Preprint
Hypothesis

Do Face Masks Inhibit Yawning? An Important Consideration in Sleep Studies

Altmetrics

Downloads

92

Views

45

Comments

0

This version is not peer-reviewed

Submitted:

25 August 2024

Posted:

27 August 2024

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic has led to the widespread use of face masks to reduce disease transmission, but one unintended consequence is that this may negatively impact sleep and arousal by altering the expression of yawning. Yawns are defined by an involuntary and powerful gaping of the jaw with inspiration, and the typical design of face masks may restrict and/or disrupt the mandibular jaw movement and counter-current heat exchange during this behavior. Face masks additionally impair the contagious nature of yawning by disrupting the recognition of yawning in others within social/community settings. Given that the neurophysiological consequences of yawning function in facilitating changes in state and arousal, and most notably transitions in waking and sleeping via thermoregulation, face mask usage could be an important variable to consider within sleep studies. We briefly outline these connections, highlight some important factors to consider, and discuss the implications for both prior and current research on sleep.
Keywords: 
Subject: Medicine and Pharmacology  -   Anatomy and Physiology

Highlights

  • Sleep and fatigue are closely associated with yawning.
  • Face mask-wearing could impair the initiation, intensity, duration, and physiology of normal yawning and thus might interfere with sleep physiology and epidemiology.
  • Face mask-induced effects may confound the results of sleep-related epidemiologic studies in many ways.

Introduction

Sleeping and waking transitions and hypersomnolence are strongly associated with yawning [1,2]. Yawning is a physiological behavior that takes the form of a complex reflex. A yawn is a paroxysmic cycle characterized by a standard cascade of movements over roughly 6 seconds in duration [3]. Yawns are highly stereotyped, including an involuntary and powerful gaping of the jaw with deep inspiration, followed by a peak muscular contraction and a passive closure of the jaw with expiration. Phylogenetically old, comparative research shows that yawning functions in facilitating changes in state, modifying arousal, and promoting brain thermoregulation through intracranial circulation and counter-current heat exchange [4]. Yawning is also socially contagious in humans and some non-human animals, which appears to function in the synchronization of group activity and vigilance states [5,6].
Consistent with the functional significance of yawning, current evidence suggests that drowsiness, as a marker of a down regulation in arousal and vigilance, can be regarded as the most common trigger of yawning. Subjective ratings of sleepiness predict yawning [7], the frequency of yawning is tightly coupled with sleeping and waking transitions, and excessive yawning may indicate sleep deprivation [8]. Boredom, which is also a trigger for yawning [9], occurs when the main source of stimulation in an individual’s environment can no longer sustain their attention or interest. This too induces drowsiness by stimulating the sleep generating system [10]. Furthermore, recent studies have shown that the tendency to yawn contagiously is also positively associated with subjective ratings of sleepiness [11].
Both phylogenetically and ontogenetically, there is also a potential link between yawning and REM sleep [12,13,14,15]. A parallel curve has been demonstrated the similitude of the evolution of yawn frequency and the amount of REM sleep [16,17,18]. Furthermore, the yawning rhythm changes brain wave activity in predicted ways, as measured by EEG [19,20]. For instance, yawning changes sleep EEG slow-wave activity [20]. In one study, people complaining of excessive sleepiness were spontaneously yawning while trying to stay awake in a darkened and quiet room [19]. Yawning bouts were preceded and followed by a greater delta activity in EEG than movements, while after yawning alpha rhythms were significantly attenuated, decelerated, and shifted towards the central brain region [19].
Aside from various and consistent links between yawning and sleep propensity, sleep problems, and sleep stages, the physiology of yawning intersects with thermoregulatory function [21], and atypical or frequent patterns of yawning have been associated with conditions such as epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, stroke, and systemic lupus erythematosus [22,23]. From the standpoint of neuropharmacology, a wide range of neurotransmitters and neurohormones are involved in the induction and regulation of yawning, including dopamine, acetylcholine, serotonin, glutamate, opioids, oxytocin, GABA, nitric oxide, adrenergics, as well as the proopiomelanocortin -derived peptides ACTH and a-MSH. Interestingly, despite this diverse set of neurotransmitters, the major part of the yawning process is mediated by at least three distinct pathways, all of which appear to converge on cholinergic neurons within the hippocampus [24]. Notably, the pattern of yawning is distinguishably different in physiologic and pathologic conditions [13]. For further examination of these and other intersections, we refer the interested reader to recent review articles covering these topics [22,24,25,26,27].
Hypothesis: 
By disrupting normal yawning, face masks may impact the results of sleep studies.
The typical and everyday expression of yawning appears to regulate important daily activities and mental states, namely those involved in transitions in arousal and state change linked with sleeping and waking. Here, we propose that face mask-wearing during COVID-19 may inhibit and/or modify this adaptive response in important ways, thus making it an important factor in sleep studies carried out during or after the COVID-19 pandemic. Most masks used for reducing the transmission of COVID-19 are designed with a fabric that is in immediate proximity to the mouth, and thus are known to impair mouth movement [28]. As a result, normal patterns of yawning could be modulated by restricting mandibular jaw movement. That is, wearing a tightly fitted mask may inhibit the trigger of yawns altogether, as well diminish their intensity and/or duration upon initiation. Given that face masks also impact air flow, they could further impair counter-current heat exchange with the ambient air, which is a main functional component to yawning [29]. Thus, widespread face masks use thereby could alter critical functional patterns of spontaneous yawning in an individual setting by reducing the incidence and/or magnitude of this behavior, which could subsequently alter sleep onset, sleep duration, and fatigue.
Furthermore, face masks additionally impair contagious yawning through the inhibition of yawn detection [30]. Given that typical patterns of spontaneous yawning appear to facilitate normal sleeping and waking cycles [9,31], and contagious yawning can modify vigilance and synchronize sleep transitions within groups [32], face mask-wearing could have important social impacts on sleep onset and fatigue as well. Taken together, we argue that the use of masks could create clinical uncertainty and be an important factor to consider in epidemiological studies of sleep.

