1. Introduction
The issue of how various types of inflection, such as regular (e.g., work–worked) and irregular (e.g., catch–caught) forms, are represented and processed in the brain remains a central topic in psycholinguistics. Classical full-listing models argue that all morphologically complex words are stored in their entirety, regardless of regularity [
1]. On the other hand, full-parsing models propose that these forms undergo an obligatory decomposition into their morphemic units, with only morphemes being stored [
2,
3]. Bridging these two opposing views, dual-mechanism accounts argue that there are categorical differences between regular and irregular forms, suggesting they are processed by distinct mechanisms [
4,
5,
6]. Regular forms, which follow predictable patterns of inflection (e.g., adding ‘–ed’ to form the past tense), are processed through a rule-based system, while irregular forms are stored and processed in the mental lexicon through lexical-semantic memory. In contrast, single-system accounts challenge this dichotomy, suggesting continuous rather than categorical distinctions between regular and irregular inflections [
7,
8,
9]. These theories, based on earlier connectionist models [
10,
11], advocate for a single associative system that maps orthographic or phonological forms to semantic meanings, leading to graded effects in inflectional morphology.
In the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) domain, recent studies have investigated brain regions involved in processing morphologically complex words. Initial fMRI research focused on identifying brain regions specifically tuned to morphological processing [
12,
13]. Devlin et al. [
13] observed reduced activation in temporal and parietal regions, such as the bilateral angular gyrus (AG), left occipitotemporal cortex, and left middle temporal gyrus (MTG) for morphologically related word pairs compared to unrelated pairs, suggesting that morphology results from the convergence of form and meaning. Similarly, in an fMRI study using a masked priming paradigm, Gold and Rastle [
14] reported the involvement of occipital regions, including the fusiform gyrus (FG) and middle occipital gyrus (MOG), in both morphological and orthographic relationships, with semantic conditions reducing activation in the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), indicating a structural nature of early morphological decomposition. The debate continues about whether morphologically complex words are processed as whole units or decomposed into morphemes. Some neuroimaging evidence supports whole-word processing [
13,
15], while other evidence favors morpheme decomposition [
16]. Whole-word processing highlights lexical-semantic effects in widespread bilateral frontotemporal regions, including the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), superior temporal gyrus (STG), and inferior frontal gyrus pars orbitalis (IFG, BA 47) [
15,
17]. Conversely, decomposition emphasizes the role of the posterior left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG, BA 44/45) [
16].
The LIFG has been consistently implicated in the literature as a core region for processing inflectional morphology, particularly the English past tense, demonstrating distinct activation patterns for regular and irregular inflections. Both types show increased activity in temporal and hippocampal regions, including the fusiform gyrus (FG), middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG). Regular verbs, however, exhibit greater activation in the LIFG, along with additional regions such as the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), basal ganglia, and cerebellum [
18,
19,
20,
21]. This binary distinction aligns with the dual-route theory, positing two memory systems: the procedural memory system for regular inflections and the declarative memory system for irregular ones [
6,
22,
23]. Neuroimaging studies corroborate this theory, showing selective activation for regular inflections in the procedural memory network, characterized by the left-lateralized frontotemporal network, including the LIFG, basal ganglia, and cerebellum [
24,
25,
26].
However, some researchers provide evidence for a single-system theory, which suggests continuous differences in inflection processing [
7,
27]. Joanisse and Seidenberg [
7] found greater activation for regular compared to irregular verbs in the bilateral IFG during a past-tense generation task. They observed that phonologically similar irregulars elicited similar activation to regulars, while irregulars with no phonological similarity showed increased activity. Desai et al. [
27] also reported no additional activation for regular inflected verbs compared to irregulars when phonological complexity was matched. These findings suggest that inflectional morphology is influenced by phonological, semantic, and probabilistic factors rather than a binary rule-based system.
