1. Introduction
Garon, Bryson, & Smith (2008) [
1] consider that development of the brain involves
a process of continuous construction, organisation and re-structuring of the
connections of the brain in response to the input of the environment, thus
producing in parallel an increase in the complexity of mental structures. These
changes can be associated with and linked to the development of EFs (Miyake,
Friedman, Emerson, Witzkim Howerter, & Wager, 2000) [
2].
1.1. Dimensions and Development of EFs in Early Childhood
EFs (EFs) are the basis of the ability to set a
goal and then work towards that goal, while coordinating thoughts and actions [
3]. EFs are, therefore, fundamental to various
aspects of daily life, such as mental and physical health, academic success and
success in daily living, long with social and psychological development [
4]. They include diverse dimensions, such as,
cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control and working memory [
5,
6,
7] for the functioning of the human being in
society.
EFs include processes that involve intentionality
in the control of other processes, such as impulse control, attention, thought
and behaviour [
2,
8].
For Lezak [
9], EFs
are essential mental capacities for carrying out effective, creative and
socially adapted behaviour. Sohlberg & Mateer [
10]
consider that EFs cover a series of cognitive processes, among which are
anticipation, selection of objectives, planning, behaviour selection,
self-regulation, self-control and the use of feedback. Tirapu [
11], for example, focuses on predicting the
consequences that can lead us to anticipated solutions. Funahashi (2001) [
12] postulates that they are the result of the
coordination of the processes which are necessary to meet a particular
objective in a flexible way. Zelazo [
13]
focuses on the self-regulation skills involved in achieving a goal, that are
modulated by thinking and emotion, while basically differentiating between
executive and motivational executive processes or “hot EFs” and purely
cognitive aspects or “cool EFs”.
We can say that the “EFs” construct does not really
constitute a unitary concept, just like the prefrontal cortex, which area
considered as the neuropsychological substrate of EFs.
In relation to the number of dimensions that
constitute the EFs throughout the development in these first stages, the
results are contradictory and inconclusive. According to Monette, Bigras, &
Lafrenière [
14], the results being more
consistent at 3 years than at 4 - 5 years of age [
15,
16].
A solution with three factors implies a greater
differentiation of dimensions, which would be the most appropriate for older
children, between the ages of 8 and 13 years, for example [
17] or 15 years of age [
18].
Other studies point to the configuration of EFs by
two factors for children aged 3 to 5 years [
19]
or 5 to 13 years [
18].
Thus, some studies support a single factor for
5-year-old children [
16], in the age ranges of
2.5 to 6 years [
20] and 7 to 9 years [
21]. Brydges, Fox, Reid, & Anderson [
21] reviewed the body of literature and suggest
that the solution of a single factor is the most appropriate for children under
9 years of age.
Dimensions that configure the executive function
have been studied during the preschool stage using different techniques, mainly
factorial analysis, that allows us to know the degree of
dependence-independence of the executive function, by studying the construct
underlying it [
14,
22,
23].
There is growing evidence that EFs are key to
academic success and key as a predictor of the development of academic skills [
24,
25,
26,
27,
28,
29,
30].
Empirical studies of typical development show that
EFs test performance increases at between 3 and 6 years of age [
16,
31].
The development of EFs is a crucial aspect of
cognitive growth in children, especially during the early years of life. These
skills, which include working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive
flexibility, play a fundamental role in children's ability to plan, solve
problems, and regulate their behavior. In the context of Early Childhood
Education, analyzing the development of these functions can provide valuable
insights into how children progress through different educational stages.
1.2. Evolution of Cognitive and Social Development Across Early Childhood Education Cycles
In terms of cognitive and psychological
development, students in the first cycle (ages 0-3) are primarily engaged in
sensory exploration and motor activities. Their learning is driven by direct
experiences and free play, with a focus on developing basic skills such as fine
and gross motor coordination, imitation, and emerging language abilities. At
this stage, children are beginning to develop self-regulation and autonomy but
still depend heavily on adults for guidance and decision-making. In contrast, during
the second cycle (ages 3-6), students begin to acquire more advanced cognitive
skills. They develop working memory, sustained attention, and problem-solving
abilities. There is also a notable expansion in language use, allowing them to
follow complex instructions and express their thoughts and emotions more
clearly. Additionally, their capacity for emotional and social regulation
improves, and they start engaging in more complex and cooperative social
interactions.
The educational environment also evolves
significantly between these two cycles. In the first cycle, the learning
environment is highly stimulating and designed for safety, with an emphasis on
exploration and free play. Educators act as guides, facilitating learning
through interactive play. By the second cycle, the environment becomes more
structured, incorporating daily routines and organized activities, including
guided play and group tasks. The focus shifts toward preparing children for
primary school with an introduction to pre-academic skills such as letter and
number recognition and the ability to follow instructions and work in groups.