Evaluation of Hypothesis

Important Factors to Consider in Relation to Face Masks and Yawning

Since there are some important similarities and differences in terms of face mask-wearing, yawning, and sleep quality in different populations and settings, we present some important factors to consider during the COVID-19 pandemic that could alter the results of sleep studies. In this brief overview, additional factors created by the COVID-19 pandemic itself, will not be covered.
(i.) Gender and affiliation: Recent studies report that women have a more responsible attitude toward COVID-19 than men [33,34]. Most studies suggest that men are more inconsiderate towards face mask-wearing, though this is not always the case [35]. Collectively, a cavalier attitude among men regarding mask-wearing may negatively affect their COVID-19 risk profile [36]. While the rate of spontaneous yawning does not vary between men and women [37], and most evidence on contagious yawning in humans demonstrates that there is no difference between women and men [38], one study indicated that women were most susceptible of yawn contagion [39]. If true, women could be more affected by using face masks. Relatedly, a study investigating gender differences in the association between smartphone use and sleep quality found that the inverse correlation between smartphone use and sleep quality was mainly driven by women [25].
Observational studies have indicated that social bonding and affiliation predicts the occurrence of yawn contagion, i.e., contagious yawning is reported to be highest between friends and family members [40]. While this finding could be driven by differences in social attention, i.e., people may be more likely to notice the yawns of those they are about [41], the use of face masks is expected to alter the rate of yawn contagion more so in social settings where people are closely affiliated.
(ii.) Age: The sociodemographic profile of mask-use during the COVID-19 outbreak is different among young and older adults [42,43,44]. For example, people 55 years and older reportedly had higher odds of wearing masks most or all the time [45], suggesting higher compliance with recommended preventive behaviors among the elderly compared to younger people [46].
In this case, older adults also tend to yawn less than younger folks [7], mainly in the morning and mid-afternoon. Like young people, older subjects yawn mostly after awakening and before sleep but manifest an earlier morning peak and evening rise [7]. A similar negative correlation between yawn contagion and age also been documented [47,48]. Thus, these age-related differences in behavior (in terms of face mask-wearing behavior and yawning frequency/timing) suggest that, among those that regularly use masks, youngest people may be most influenced by their potential negative impact.
(iii.) Occupation settings: While only 2.3% of the Chinese workforce reports insomnia [49], 58% of the frontline medical staff reports suffering from severe insomnia [50]. Similar variability has also been observed in other countries. In Italy, for example, the prevalence of insomnia ranged from 37.6% to 57% [51].
During the pandemic, many night workers were required to wear a face mask. Yawning is disproportionately more frequent in the early morning and the late evening, i.e., close to sleeping and waking transitions. Consistent with the view that yawns represent a behavioral sign of sleep propensity, the temporal distribution of yawning frequency also varies between chronotypes [2]. Similarly, interindividual variation in contagious yawning is thought to depend on multiple factors, such as an individual’s propensity to spontaneously yawn and a heightened level of sleepiness [32]. As a result, the contagious effect of yawning also varies across the day. Both spontaneous and contagious yawning peak in the early morning and the late evening, according to the sleepiness time course [7]. Thus, disruptions in yawning resulting from face mask use would be expected to disproportionately influence individuals working in certain professions, potentially interacting with differences in sleep-wake rhythms. Consistent with this view, night shift workers, including health care professionals, are concomitantly at a higher risk of COVID-19 and sleep disorders [52,53,54,55,56].