Albeit the heavy focus on English past tense, some neuroimaging studies have also investigated the neural correlates of inflectional morphology employing morphologically rich languages like Finnish, Japanese, and Korean. Finnish studies showed that inflected nouns elicited an increased activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG, BA 47), superior temporal gyrus (STG, BA 22), middle temporal gyrus (MTG, BA 21), and angular gyrus (AG, BA 39), indicating that morphological processing involves suffix stripping at the semantic-syntactic level rather than the visual word form level [
28,
29]. Japanese studies indicated greater activation in the left MTG (BA 21) for verbs compared to nouns, with selective activation for inflected verbs in the LIFG (BA 44/45) [
30]. Yim et al. [
31], the only fMRI study on Korean inflected verbs to the best of our knowledge, found similar activation patterns for regular and irregular verbs in temporal regions, including the left middle temporal gyrus (MTG), medial frontal gyrus (MFG), superior temporal gyrus (STG), and parahippocampal gyrus PHG), suggesting that Korean relies on memory and meaning irrespective of verb regularity, supporting the single-system theory.
In sum, previous fMRI literature has highlighted the distinct neural correlates involved in processing morphologically complex words, identifying widespread activation across occipital, temporal, and frontal regions [
13,
14,
15,
16,
25,
32]. Notably, the left frontotemporal regions, particularly the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG), have been proposed as the core network (procedural memory network) for the rule-based computation of regular verbs. In contrast, temporal-hippocampal regions are implicated as the declarative memory network for processing irregular verbs, which relies on the retrieval of semantic information, similar to whole-word processing [
20,
21,
24]. However, some researchers have reported no significant difference between regular and irregular verbs after controlling for phonological complexity [
7,
27] or found equivalent activation for both inflection types in the temporal regions [
31]. These findings suggest that verb regularity effects may be gradually modulated by the convergence of orthographic, semantic, and phonological information rather than by categorical differences among inflection types.
With respect to semantic processing, the LIFG has been also highlighted for its role of top-down control over activating and selecting meanings of homonyms [
33,
34,
35]. Other relevant regions include the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), angular gyrus (AG), and supramarginal gyrus (SMG), associated with accessing and encoding lexical-semantic representations [
34,
36,
37,
38,
39,
40]. For instance, Hoffman and Tamm [
38] found that the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG) play roles in semantic control and representation, respectively. Nevertheless, it remains uncertain whether the semantic information engages in the processing of inflected verbs, and if so, whether it is processed in a distinct mechanism depending on the regularity.
In the present study using rapid event-related fMRI design with the lexical decision task (LDT), we aimed to investigate if different inflection types in Korean verbs show distinct brain activation patterns and how semantic information affects morphological processing. In this regard, we manipulated three inflection types including the uninflected base form as well as regularly and irregularly inflected verbs to examine whether inflected forms incur a processing cost and whether base and inflected forms exhibit graded effects or distinct differences [
41,
42]. Furthermore, by orthogonally manipulating the semantic ambiguity of verbs and comparing homonymous verbs with those possessing a single meaning within each type of inflection, we sought to elucidate the underlying processes involved in morphological and semantic processing. In Korean, morphologically complex words convey semantic and syntactic information at the single-word level. Moreover, Korean verbs are inherently polymorphemic, composed of a stem and various suffixes. These unique characteristics make the Korean writing system suitable for investigation of the complex interplay of form and meaning during the processing of inflectional morphology.
We hypothesized that memory-based retrieval of inflected forms would activate widespread bilateral frontotemporal regions, including the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), superior temporal gyrus (STG), and anterior inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, BA 47). In contrast, rule-based decomposition was expected to activate the posterior left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG, BA 44/45). If regular and irregular inflections showed categorical differences, regular inflections would increase left frontotemporal activation, especially in the posterior LIFG. If graded effects were assumed, both inflections would increase activation in the left frontotemporal network, with regular verbs showing the most pronounced activation. Semantic effects were expected to modulate activation in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL), including the angular gyrus (AG) and supramarginal gyrus (SMG).
3. Results
3.1. Behavioral Results
Behavioral performance on the lexical decision task is displayed in
Table 1. Trials with reaction times exceeding 3
SDs (standard deviations) from the mean or error rates above 30% were excluded from the analysis. This accounted for 9.17% of the responses to word targets. Only correct responses were included in the RT analysis. The overall accuracy rate was 90.94 ± 28.72%, and the RT was 580 ± 117 ms on average.
