delivered to their nasal region. Saliva samples were taken every 10 min for reproductive age women (total of 48 samples per woman) using Sali-Caps® sets (IBL, Germany).
Women completed VAS and PANAS four times during the experiment: 15 minutes after the first application of the control solution at 10 a.m., 15 minutes before and after first application of EMAS at 2 p.m., at the end of the experiment.
Changes in salivary cortisol concentration after EMAS application in a group of reproductive age women were evaluated relative to a group of premenopausal women. For all groups testing continued for 4 hours from 12 a.m. to 4 p.m. and saliva samples were taken every 20 minutes (total of 12 samples per woman). EMAS or control solution (0.5 ml) were applied to a cotton pad and placed within 15 cm of the woman’s nose once every two hours.
2.5. Hormone’s Measurements and Further Calculations
Saliva samples were stored at -32 °C, thawed at room temperature and centrifuged before testing. Concentration of luteinizing hormone (LH) was assessed using EIA technique (abx576540, Abbexa, UK) in all saliva samples. After completion of the immunoassay, the absorbance in the wells were measured at λ = 450 nm with SpectraMax 340PC 384 spectrophotometer (Molecular Devices, United States). Data analysis was performed with software Spectra-MaxSoftware.
LH baseline was calculated individually for each woman and separately for control and experimental periods [
20,
21,
22]. To calculate the basal LH level, the mean value of the LH concentration for each woman was added to 1.5 of standard deviation (SD). Values that exceeded the result were pro tempore excluded, and the value of the mean + 1.5 SD was calculated for remaining values again. This procedure was repeated until none of the remaining values in the sample size exceeded the mean +1.5 SD. The received remaining value was considered as the basal hormone concentration for a particular woman.
We counted the number of LH peaks (values exceeding the basal level) and determined their mean amplitude separately during control and experimental periods. We also took into account individual differences in the form of LH peak for each woman and the fact that the number of salivary LH peaks cannot exceed previously reported number of LH pulses in blood (on average one peak per hour in follicular phase and one peak per 2-4 hours in luteal phase) [
6,
23] (
Figure 1).
Cortisol was monitored using an EIA technique (EIA Can-C-290, DBC, Canada). As cortisol secretion is cyclical in nature, we calculated the average value for two hours during control period and for two hours during EMAS application to catch the peaks that occur about every 90 min [
24], we used samples that were collected from 12:20 p.m. to 3:40 p.m. from reproductive age women and premenopausal women. After completion of the immunoassay, the absorbance in the wells were obtained at λ = 450 nm with SpectraMax 340PC 384 spectrophotometer (Molecular Devices, United States). Data analysis was performed with software SpectraMaxSoftware.
2.6. Statistical Analyses
Data analysis was carried out with STATISTICA 8.0 software (StatSoft, Inc.) and with R (version 4.3.0).
All data are reported as mean ± SD. All data were checked to fit in normal distribution (Shapiro-Wilk test, p > 0.05) and to be homoscedastic (Levene’s test, p > 0.05) before performing Repeated Measures ANOVA or One-way ANOVA, otherwise we used data transformation prior analysis, non-parametric Wilcoxon Signed-Ranks test or permutation test with Monte-Carlo approximation in R (package lmPerm (
https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmPerm/lmPerm.pdf) function aovp(); and package rcompanion (
https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rcompanion/rcompanion.pdf) function pairwisePermutationTest()). The alpha level for all analysis was set a priori to 0.05.
Analysis of variance for repeated measurements with the within-subject factor treatment (control solution vs. EMAS) and the between-subject factor phase (follicular vs. luteal) was used for comparison of the number of LH peaks.
LH amplitude data did not fit in normal distribution, that is why we counted percent of change between mean amplitude during control period and mean amplitude after beginning of EMAS application for each test subject, then we performed a One-way ANOVA followed by Fisher HSD test.
Cortisol data also did not fit in normal distribution, that is why we counted percent of change between mean cortisol level during control period and mean cortisol level during EMAS application for each test subject, then we performed permutation test with Monte-Carlo approximation in R.
4. Discussion
Presentation of extracts of male axillary secretions (EMAS) to healthy heterosexual women could cause a number of different responses as body odor may provide gender specific information even if it doesn’t register on a conscious level [
4]. These responses may include changes in hormone secretion in both HPA and HPG axes as well as mood changes and other adaptive reactions in accordance with the context of the signal [
3]. With this in mind we consider the following results of our study as its key findings: 1). The menstrual cycle phase of the recipient determines the hormonal EMAS effects: increase in number of salivary LH peaks and average LH peak amplitude for application in follicular phase and vice versa a decrease in LH amplitude for application in luteal phase. 2). EMAS application increases salivary cortisol level in reproductive age women relative to premenopausal women 3). EMAS may affect the emotional state of women, with changes of positive and negative affect by the end of testing as well as parameters “experiencing sexual arousal” and “focused” obtained with VAS.