Discussion

In summary, we put forth the argument that masks may alter the results of sleep-related clinical/epidemiologic studies in important many ways, namely through impairment or modification of yawning as well as the recognition of yawns in others. These potential effects, which are expected to vary across individuals and settings, should be separately considered when analyzing data to segregate genuine sleep disturbances from those that are confounded by wearing a face mask. Given that policies regarding COVID-19 prevention/mitigation have varied considerably at the international level [57], the impact of face masks on prior sleep studies would need to be evaluated based on the sample collected. Similarly, the practice and technique of face mask use has also varied across different community settings [58,59,60,61].
The potential impacts of face mask use on yawning, and ultimately sleep, are likely to be linked to variables such as duration of wearing and frequency of changing, as well as the thickness, type, sealing, and tightness of the face mask [62,63]. We briefly discussed the possible implications here, but further research in these areas is needed. In addition to affecting thermoregulation and arousal/vigilance by potentially disrupting the typical expression and yawning, other mechanisms that may subtly alter sleep studies include the incidence of dyspnea, hypercapnia/hypoxia, and/or headache following prolonged face mask use [62]. In addition, it is already known that in systemic conditions such as Sjögren syndrome, face mask-induced dry eye could be combined with pre-existing eye dryness and exacerbate sleep disorders [64].

Conclusion

Face mask usage should be considered within systematic reviews and meta-analyses (pre-COVID-19 vs. post-COVID-19) as well as within existing and future studies on sleep. If it proves to be the case that face masks do significantly impact sleep propensity, duration, and/or quality by altering yawning, this will have important implications for sleep-related research.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of Interest

No conflicts of interest declared.

Abbreviations

ACTH, adrenocorticotropic hormone; a-MSH, alpha-melanocyte-stimulating hormone; COVID-19, coronavirus disease 2019; EEG, electroencephalogram; GABA, gamma-aminobutyric acid; REM, rapid eye movement.

