Arguably the key issue in early studies of creativity concerned its relationship with intelligence. One perspective was that creativity is just a particular kind of intelligence and may depend on it (cf. Getzels & Jackson, 1962). Empirical work fairly quickly refuted this view. Indeed, ever since Wallach and Kogan (1965a, 1965b), creativity and intelligence have been viewed as distinct. Not entirely distinct, however. They may be related at lower levels of intelligence. Guilford (1968) referred to this as triangular theory because bivariate scatterplots of creativity and intelligence data formed a triangle, without much dispersion at low levels of intelligence but quite a bit of variability at the upper levels. This implies that, at the upper levels of intelligence, an individual may be creative, but it is far from guaranteed. This view is also described by threshold theory, named because data suggested that there is a minimum level (or threshold) of intelligence that is necessary for creative performances (Runco & Albert, 1987). Various empirical efforts, including meta-analyses, have supported threshold theory (Gerwing et al. 2021; Kim, 2005), although the actual relationship of creativity and intelligence depends a great deal on what measures are used to quantify each (Runco & Albert, 1987).
More broadly, the creativity-intelligence relationship depends on the underlying definitions, given that both “intelligence” and “creativity” have been defined in diverse ways. The current study examined the relationship between creative potential, estimated with tests of divergent thinking (DT), and one particular kind of intelligence, namely emotional intelligence (EI). Two DT tests were used, one commonly used to estimate creative potential, and the other having a focus on social problems. The idea here was that the latter might be more strongly associated with EI than a more general test of DT. Along similar lines, a second objective of this investigation was to examine whether or not DT tests used in the current study would be more highly related to some EI subscales than others. In sum, one hypothesis was that the relationship would depend on the DT test type, and the second hypothesis was that the DT-EI relationship would differ based on EI subscales. These hypotheses were suggested by previous research, which is summarized below.
Emotional Intelligence and Creativity
The relationship between creativity and emotions has been studied for decades (e.g., Isen, 1999; Kaufmann & Vosberg, 1990), but interest seems to be on the rise (Hoffmann et al., 2021; Ivcevic & Hoffmann, 2019; Ivcevic et al., 2023; Newton, 2013; Sundquist & Lubart, 2022). The general conclusion is that creativity and emotions, although distinct in several ways, are not totally independent (Storbeck & Clore, 2007). There are important implications of this conclusion. A highly creative student might, for example, not perform well on a DT test, not because of his or her low DT skills, but perhaps because of test anxiety or the fear of being less creative than his or her peers. Ivcevic and Hoffmann (2019) recently demonstrated that emotions can fuel creativity. Two meta-analyses reported that positive mood can enhance creativity (Baas et al., 2008; Davis, 2009).
EI in particular might be related to creativity. For instance, the Interpersonal component of EI (one of the core components examined in the current study) may be related to creativity when an individual must convince others about his or her creative idea or product (i.e., persuasion; Simonton, 1995). Further, EI and creativity may each require some degree of Adaptability, which is an essential element of EI and creativity (Bar-On, 2006; Cohen, 1989; Kim & Pierce, 2013; Petrides et al., 2007; Runco, 1999; Sternberg et al., 2019). EI is defined in the current study as “a cross-section of interrelated emotional and social competencies, skills, and facilitators that determine how effectively we understand and express ourselves, understand others and relate with them, and cope with daily demands and challenges” (Bar-On, 2006, p. 563).
Previous research has investigated the relationship between DT and EI (Ferdowsi & Razmi, 2022; Geher et al., 2017; Giancola et al., 2024; Şahin, 2016; Sanchez-Ruiz et al., 2011; Sordia et al., 2019; Tong et al., 2022; Tu et al., 2020). Perhaps the most comprehensive work in this area was a meta-analysis conducted by Xu et al. (2019), which reported a moderate relationship between creativity and EI (r = .32). This overall effect size was, however, based on EI and varied measures of creativity, such as creative personality, creative behavior, creative product, and DT. Individual studies on the relationship between EI and DT have shown mixed results. This probably reflects the different EI and DT assessments used, different indices of DT, task modality (figural vs. verbal DTs), and other factors such as gender, age, and culture. Xu et al. (2019) did not examine DT-related factors (i.e., test type, DT indices), nor did they examine the difference in the relationship between EI and DT, looking at specific EI assessment type (i.e., ability EI vs. trait EI). The current study re-examined the relationship between EI, measured by The Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory Youth Version (Bar-On & Parker, n.d.), and creative potential, as measured by two DT tests. One DT test was Titles, which has been called the best test of DT (Guilford, 1968; Runco et al., 2016). It has not been used previously in research on EI. Typically, DT in previous research on EI used the Alternative Uses Test or the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (Xu et al., 2019). The other DT test employed here was a relatively new assessment. It was chosen because it would seem to require EI (or some components of it), such as Interpersonal and Intrapersonal subscales. It assesses individuals’ ability to think about social problems and generate as many ideas as possible for these problems.
There is evidence that different DT tests elicit different performances (Erwin et al., 2022; Runco et al., 2016), which explains the hypothesis that DT-EI relationship will be affected by the DT measure used. The same applies to the EI measure used (Abdulla Alabbasi et al., 2021; O’Connor et al., 2019; Zeidner et al., 2005). Zeidner et al. (2005) examined differences between gifted and nongifted students using two different EI tests: (a) the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) and (b) the Schutte Self-Report Inventory (SSRI). Zeidner et al. (2005) found that gifted students scored higher on the MSCEIT but lower on the SSRI than their nongifted peers. In a meta-analysis on the difference between gifted and nongifted students in EI, Abdulla Alabbasi et al. (2021) found that gifted students outperformed nongifted students, and the EI measures was a significant moderator.
This is the first investigation to examine the specific components of EI (Interpersonal and Intrapersonal subscales) as they may be related to two creative potential tests. The main hypothesis was that the relationship between the Interpersonal and Intrapersonal subscales of the EI, with the Social Games test would be stronger than those using other EI subscales (i.e., Adaptability and Stress Management) because (a) the task nature of the Social Games and (b) research sometimes showing only moderate correlations between Interpersonal and Intrapersonal subscales (.39 to .75; Ghenaati & Naeini, 2019; Tommasi et al., 2023). A second objective also confirms that the present study offers a unique contribution to the creativity and EI literature. This involved taking giftedness status into account in the analyses of the relationship between DT and EI. The rationale here was that recent evidence showed that gifted students are more emotionally intelligent than their nongifted peers (Abdulla Alabbasi et al., 2021; Ogurlu, 2021) and are more creative than nongifted students (Abdulla Alabbasi et al., 2024; Dereli, 2023; Kahveci & Akgul, 2019). With few exceptions (Chan, 2005; Şahin et al., 2016; Şahin, 2016; Sanchez & Blanc, 2023), the EI-DT relationship was studied with gifted samples, and none of these works examined the difference in the EI-DT relationship based on giftedness status. A third novel contribution of the current investigation was its use of canonical correlation and path analysis to compare the association between EI and two tests of creative potential for the total sample and the gifted sample. This statistical approach minimizes the possibility of Type 1 errors. In sum, the present research addressed the following questions:
Would the EI-DT relationship differ based on the nature of the task of the two DT tests used in the current study (Social Games vs Titles Game)?
Would the Interpersonal and Intrapersonal subscales of EI be more associated with DT than the Adaptability and Stress Management EI subscales?