A repeated measures ANOVA conducted on the reaction time (RT) data revealed a significant main effect of Morphology [F(2, 42) = 21.05, p < .001, η² = .06]. Specifically, the base form elicited faster responses compared to both regularly and irregularly inflected forms. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests confirmed that responses were significantly faster for the base form compared to the regular form [t(43) = –5.54, p < .001] and the irregular form [t(44) = –6.05, p < .001]. There was no significant difference in RTs between the regular and irregular forms [t(43) = 0.44, p > .1].
Additionally, a significant interaction between Morphology and Ambiguity was found [F(2, 42) = 4.13, p = .023, η² = .01]. Subsequent post-hoc pairwise t-tests revealed a significant ambiguity advantage for the base form, where homonyms elicited faster latencies compared to control words [t(22) = –2.24, p = .04]. Conversely, a marginally significant ambiguity disadvantage was observed in the regular form, with homonyms resulting in slower latencies compared to control words [t(22) = 1.87, p = .076].
An analysis of error rates also demonstrated a significant main effect of Morphology [F(2, 42) = 12.9, p < .001, η² = .09], consistent with the RT analysis. Participants responded more accurately for the base form compared to the regular form [t(44) = 4.24, p < .001]. There was a trend-toward-significance in the comparison between the base and irregular forms, with the base form showing a higher accuracy [t(44) = 2.39, p = .063]. No further significant effects were observed in the error rate analysis.
3.2. GLM Results
The results of the two-staged mixed effect general linear model (GLM) analysis on whole-brain activation are detailed in
Table 2 and
Table 3 and illustrated in
Figure 2 and
Figure 3. A broad array of brain regions associated with language processing showed activation for each type of morphology and semantic ambiguity condition compared to the non-linguistic baseline mask condition. These regions included the precentral gyrus, thalamus, supplementary motor area, middle occipital gyrus, lingual gyrus, and fusiform gyrus.
Follow-up repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted on significant clusters identified in the whole-brain analysis, focusing on occipitotemporal regions including the left fusiform gyrus (FG), lingual gyrus (LG), and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). The factors were Morphology (3 levels: base, regular, irregular) and Semantic Ambiguity (2 levels: control, homonym). When significant effects were detected, post-hoc pairwise t-tests with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons were performed, as illustrated in
Figure 4.
The main effect of Morphology was significant in the FG [F(2, 42) = 10.74, p < .001, η² = .09] and MOG [F(2, 42) = 14.85, p < .001, η² = .09], and marginally significant in the LG [F(2, 42) = 3.11, p = .055, η² = .03]. Post-hoc pairwise t-tests revealed that the regular form elicited increased activation in the FG compared to the irregular form [t(44) = –4.26, p < .001]. There was also a marginally significant increase in activation for the regular form compared to the base form [t(44) = –2.47, p = .052]. In the MOG, there was a significant decrease in activation for irregular forms compared to both the base form [t(44) = 3.56, p = .003] and the regular form [t(44) = –4.58, p < .001]. The LG showed a marginally significant increase in activation for the regular form compared to the irregular form [t(44) = –2.34, p = .071].
The main effect of Semantic Ambiguity was significant in all regions examined. In the FG [F(1, 21) = 21.17, p < .001, η² = .07], LG [F(1, 21) = 10.99, p = .003, η² = .03], and MOG [F(1, 21) = 33.37, p < .001, η² = .13], homonyms showed reduced activation relative to control words. The differences were statistically significant with t(65) = 3.69, p < .001 for FG, t(65) = 3.00, p = .004 for LG, and t(65) = 5.19, p < .001 for MOG.
The interaction between Morphology and Semantic Ambiguity was significant in the LG [F(2, 42) = 6.04, p = .005, η² = .04], and marginally significant in the MOG [F(2, 42) = 2.47, p = .096, η² = .03]. An ambiguity advantage, characterized by increased activation for control words relative to homonyms, was observed in the MOG for both the base form [t(21) = 3.53, p = .002] and the irregular form [t(21) = 5.42, p < .001]. In the LG, a significant increase in activation for control words compared to homonyms was found for the base form [t(21) = 3.81, p = .001]. However, no significant differences between control and homonyms were found in the regular condition for any region (all ps > .1).
3.3. ROI Results
Region of interest (ROI) analyses were conducted on brain regions implicated in previous fMRI literature [
19,
56], including the left inferior frontal gyrus pars triangularis (LIFG tri) and pars orbitalis (LIFG orb), bilateral angular gyrus (AG), and middle temporal gyrus (MTG).