References

  1. Giganti F, Hayes MJ, Akilesh MR, Salzarulo P. Yawning and behavioral states in premature infants. Developmental psychobiology 2002, 41, 289–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Zilli I, Giganti F, Salzarulo P. Yawning in morning and evening types. Physiology & behavior 2007, 91, 218–222.
  3. Gallup AC, Church AM, Pelegrino AJ. Yawn duration predicts brain weight and cortical neuron number in mammals. Biol. Lett. 2016, 12, 20160545. [Google Scholar]
  4. Gallup AC, Eldakar OT. The thermoregulatory theory of yawning: what we know from over 5 years of research. Front. Neurosci. 2013, 6, 188. [Google Scholar]
  5. Casetta G, Nolfo AP, Palagi E. Yawn contagion promotes motor synchrony in wild lions, Panthera leo. Animal Behaviour 2021, 174, 149–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Gallup AC, Meyers K. Seeing others yawn selectively enhances vigilance: an eye-tracking study of snake detection. Animal Cognition 2021, 24, 583–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Giganti F, Zilli I, Aboudan S, Salzarulo P. Sleep, sleepiness and yawning. In The mystery of yawning in physiology and disease. 28 ed. Karger Publishers, 2010; pp. 42–46.
  8. Catli T, Acar M, Hanci D, Arikan OK, Cingi C. Importance of yawning in the evaluation of excessive daytime sleepiness: a prospective clinical study. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology 2015, 272, 3611–3615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Provine RR, Hamernik HB. Yawning: effects of stimulus interest. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1986, 24, 437–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Walusinski, O. The mystery of yawning in physiology and disease. Karger 2010.
  11. Gallup AC, Kret ME, Eldakar OT, Folz J, Massen JJ. People that score high on psychopathic traits are less likely to yawn contagiously. Scientific reports 2021, 11, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  12. Walusinski, O. Fetal yawning. Front Neurol Neurosci 2010, 28, 32–41. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  13. Walusinski, O. Yawning in diseases. Eur.Neurol 2009, 62, 180–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Walusinski, O. Yawning: unsuspected avenue for a better understanding of arousal and interoception. Med Hypotheses 2006, 67, 6–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Lobo LL, Neumann BG, Eidman DS, Tufik S+. Effects of REM sleep deprivation on ACTH-induced yawning. Pharmacology 1990, 40, 174–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Menin D, Ballardini E, Panebianco R, Garani G, Borgna-Pignatti C, Oster H, Dondi M. Factors affecting yawning frequencies in preterm neonates. PLoS One 2022, 17, e0268083. [Google Scholar]
  17. Menin D, Aureli T, Dondi M. Two forms of yawning modulation in three months old infants during the Face to Face Still Face paradigm. PLoS One 2022, 17, e0263510. [Google Scholar]
  18. Menin D, Costabile A, Tenuta F, Oster H, Dondi M. Identifying fetal yawns based on temporal dynamics of mouth openings: A preterm neonate model using support vector machines (SVMs). PLoS One 2019, 14, e0226921. [Google Scholar]
  19. Guggisberg AG, Mathis J, Herrmann US, Hess CW. The functional relationship between yawning and vigilance. Behavioural brain research 2007, 179, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Opp MR, Obal Jr FERE, Krueger JM. Effects of alpha-MSH on sleep, behavior, and brain temperature: interactions with IL 1. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology 1988, 255, R914–R922.
  21. Gallup, G.G., Jr. Gallup AC. Excessive yawning and thermoregulation: two case histories of chronic, debilitating bouts of yawning. Sleep Breath. 2010, 14, 157–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Teive HA, Munhoz RP, Camargo CH, Walusinski O. Yawning in neurology: a review. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria 2018, 76, 473–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Walusinski, O. Associated diseases. In The mystery of yawning in physiology and disease. 28 ed. Karger Publishers, 2010; pp. 140–155. [Google Scholar]