To the best of our knowledge this is the first report of the influence of body odor on LH secretion during the luteal phase of the recipient. We observed a considerable decrease in the average amplitude of LH peaks after EMAS application during the luteal phase, while the number of salivary LH peaks remained the same. Our findings of increase in the number of LH peaks in saliva in follicular phase are in good agreement with findings of Preti et al. in blood [
6] - extracts of male axillary secretions advance the onset of the next LH peak in women. Moreover, for the first time we found that the average amplitude of salivary LH peaks also increased. Apparently, the observed increase in LH secretion during follicular phase could be regarded as a possible mechanism for shortening of abnormally long menstrual cycles, shown in our previous research [
9], since the increase in pulsation of LH may contribute to the acceleration of ovulation. Possible acceleration of the ovulation onset due to EMAS application in women with normal cycle length suggests a mechanism of cycle lengthening during the luteal phase. Otherwise, it is difficult to explain the constant cycle length [
9]. In the present study we observed a decrease in the average amplitude of LH peaks (a decrease in hormone secretion) after EMAS application during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. We assume that the basis for these results lies in a completely different role that LH performs in the luteal phase compared to the follicular one [
23]. At the same time, the onset of a new menstrual cycle is manifested by primarily sharp increase of LH pulse frequency, not change in their amplitude [
25].
We have designed our experiments with the assumption that the frequency of LH peaks was affected by the phase of the menstrual cycle of test subjects. In particular, during a normal menstrual cycle one can expect a single LH pulse in blood every 60–90 min during the follicular phase and one pulse every 3–4 h during luteal phase [
23]. Our current data obtained for saliva have a similar pattern. Besides, we obtained the values of LH concentrations in saliva similar to those obtained in other works [
26,
27].
Since the profile of the LH concentration curve in blood has a specific pulse shape with a pronounced maximum, the software used for detection of LH pulses in blood, for example MATLAB module Cluster Analysis, developed by Urban as described in [
28] is based on capturing the sudden change in sign of the derivative of the curve from positive to negative. Meanwhile in saliva, LH peaks have smoother shape and as a result, they are not detected by the specific software described above as pulses. Due to the lack of a distinct pulsatile pattern, we have used the baseline method for peak detection of LH [
20,
21,
22].
One can assume contribution of diurnal changes of LH secretions on the observed effects. Several works are devoted to diurnal changes of LH secretion [
29,
30,
31,
32], but their results are inconsistent. One of them showed absence of circadian rhythms of gonadotropin secretion in women in the early follicular phase [
30]. Another showed presence of some diurnal changes of LH secretion only in the follicular phase [
32]. One more study didn’t report menstrual cycle phase, and no circadian rhythms for LH secretion was found [
31]. So it is difficult to conclude whether diurnal changes of LH secretion had an impact in our research. At the same time, according to Rossmanith [
29] days of the cycle in which we have chosen to perform our experiment correspond to late follicular and mid-luteal phases of test subjects. For the late follicular phase there was no increase of LH pulsation in blood during 2 p.m. – 6 p.m. when we observed an increase in LH amplitude. For the luteal phase they observed an increase of LH pulsation from 1 p.m. till 9 p.m., whereas we found a decrease of LH amplitude for the period from 2 p.m. to 6 p.m.
In this work we also evaluated the effect of EMAS application on secretion of the main human glucocorticoid – cortisol in women of different age groups. We found that salivary cortisol level in reproductive age women did not depend on the phase of their menstrual cycle. However, comparing reproductive age and premenopausal women, we found that relative change of salivary cortisol after EMAS application depended on age group of test subjects. There are diurnal changes in salivary cortisol with the peak during the first hour after awakening - so called cortisol awakening response (CAR), which stay in consistent ranges for a single woman during the whole menstrual cycle [
33]. During the experiment (12.00-16.00) for every 2 hours salivary cortisol doesn’t change significantly or have a tendency to decline [
24,
34]. On the contrary, EMAS caused an increase in the level of salivary cortisol in reproductive age women by an average of 15.1%. At the same time cortisol concentrations were within the physiological range both in the experiment and the control and lower than CAR [
33], indicating a possible stimulating effect of EMAS rather than a stressful one.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report on the effect of intact male axillary secretions on the level of cortisol in women. However, for a single component of the axillary secretions – androstadienone, it was shown that its sniffing led to an increase in the level of salivary cortisol in reproductive age women with mean age of 20-23 y.o. [
35].