Figure 5 illustrates the ROIs that showed a significant main effect of Morphology in the repeated measures ANOVAs, which included factors Morphology and Ambiguity.
Significant main effects of Morphology were observed in the LIFG tri [F(2, 42) = 5.52, p = .007, η² = .031] and bilateral AG [left: F(2, 46) = 3.83, p = .03, η² = .03; right: F(2, 46) = 4.17, p = .02, η² = .031]. Post-hoc paired t-tests indicated a significantly increased activation for regular verbs compared to base forms in the LIFG tri [t(43) = –2.95, p = .016]. Conversely, the regular form showed decreased activation relative to the base form in the bilateral AG [left: t(43) = 2.91, p = .006; right: t(43) = 2.58, p = .04].
One-sample t-tests were performed to test for activation of each inflection type relative to the baseline mask. The results revealed that processing inflected forms (regular and irregular) selectively increased activation in the LIFG tri [regular: t(43) = 3.52, p = .001; irregular: t(43) = 2.09, p = .04], with regular forms also showing increased activation in the LIFG orb [t(43) = 2.45, p = .02]. In contrast, processing verbs, regardless of inflection type, was associated with increased deactivation in the left AG [base: t(43) = –2.12, p = .04; regular: t(43) = –4.89, p < .001; irregular: t(43) = –2.55, p = .014] and right AG [base: t(43) = –2.05, p = .046; regular: t(43) = –5.12, p < .001; irregular: t(43) = –3.56, p = .001]. A similar pattern of deactivation was also found in the right MTG for all types of verbs [base: t(43) = –2.98, p = .004; regular: t(43) = –2.29, p = .027; irregular: t(43) = –3.07, p = .003].
As illustrated in Figure 6, repeated measures ANOVAs revealed a significant main effect of semantic ambiguity in the right middle temporal gyrus (MTG) [F(1, 21) = 5.84, p = .025, η² = .014]. Specifically, homonyms showed a marginally significant increase in deactivation relative to control words [t(65) = 1.91, p = .061]. No other regions exhibited significant differences in response to semantic ambiguity. Further analysis using one-sample t-tests indicated that homonyms were significantly deactivated compared to the baseline mask in the bilateral angular gyrus (AG) [left: t(65) = –3.49, p < .001; right: t(65) = –4.09, p < .001] and the right MTG [t(65) = –4.25, p < .001]. Similarly, control words also showed significant deactivation in the same regions [left AG: t(65) = –4.16, p < .001; right AG: t(65) = –4.5, p < .001; right MTG: t(65) = –2.42, p = .018]. Importantly, the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) displayed an opposite pattern of activation. Both homonymous and control words elicited increased activation in the LIFG pars triangularis (LIFG tri) [control: t(65) = 3.09, p = .002; homonym: t(65) = 2.11, p = .039].
4. Discussion
The present study aimed to investigate the spatial localization of morphological processing using a rapid event-related fMRI design combined with a lexical decision task. The experiment explored how morphological inflection (base, regular, and irregular) and semantic ambiguity (control vs. homonymous) influence both behavioral performance and brain activation patterns during the visual recognition of morphologically complex Korean verbs. It was hypothesized that verb regularity and inflectional cost would be reflected in differential activation in the left frontotemporal regions, particularly the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG), with regularly inflected verbs showing increased activity relative to base or irregular forms. An interaction between form and meaning was also anticipated, with regularly inflected homonyms expected to show increased activation relative to control words in temporal regions, while base and irregular forms were expected to elicit the reverse pattern.
The behavioral results revealed a significant inflectional cost on reaction times and accuracy, indicating that participants responded faster and more accurately to base forms compared to both regular and irregular inflected forms. Additionally, a significant interaction was observed between Morphology and Ambiguity. Base forms exhibited an ambiguity advantage, whereas regular forms displayed a trend towards an ambiguity disadvantage. These findings align with previous research on inflected words in morphologically rich languages, suggesting that uninflected forms are processed more efficiently due to their direct access to semantic representations, while inflected forms require additional processing effort, leading to increased processing time and error rates [
28,
59]. Furthermore, the significant interaction between form and meaning suggests that the processing of homonymous words can be facilitated or impeded depending on their morphological form, potentially due to the interplay between morphological parsing and access to semantic representations [
60].
