Introduction
Sleeping and waking transitions and hypersomnolence are strongly associated with yawning [
1,
2]. Yawning is a physiological behavior that takes the form of a complex reflex. A yawn is a paroxysmic cycle characterized by a standard cascade of movements over roughly 6 seconds in duration [
3]. Yawns are highly stereotyped, including an involuntary and powerful gaping of the jaw with deep inspiration, followed by a peak muscular contraction and a passive closure of the jaw with expiration. Phylogenetically old, comparative research shows that yawning functions in facilitating changes in state, modifying arousal, and promoting brain thermoregulation through intracranial circulation and counter-current heat exchange [
4]. Yawning is also socially contagious in humans and some non-human animals, which appears to function in the synchronization of group activity and vigilance states [
5,
6].
Consistent with the functional significance of yawning, current evidence suggests that drowsiness, as a marker of a down regulation in arousal and vigilance, can be regarded as the most common trigger of yawning. Subjective ratings of sleepiness predict yawning [
7], the frequency of yawning is tightly coupled with sleeping and waking transitions, and excessive yawning may indicate sleep deprivation [
8]. Boredom, which is also a trigger for yawning [
9], occurs when the main source of stimulation in an individual’s environment can no longer sustain their attention or interest. This too induces drowsiness by stimulating the sleep generating system [
10]. Furthermore, recent studies have shown that the tendency to yawn contagiously is also positively associated with subjective ratings of sleepiness [
11].
Both phylogenetically and ontogenetically, there is also a potential link between yawning and REM sleep [
12,
13,
14,
15]. A parallel curve has been demonstrated the similitude of the evolution of yawn frequency and the amount of REM sleep [
16,
17,
18]
. Furthermore, the yawning rhythm changes brain wave activity in predicted ways, as measured by EEG [
19,
20]. For instance, yawning changes sleep EEG slow-wave activity [
20]. In one study, people complaining of excessive sleepiness were spontaneously yawning while trying to stay awake in a darkened and quiet room [
19]. Yawning bouts were preceded and followed by a greater delta activity in EEG than movements, while after yawning alpha rhythms were significantly attenuated, decelerated, and shifted towards the central brain region [
19].
Aside from various and consistent links between yawning and sleep propensity, sleep problems, and sleep stages, the physiology of yawning intersects with thermoregulatory function [
21], and atypical or frequent patterns of yawning have been associated with conditions such as epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, stroke, and systemic lupus erythematosus [
22,
23]. From the standpoint of neuropharmacology, a wide range of neurotransmitters and neurohormones are involved in the induction and regulation of yawning, including dopamine, acetylcholine, serotonin, glutamate, opioids, oxytocin, GABA, nitric oxide, adrenergics, as well as the proopiomelanocortin -derived peptides ACTH and a-MSH. Interestingly, despite this diverse set of neurotransmitters, the major part of the yawning process is mediated by at least three distinct pathways, all of which appear to converge on cholinergic neurons within the hippocampus [
24]. Notably, the pattern of yawning is distinguishably different in physiologic and pathologic conditions [
13]. For further examination of these and other intersections, we refer the interested reader to recent review articles covering these topics [
22,
24,
25,
26,
27].
Hypothesis: By disrupting normal yawning, face masks may impact the results of sleep studies.
The typical and everyday expression of yawning appears to regulate important daily activities and mental states, namely those involved in transitions in arousal and state change linked with sleeping and waking. Here, we propose that face mask-wearing during COVID-19 may inhibit and/or modify this adaptive response in important ways, thus making it an important factor in sleep studies carried out during or after the COVID-19 pandemic. Most masks used for reducing the transmission of COVID-19 are designed with a fabric that is in immediate proximity to the mouth, and thus are known to impair mouth movement [
28]. As a result, normal patterns of yawning could be modulated by restricting mandibular jaw movement. That is, wearing a tightly fitted mask may inhibit the trigger of yawns altogether, as well diminish their intensity and/or duration upon initiation. Given that face masks also impact air flow, they could further impair counter-current heat exchange with the ambient air, which is a main functional component to yawning [
29]. Thus, widespread face masks use thereby could alter critical functional patterns of spontaneous yawning in an individual setting by reducing the incidence and/or magnitude of this behavior, which could subsequently alter sleep onset, sleep duration, and fatigue.
Furthermore, face masks additionally impair contagious yawning through the inhibition of yawn detection [
30]. Given that typical patterns of spontaneous yawning appear to facilitate normal sleeping and waking cycles [
9,
31], and contagious yawning can modify vigilance and synchronize sleep transitions within groups [
32], face mask-wearing could have important social impacts on sleep onset and fatigue as well. Taken together, we argue that the use of masks could create clinical uncertainty and be an important factor to consider in epidemiological studies of sleep.
Discussion
In summary, we put forth the argument that masks may alter the results of sleep-related clinical/epidemiologic studies in important many ways, namely through impairment or modification of yawning as well as the recognition of yawns in others. These potential effects, which are expected to vary across individuals and settings, should be separately considered when analyzing data to segregate genuine sleep disturbances from those that are confounded by wearing a face mask. Given that policies regarding COVID-19 prevention/mitigation have varied considerably at the international level [
57], the impact of face masks on prior sleep studies would need to be evaluated based on the sample collected. Similarly, the practice and technique of face mask use has also varied across different community settings [
58,
59,
60,
61].
The potential impacts of face mask use on yawning, and ultimately sleep, are likely to be linked to variables such as duration of wearing and frequency of changing, as well as the thickness, type, sealing, and tightness of the face mask [
62,
63]. We briefly discussed the possible implications here, but further research in these areas is needed. In addition to affecting thermoregulation and arousal/vigilance by potentially disrupting the typical expression and yawning, other mechanisms that may subtly alter sleep studies include the incidence of dyspnea, hypercapnia/hypoxia, and/or headache following prolonged face mask use [
62]. In addition, it is already known that in systemic conditions such as Sjögren syndrome, face mask-induced dry eye could be combined with pre-existing eye dryness and exacerbate sleep disorders [
64].