When considering development and learning
expectations, the goals for the first cycle are centered around sensory-motor
development, early socialization, and basic language skills. Evaluation during
this period is more observational, focusing on progress in these fundamental
areas. In the second cycle, learning goals become more formal, aiming at
preparing children for primary education. This includes a focus on initial
academic skills, understanding basic concepts, and working on structured tasks.
Assessment methods become more formal, although still adapted to the child's
age, to evaluate cognitive, linguistic, and social development.
Social interaction and behavior change notably
between the two cycles. In the first cycle, social interactions are simpler,
typically involving imitation and parallel play, with early development in
understanding social norms and cooperative play. By the second cycle, children
engage in more cooperative and group activities, showing improved understanding
and adherence to social rules, as well as enhanced abilities to share and
collaborate with peers.
These differences underscore a significant
evolution in children's development as they advance from the first to the
second cycle of Early Childhood Education, reflecting their cognitive, social,
and emotional growth.
The first and second cycles of Early Childhood
Education represent key phases in this developmental process. While the first
cycle focuses on initial exploration and the acquisition of basic skills, the
second cycle is oriented towards a greater consolidation and refinement of
these abilities. The transition between these two cycles can be a critical
period for the development of EFs, marking a significant change in how children
manage their cognitive and emotional skills.
Given the potential impact that this transition may
have on the development of EFs, it is essential to investigate how the level of
development of these skills varies between children in the first and second
stages of Early Childhood Education. Additionally, identifying the differences
in development during the transition period between the two cycles is important
for better understanding the needs and appropriate supports for children at
this crucial stage of their development.
1.3. Aim
We set ourselves the objective of knowing,
analyzing and comparing the level of development of the EFs of the infant of
the first cycle of Infant Education versus the second cycle, the age of
intercycle transition (first versus second).
The transition from the first stage to the second
cycle in Early Childhood Education marks several significant changes in the
developmental, educational, and behavioral aspects of students. These
differences are crucial for understanding how children evolve as they move
through these formative years.
How does the level of executive function
development in children in the first stage of Early Childhood Education compare
to that of children in the second stage, and what differences are observed in
the development of these functions during the transition period between the two
stages?
4. Discussion
The objective of the present study was to analyse and compare the development of EFs among schoolchildren who are in the transition period between the first and second cycles of Early Childhood Education, in order to identify the executive dimensions that are developed in this educational period, which will mark the subsequent development and academic performance.
The choice of two groups of schoolchildren with very different ages, we have allowed us to know whether, in these first periods of development, EFs form a segment or set, or whether, on the contrary, the dimensions that make up this construct are already differentiated according to different rhythms in their trajectory.
When the comparison is made using educational cycles, the differences increase in the number of indexes and clinical scales within which there are statistically significant differences, particularly, in the case of parents, having significant differences in: Inhibition, Flexibility (Index and Scale) Emotional Control, Working Memory and Global Executive Composite. In the case of teachers, there are still statistically significant differences between schoolchildren of both cycles at all scales and clinical indexes. It can be affirmed that when the comparison is made by educational level, a greater number of dimensions are obtained in which there are statistically significant differences among school children in order to design intervention programs aimed at the development of EFs in Early Childhood Education.
The results obtained are in line with the results obtained by [
15] who allude to the fact that at 3 years of age, more complex executive skills are developed (conflict resolution or active manipulation of information in working memory). At this same age, some 3-year-olds are flexible with respect to their attention in response to the demands of the situation and change, for example, their responses following clear verbal instructions. The foundations of planning and organisational skills have been evidenced in children aged 3 and 4 years [
33].
In relation to inhibitory control, the results obtained take on the same direction as those obtained by Pérez, Carboni, & Capila [
11] who conclude that in tasks requiring set change (e.g., Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) 3-year-olds are unable to inhibit the mental set in which they are currently engaged and redirect their attentional focus to the new set.
We can consider that the stage between 3 years and 5 years of age is the period in which there is improvement of the processes of memory and inhibitory control that have arisen in early childhood (0-2 years of age). These basic skills will be fully developed in the school year.
Regarding cognitive flexibility, the results also confirm what Diamond [
4] pointed out earlier, which began to develop later in the period of 2 years 10 weeks to 3 years of age, showing a significant increase during the school years.
Concerning self-regulation abilities, the data obtained are consistent with what indicates that from 3 years 6 months of age, a new stage is reached in terms of the inhibitory properties of adult speech, which is intended to be the child’s own voice as the factor that achieves some voluntary control. This is achieved in children between the ages of 3 and 4 years as during this period the impulsive function of children’s speech continues to predominate, although it slowly gives way to a semantic control that in later stages will really be determinant.
Functions of the central executive (inhibitory control, working memory and cognitive flexibility) are especially important in this stage of Early Childhood Education. Conditioning subsequent development and access at the Primary Education stage. At the end of the first cycle of Early Childhood Education, it is expected that the child will be able to initiate skills in logical/mathematical intelligence and in reading/writing, among others.