The whole-brain fMRI results revealed activations predominantly in occipitotemporal regions associated with morphology and semantics, including the fusiform gyrus (FG), lingual gyrus (LG), and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). Significant modulation of activation by inflection type was observed, with regular forms showing increased activation in the FG and LG compared to base and irregular forms. In contrast, irregular forms exhibited significantly reduced activation in the MOG relative to base and regular forms. These findings are consistent with previous studies implicating the occipitotemporal cortex in visual word form processing and morphological analysis [
14,
61].
The occipitotemporal cortex, especially the visual word form area (VWFA) located within the left FG, has been consistently associated with orthographic processing during visual recognition of words [
14,
62,
63,
64], and thus often suggested as evidence for a morphological decomposition driven by orthography in a blind-to semantic manner. For instance, in a masked priming fMRI experiment, Gold and Rastle [
14] found an overlapping activation in occipitotemporal regions, including the left fusiform and lingual gyri for pseudo-morphological (e.g.,
corner–CORN) and orthographic (e.g.,
brothel–BROTH) conditions, but not for the semantic (
bucket-PAIL) condition, related and more importantly, a distinctive selective activation of lingual gyrus (BA 19) for morphologically word pairs compared to unrelated condition, suggesting early morphological decomposition is driven by orthography in a blind to semantics manner. Indeed, the current data demonstrated the contribution of the FG, LG, and MOG to morphological effects, however with regular forms showing increased activation compared to either base or irregular forms, challenging the notion of obligatory segmentation processes for all morphologically complex words [
2,
64].
Furthermore, semantic effects were observed in occipitotemporal regions, with homonyms showing decreased activation relative to unambiguous words. A form-with-meaning interaction was found, where decreased MOG activation for homonyms relative to unambiguous words was evident for base and irregular forms, but not for regular forms. This pattern is consistent with behavioral findings showing a reversal of the semantic ambiguity effect for base and regular forms. The FG’s role in lexical-semantic processing, as suggested by previous literature [
61,
66] fits well with the current data, which demonstrated both morphological and semantic effects, indicating the occipitotemporal regions’ role in interfacing form and meaning in morphologically complex Korean verbs.
ROI analyses, based on previous fMRI literature [
19,
56], further elucidated the neural substrates underlying the processing of different morphological and semantic properties of verbs. The analyses focused on the LIFG pars triangularis and pars orbitalis, bilateral AG, and MTG. A significant main effect of Morphology was observed in the LIFG tri and bilateral AG. Regular forms showed increased activation in the LIFG tri compared to base forms, while the regular form showed decreased activation relative to the base form in the bilateral AG. One-sample t-tests indicated that processing inflected forms (regular and irregular) selectively increased LIFG activation in the LIFG tri, with regular forms also showing increased activation in the LIFG orb. In contrast, all verb types, regardless of inflection, were associated with increased deactivation in the bilateral AG and right MTG.
In the dual-route model of inflectional morphology [
6,
22,
23], the declarative memory network involving the temporal-hippocampal regions is expected to modulate both types of inflection. In contrast, rule-based processing of regularly inflected verbs is associated with distinctive activation in the procedural memory network, which includes the LIFG, cerebellum, and basal ganglia. According to this model, regular inflections should uniquely engage the LIFG and related structures, while both regular and irregular inflections should activate the temporal-hippocampal network. We indeed observed a significant reduction in AG activation for all verb types compared to the baseline mask, with the regular inflections showing reduced activity relative to base forms. Given the functional association of AG and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) with semantic processing [
56,
67], these results suggest that processing base form verbs benefited more from memory-based retrieval processes relative to regularly inflected verbs and that these processes are involved in processing Korean verbs, regardless of whether they are inflected, and if so, whether they are regularly or irregularly inflected.