  24. Gupta S, Mittal S. Yawning and its physiological significance. Int.J Appl.Basic Med Res. 2013, 3, 11–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Amez S, Vujic S, Soffers P, Baert S. Yawning while scrolling? Examining gender differences in the association between smartphone use and sleep quality. J Sleep Res. 2020, 29, e12971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Krestel H, Bassetti CL, Walusinski O. Yawning-Its anatomy, chemistry, role, and pathological considerations. Prog.Neurobiol. 2018, 161, 61–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Miki H, Minakuchi H, Miyagi M, Hara ES, Shigemoto S, Suzuki Y, Maekawa K, Matsuka Y, Clark GT, Kuboki T. Association of masticatory muscle activity with sleep arousal and other concomitant movements during sleep. J Oral.Rehabil. 2020, 47, 281–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Cappa CD, San Francisco Opera Costume Department, Ristenpart WD, Barreda S, Bouvier NM, Levintal E, Wexler AS, Roman SA. A highly efficient cloth facemask design. Aerosol Science and Technology 2021, 1–21.
  29. Gallup AC, Gallup GG, Jr. Yawning and thermoregulation. Physiol Behav. 2008, 95, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gori M, Schiatti L, Amadeo MB. Masking Emotions: Face Masks Impair How We Read Emotions. Front Psychol. 2021, 12, 669432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Baenninger R, Binkley S, Baenninger M. Field observations of yawning and activity in humans. Physiology & behavior 1996, 59, 421–425. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gallup, AC. The causes and consequences of yawning in animal groups. Animal Behaviour 2022, 187, 209–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Muslih M, Susanti HD, Rias YA, Chung MH. Knowledge, attitude, and practice of indonesian residents toward covid-19: A cross-sectional survey. International journal of environmental research and public health 2021, 18, 4473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Oliver N, Barber X, Roomp K, Roomp K. Assessing the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic in Spain: large-scale, online, self-reported population survey. Journal of medical Internet research 2020, 22, e21319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Howard, MC. Gender, face mask perceptions, and face mask wearing: Are men being dangerous during the COVID-19 pandemic? Personality and individual differences 2021, 170, 110417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Bwire, GM. Coronavirus: Why Men are More Vulnerable to Covid-19 Than Women? SN Compr Clin Med. PubMed. Ref Type: Generic. 2020, 1GÇô3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Schino G, Aureli F. Do men yawn more than women? Ethology and Sociobiology 1989, 10, 375–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gallup AC, Massen JJM. There is no difference in contagious yawning between men and women. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2016, 3, 160174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Norscia I, Demuru E, Palagi E. She more than he: gender bias supports the empathic nature of yawn contagion in Homo sapiens. R.Soc.Open.Sci 2016, 3, 150459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Norscia I, Zanoli A, Gamba M, Palagi E. Auditory Contagious Yawning Is Highest Between Friends and Family Members: Support to the Emotional Bias Hypothesis. Front Psychol. 2020, 11, 442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gallup, AC. On the link between emotional contagion and contagious yawning. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2021, 121, 18–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mickells GE, Figueroa J, West KW, Wood A, McElhanon BO. Adherence to Masking Requirement During the COVID-19 Pandemic by Early Elementary School Children. J Sch Health 2021, 91, 555–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Tong KK, Chen JH, Yu EW, Wu AMS. Adherence to COVID-19 Precautionary Measures: Applying the Health Belief Model and Generalised Social Beliefs to a Probability Community Sample. Appl.Psychol.Health Well.Being. 2020, 12, 1205–1223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Vancini RL, Camargo-Neto L, Andrade M, de Lira CA, Dos Santos RG, Nikolaidis P, Knechtle B, Piacezzi LHV, Teixeira-Lopes MCB, ssayag Batista RE, et al. Sociodemographic profile of mask-use during the COVID-19 outbreak among young and elderly Brazilian people. JMIR.Aging. 2021.
  45. Nguyen QC, Yardi I, Gutierrez FXM, Mane H, Yue X. Leveraging 13 million responses to the US COVID-19 Trends and Impact Survey to examine vaccine hesitancy, vaccination, and mask wearing, January 2021-February 2022. BMC Public Health 2022, 22, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  46. Duan Y, Hu C, Lin Z, Liang W, Shang B, Baker JS, He J, Wang Y. Individual Preventive Behaviors of COVID-19 and Associated Psychological Factors Among Chinese Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Online Survey. Frontiers in Psychology 2022, 13.