In relation to the informant, we can state in general terms that parents and teachers are reliable sources for assessing development of EFs in Early Childhood Education. However, there are differences and similarities in their perception of the development of EFs, being in agreement with other authors [
34,
35]. These results may indicate that, when the teachers are the informants they are more sensitive to development compared to the parents. On the other hand, in children of 3 years of age and of the first cycles present more difficulties in EFs, but these difficulties are reduced in the following year as a result of their own neurological maturation and their own educational intervention, that is developed in Infantile Education.
The obtained data indicate that when the informants are the teachers, statistically significant differences are obtained in all scales and clinical indexes of BRIEF-P, both when the comparison is made by educational cycles as well as by chronological age.
When the informants are the parents, statistically significant differences are obtained in a smaller number of scales and clinical indexes of BRIEF-P. Specifically, when the comparison is made by age and the informants are the parents, we obtain differences in scale and clinical index flexibility. When the comparison is made equally by age by teachers, statistically significant differences are obtained in all scales and clinical indexes.
Thus, we researched how to analyse the degree of “sensitivity” that parents and teachers have toward this development, and also to know the variable that has the greater predictive power of the performance of the EFs. The absence of statistically significant differences in the development of EFs in the two age groups (first cycle versus second cycle of Early Childhood Education) was hypothesised in this study, since the study dimensions that configure the construct of EFs are interrelated with the first periods of development. Further, it is not easy for observers (parents and teachers) to discriminate whether there is a year of difference between the two groups of participants, when comparing two groups of subjects with very similar ages.
The development of EFs is analysed differently by informants (parents versus teachers), with significant differences between respondents. Thus, when the informants are teachers, statistically significant differences are obtained between the two age groups that are being analysed, whereas when the informants are the parents, there are no statistically significant differences in different clinical scales (Inhibition, and Planning and Organisation) and in clinical indexes (Inhibitory Self-Control, and Emerging Metacognition). The differences found among the observers are in line with the results obtained by other researchers [
34,
35,
36,
37].
The obtained results allow us to acknowledge the existence of statistically significant differences according to the two age groups that were analysed, when evaluated by both parents and teachers. These data confirm that the different executive dimensions are supported by general neural circuits that mature, and change throughout the life cycle, making it possible to analyse and make comparisons from the first periods of the life cycle. Our data, as well as those provided by other studies [
1,
38,
39] suggest that as the development of typical children progresses, the children are maturing with respect to their executive competencies. This mean that those children are abler to inhibit automatic responses that positively affect their attentional capacity, they are more flexible, they have greater emotional control, are they are more capable of storing and managing information [
40], in the field of mathematics [
41] and in other fields of study. This typical developmental milestone will allow us to understand the difficulties that some children have in these competences; for example, children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ( [
42,
43], children with low birth weight (Anderson, McNamara, Andridge, & Keim, 2015) [
44] , children with language disorders [
45], learning disabilities [
46] or children with autism spectrum disorder [
47,
48].
We can conclude by affirming that BRIEF-P is an instrument that is sensitive to the development of EFs and it is the informant that interacts with the child, guaranteeing a plural and diverse view depending on the development contexts (home
versus school) and guarantees ecological validity of the scores. These results are in line with results obtained by other researchers [
49] and justify the creation of different scales according to age groups and educational cycle. It is observed that the different dimensions evaluated (Emergent Metacognition, Inhibitory Self-Control, and Flexibility) follow different trajectories, being in line with the approach proposed by several researchers in relation to the EFs topic [
4,
24,
50,
51,
52].
These results lead to a need to proffer intervention proposals that are sensitive to these differences in development during the first stage of the life cycle, avoiding consideration of the EFs as a segment or set, since for this first stage evidences of differences in their development have been verified among children of age groups first cycle versus second cycle of Early Childhood Education [
3,
53]. These results are consistent with neuroimaging studies, which confirm that the maturation of neural connections occurs by following a process, from the interconnected local regions to the regions of distributed connectivity, which work as the basis of the same function and this is reflected in the maturation of cognitive abilities [
54].
In the case of cognitive flexibility, the data are in line with those data provided by [
3], who indicate that flexibility emerges towards the end of the third year of life, being therefore at the border between the two age groups at were analysed.
Also, these results force us to include teachers and parents as evaluation agents and not to ignore their contributions. In accordance with this, also the need to train and instruct parents and teachers in this construct is estimated (for example, EFs and their relationship with basic instrumental skills, such as mathematical competence). The deep knowledge of the qualitative changes (and not limited to the quantitative aspects) of these cognitive processes of higher order are necessary in order to be able to have a referent norm and to be able to understand all the differences and / or deviations that occur in a typical development or disorders from the early stages of the life cycle (primary intervention) [
55].
One of the strengths that we estimate from the present study is the sample that has been extracted from all of the national territory, which guarantees the representativeness and generalisation of the obtained results.