In premenopausal women, application of EMAS, on the contrary, lowered the level of cortisol in saliva compared to the control period by 25%, which corresponded to diurnal changes of human cortisol secretion [
24]. Such a different reaction is probably associated with a different hormonal status of women of different age groups.
We evaluated the influence of EMAS on the emotional state of women, depending on the phase of the menstrual cycle using Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) and Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) [
17,
18]. We did not observe any short-term changes in positive or negative affect, but for the entire time (4 hours) of EMAS application we observed a decrease in positive affect for women in both phases as well as an increase of negative affect in women only in the follicular phase. We suppose that such result may be not only due to EMAS application, but rather represents general fatigue of test subjects by the end of 8 hours testing. Meanwhile, VAS showed significant short-term increase of a parameter “experiencing sexual arousal” during EMAS application in women in the follicular phase of menstrual cycle and a tendency to increase in the luteal phase of menstrual cycle. At the first glance, this is not very consistent with an increase in salivary cortisol level in the same subjects, since sexual arousal has been shown to be associated with a decrease in cortisol levels [
36]. However, we evaluated a subjective sensation, not the physiological response, and as it was mentioned before, the increase was significant, but lower than the average value of CAR [
33]. Stimulating effect of an increase in salivary cortisol level is also consistent with an increase of the parameter “focused” in the group of reproductive age women in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle.
Various modulatory effects were shown for male axillary secretions or their components, usually using Visual Analogue Scales (VAS). For example, male axillary secretions made women more relaxed and less tense [
6], while low concentrations of androstadienone made subjects more focused [
37]. Activation of the cortical area of the brain, responsible for attention, was also shown when sniffing androstadienone at a sufficiently high concentration exceeding naturally occurring in the axillae [
38]. Male axillary secretions can affect the mood of women through the modulation of the serotonin (5-HT) system, namely, by a rapid increase in the affinity of the 5-HT transporter without quantitative changes [
39]. Although women in this study were tested only during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle, besides, extract of male axillary secretions was applied directly to the area under the nose, in this case extract can be absorbed through the skin and enter the bloodstream.
It is possible that more than one compound in axillary secretions can induce endocrine and modulatory changes in human recipients, as far as chemical signals in mammals can be a multicomponent combination of molecules exhibiting a synergistic effect in contrast to an action of separate one [
40]. The candidates for the role of chemical signal from axillary secretions could be among: organic unsaturated fatty acids 3-methyl-2-hexenoic acid (3M2H) and 3-hydroxy-3-methyl-hexanoic acid (HMHA), sulfur-containing 3-sulfanyl-3-methyl-hexan1-ol, and volatile steroids, such as 5α-androst-16-en-3α-ol (androstenol) and 4,16-androstadien-3-one (androstadienone), although their concentration is much lower than that of other compounds [
41,
42].
Up to day, most works on the study of components from axillary secretions as chemical signals were devoted to volatile steroids, whereas works on such components as fatty acids are sparse. In a preliminary study by Ferdenzi [
41], the perception of the main compound of human sweat odor more characteristic of men, 3-hydroxy-3-methylhexanoic acid (HMHA), was studied in representatives of the European and Madagascar populations. A change in sensitivity to this acid was found to depend on the phase of the menstrual cycle: women in the fertile phase of the cycle (the period of 6 days before ovulation) perceived 3-hydroxy-3-methylhexanoic acid as more intense than women in the non-fertile phase, which made it a possible candidate for being a chemical signal, though further study is required. The changes in LH secretion after EMAS application shown in the current study can be used in future research as a bioassay to test activity of the mentioned components from axillary secretions. Candidate/s for the role of a human chemical signal should be necessary and sufficient to recreate the original activity of axillary secretions with the original bioassay at naturally occurring concentrations [
40].
There are two hypotheses on how individuals assess potential mating partners via telereceptive senses such as vision, olfaction, and hearing. According to the backup signals hypothesis [
43,
44,
45], multimodal cues provide redundant information, whereas the multiple messages hypothesis [
43] suggests that different modalities provide independent and distinct information about an individual’s mating-related quality. Our results of the significant effect of EMAS applications on hormone secretion fall in better with multiple messages hypothesis. They are also in good agreement with meta-analysis [
46] that showed that body odor may provide distinct and non-redundant information about an individual’s mating-related qualities as opposed to that accessible through either facial or vocal cues.