Contrary to the expectation of the dual-route model, the posterior division of the LIFG (i.e., LIFG pars triangularis, BA 44) showed selective activation for both inflected forms but not for uninflected base forms. Importantly, regular and irregular verbs did not differ significantly in LIFG activation, with irregular verbs eliciting equivalent levels of activation, challenging the binary distinction of LIFG activation patterns for regular and irregular inflections. Furthermore, the current results contradict the notion that all morphologically complex words are processed as whole words, which would predict similar activation levels for regular and irregular inflections across widespread bilateral regions, including the IFG pars orbitalis, MTG, and occipitotemporal regions. Instead, the present findings support a single-system mechanism where continuous rather than categorical differences between regular and irregular inflections are expected. These results align with a more general function of the LIFG, which involves processing morphologically complex words, as suggested by previous fMRI literature on derivation and inflection [
16,
20,
28,
29].
Finally, semantic effects were reflected in the modulation of the temporal region, particularly the right MTG, along with the occipitotemporal regions observed in the whole-brain results. The functional contribution of temporal regions to semantic processing is well-documented [
36,
37]. Surprisingly, increased activation in these regions, interpreted as engaging greater neural resources to resolve semantic ambiguity in previous research [
34], contradicts the behavioral findings of this study, where facilitative effects for homonyms were observed for base and irregular forms. However, a significant difference in right MTG activation between homonymous and unambiguous words suggests that facilitative effects for homonyms may be due to greater activation of abundant lexical-semantic representations [
68,
69]. Furthermore, both homonymous and unambiguous words elicited equivalent activation in the LIFG pars triangularis, suggesting that processing homonyms relies more on lexical-semantic representations than on the top-down regulation of multiple competitors.
Figure 1.
Rapid event-related fMRI paradigm used in Experiment 2.
Figure 1.
Rapid event-related fMRI paradigm used in Experiment 2.
Figure 2.
The results of whole-brain analysis. for each type of condition contrasted with the baseline mask condition. (a) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of Morphology, where highlighted regions indicate significant effects for base (green), regular (red), and irregular (blue) conditions, and overlapping regions (yellow). (b) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of Semantic ambiguity condition, where highlighted regions represent significant effects for control (green) and homonym (red) conditions. The yellow color indicates overlapping regions.
Figure 2.
The results of whole-brain analysis. for each type of condition contrasted with the baseline mask condition. (a) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of Morphology, where highlighted regions indicate significant effects for base (green), regular (red), and irregular (blue) conditions, and overlapping regions (yellow). (b) The results of whole-brain analysis for each type of Semantic ambiguity condition, where highlighted regions represent significant effects for control (green) and homonym (red) conditions. The yellow color indicates overlapping regions.
Figure 3.
(a) The whole brain activation map of the fusiform gyrus (FG), lingual gyrus (LG), and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). The highlighted regions indicate FG (red), LG (blue), and MOG (yellow). (b) Post-hoc analysis results on the main effect of Morphology. (c) Post-hoc analysis results on the main effect of semantic ambiguity. (d) Post-hoc analysis results on the two-way interaction. Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05, † p < .1.
Figure 3.
(a) The whole brain activation map of the fusiform gyrus (FG), lingual gyrus (LG), and middle occipital gyrus (MOG). The highlighted regions indicate FG (red), LG (blue), and MOG (yellow). (b) Post-hoc analysis results on the main effect of Morphology. (c) Post-hoc analysis results on the main effect of semantic ambiguity. (d) Post-hoc analysis results on the two-way interaction. Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05, † p < .1.
Figure 4.
ROI analysis results that showed a significant main effect of Morphology. (a) The regions highlighted in red, blue, green, and yellow represent LIFG pars triangluaris, LIFG pars orbitalis, AG, and MTG, respectively. (b) The beta estimates for each condition and statistical comparisons among inflection types. Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05, † p < .1.
Figure 4.
ROI analysis results that showed a significant main effect of Morphology. (a) The regions highlighted in red, blue, green, and yellow represent LIFG pars triangluaris, LIFG pars orbitalis, AG, and MTG, respectively. (b) The beta estimates for each condition and statistical comparisons among inflection types. Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05, † p < .1.
Figure 5.
ROI analysis results illustrating a significant main effect of Ambiguity. The left panel shows the corresponding regions highlighted in red (SMG), blue (left MOG), and yellow (right MTG). The right panel presents the beta estimates for each Ambiguity condition and statistical comparisons between control and homonymous words. Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05, † p < .1.
Figure 5.
ROI analysis results illustrating a significant main effect of Ambiguity. The left panel shows the corresponding regions highlighted in red (SMG), blue (left MOG), and yellow (right MTG). The right panel presents the beta estimates for each Ambiguity condition and statistical comparisons between control and homonymous words. Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05, † p < .1.
Table 1.
Mean reaction time (RT, in ms), percentage of accuracy (ACC, %), and standard deviations (SD, in parenthesis) for six conditions.
Table 1.
Mean reaction time (RT, in ms), percentage of accuracy (ACC, %), and standard deviations (SD, in parenthesis) for six conditions.
Inflection type |
Semantic ambiguity |
Control |
Homonym |
RT (ms) |
ACC (%) |
RT (ms) |
ACC (%) |
Base |
568.66 (113.13) |
93.38 (24.89) |
554.9 (105.14) |
96.78 (17.67) |
Regular |
583.21 (114.33) |
87.98 (32.56) |
601.97 (121.54) |
85.19 (35.58) |
Irregular |
587.87 (121.57) |
90.85 (28.87) |
593.92 (121.29) |
90.12 (29.87) |
Table 2.
Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of morphological condition, relative to the baseline mask condition.
Table 2.
Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of morphological condition, relative to the baseline mask condition.
Contrast |
Cluster size |
Brain regions |
Hemisphere |
MNI coordinates |
Z |
x |
y |
z |
Base > Mask |
89 |
Precentral gyrus |
L |
–36 |
–28 |
56 |
5.75 |
Reg > Mask |
71 |
Calcarine |
L |
–12 |
–100 |
–4 |
5.10 |
32 |
Middle occipital gyrus |
R |
36 |
–76 |
2 |
3.40 |
116 |
Calcarine |
R |
9 |
–82 |
8 |
4.60 |
Lingual gyrus |
L |
0 |
–73 |
5 |
3.97 |
Calcarine |
R |
6 |
–88 |
2 |
3.94 |
55 |
Postcentral gyrus |
L |
–48 |
–16 |
56 |
4.54 |
Precentral gyrus |
L |
–33 |
–10 |
65 |
4.00 |
–51 |
–4 |
50 |
3.81 |
98 |
Thalamus |
R |
21 |
–22 |
2 |
4.23 |
Putamen |
R |
30 |
–4 |
–7 |
3.87 |
65 |
Fusiform gyrus |
L |
–42 |
–40 |
–19 |
4.16 |
–39 |
–76 |
–13 |
3.91 |
–42 |
–55 |
–19 |
3.55 |
39 |
Inferior parietal gyrus |
L |
–39 |
–28 |
38 |
3.98 |
Postcentral gyrus |
L |
–42 |
–34 |
50 |
3.85 |
Irreg > Mask |
55 |
Supplementary motor area |
L |
–3 |
8 |
65 |
3.64 |
Middle cingulate cortex |
L |
–3 |
–4 |
50 |
3.44 |
Table 3.
Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of semantic ambiguity condition, relative to the mask condition.
Table 3.
Brain regions that showed significant effect for each type of semantic ambiguity condition, relative to the mask condition.
Contrast |
Cluster size |
Brain regions |
Hemisphere |
MNI coordinates |
Z |
x |
y |
z |
Con > Mask |
285 |
Calcarine |
L |
–12 |
–100 |
–4 |
5.18 |
Fusiform gyrus |
L |
–36 |
–79 |
–16 |
4.67 |
Middle occipital gyrus |
L |
–27 |
–88 |
2 |
4.65 |
105 |
Postcentral gyrus |
L |
–36 |
–34 |
56 |
4.68 |
L |
–42 |
–16 |
50 |
3.53 |
Precentral gyrus |
L |
–33 |
–16 |
50 |
4.27 |
316 |
Calcarine |
R |
9 |
–82 |
8 |
4.26 |
Lingual gyrus |
R |
15 |
–94 |
–7 |
4.23 |
Hom > Mask |
119 |
Supplementary motor area |
R |
9 |
14 |
47 |
4.55 |
L |
–6 |
5 |
56 |
4.42 |
57 |
Postcentral gyrus |
L |
–36 |
–34 |
56 |
4.10 |
Precentral gyrus |
L |
–24 |
–19 |
68 |
3.46 |
–33 |
–22 |
65 |
3.46 |