  47. Bartholomew AJ, Cirulli ET. Individual variation in contagious yawning susceptibility is highly stable and largely unexplained by empathy or other known factors. PLoS One 2014, 9, e91773. [Google Scholar]
  48. Massen JJ, Dusch K, Eldakar OT, Gallup AC. A thermal window for yawning in humans: yawning as a brain cooling mechanism. Physiology & behavior 2014, 130, 145–148. [Google Scholar]
  49. Tan W, Hao F, McIntyre RS, Jiang L, Jiang X, Zhang L, Zhao X, Zou Y, Hu Y, Luo X, et al. Is returning to work during the COVID-19 pandemic stressful? A study on immediate mental health status and psychoneuroimmunity prevention measures of Chinese workforce. Brain Behav.Immun. 2020, 87, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Lai J, Ma S, Wang Y, Cai Z, Hu J, Wei N, Wu J, Du H, Chen T, Li R, et al. Factors Associated With Mental Health Outcomes Among Health Care Workers Exposed to Coronavirus Disease 2019. JAMA Netw. Open. 2020, 3, e203976. [Google Scholar]
  51. Casagrande M, Favieri F, Tambelli R, Forte G. The enemy who sealed the world: effects quarantine due to the COVID-19 on sleep quality, anxiety, and psychological distress in the Italian population. Sleep Med 2020, 75, 12–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Fatima Y, Bucks RS, Mamun AA, Skinner I, Rosenzweig I, Leschziner G, Skinner TC. Shift work is associated with increased risk of COVID-19: Findings from the UK Biobank cohort. J Sleep Res. 2021, e13326.
  53. Maidstone R, Anderson SG, Ray DW, Rutter MK, Durrington HJ, Blaikley JF. Shift work is associated with positive COVID-19 status in hospitalised patients. Thorax. 2021, 76, 601–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Martyn J, Grabias K, Chmiel Z. Do Polish Nurses Have a Problem with Sleepiness during the Epidemiological Threat of the SARS-CoV-2 Virus? Inquiry 2021, 58, 469580211022909. [Google Scholar]
  55. Wang X, Jiang X, Huang Q, Wang H, Gurarie D, Ndeffo-Mbah M, Fan F, Fu P, Horn MA, Mondal A, et al. Risk factors of SARS-CoV-2 infection in healthcare workers: a retrospective study of a nosocomial outbreak. Sleep Med X. 2020, 2, 100028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Zhang S, Xu Y, Wu K, Wang T, Su X, Han Q, Xi Y, Zhu S, Gao Y, Wang H, et al. Improved night shift schedule related to the mortality of critically ill patients with Corona Virus Disease 2019. Sleep Med 2020, 75, 354–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Hao R, Zhang Y, Cao Z, Li J, Xu Q, Ye L, Guo X, Zheng T, Song H. Control strategies and their effects on the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020 in representative countries. J Biosaf.Biosecur. 2021, 3, 76–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ferdous MZ, Islam MS, Sikder MT, Mosaddek ASM, Zegarra-Valdivia JA, Gozal D. Knowledge, attitude, and practice regarding COVID-19 outbreak in Bangladesh: An online-based cross-sectional study. PLoS One 2020, 15, e0239254. [Google Scholar]
  59. Ho HSW. Use of face masks in a primary care outpatient setting in Hong Kong: knowledge, attitudes and practices. Public health 2012, 126, 1001–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Lee LY, Lam EP, Chan Ck, Chan Sy, Chiu Mk, Chong Wh, Chu Kw, Hon Ms, Kwan Lk, Tsang Kl. Practice and technique of using face mask amongst adults in the community: a cross-sectional descriptive study. BMC Public Health 2020, 20, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  61. Reuben RC, Danladi MMA, Saleh DA, Ejembi PE. Knowledge, Attitudes and Practices Towards COVID-19: An Epidemiological Survey in North-Central Nigeria. J Community Health 2021, 46, 457–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kisielinski K, Giboni P, Prescher A, Klosterhalfen B, Graessel D, Funken S, Kempski O, Hirsch O. Is a Mask That Covers the Mouth and Nose Free from Undesirable Side Effects in Everyday Use and Free of Potential Hazards? Int.J Environ.Res.Public Health 2021, 18. [Google Scholar]
  63. Wai YC, Yee PJ, Thastheen VRBM. The Potential Side Effect of the Face Mask.
  64. Ayaki M, Tsubota K, Kawashima M, Kishimoto T, Mimura M, Negishi K. Sleep Disorders are a Prevalent and Serious Comorbidity in Dry Eye. Invest Ophthalmol.Vis.Sci 2018, 59, DES143–DES150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated