1. Introduction
Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) is a leading health concern, with global estimates suggesting that 10 million people are annually hospitalized due to TBI [
1]. In the United States, an annual average of approximately 53 thousand TBI-associated deaths occurred between 1997 and 2007 [
2]. Both the long-term and short-term consequences of TBI are devastating to the patient and their families. Significant negative consequences on physical health, mental health, and financial stability are common, with estimates of lifetime costs among US citizens reaching
$40.6 billion in 2016 [
3]. Although mild TBI (mTBI) accounts for at least 75% of all TBI, concerns exist that total estimates do not accurately capture the full scope of affected individuals [
4].
The majority of patients with mTBI self-report the most severe symptoms occurring within the first 72 hours of injury, with improvements noticed after several days, and full recovery within weeks or months [
5]. However, 5-20% of patients report persistent symptoms into the chronic phase following initial injury [
6]. Behavioral changes, including those in sociability [
7,
8,
9] and sensory sensitivity [
10,
11,
12], are some of the many consequences that can affect patients following TBI.
Translational animal models are essential tools for investigating changes in behavior after injury, affording a means to investigate the causal relationship between TBI and functional deficits[
13]. The porcine model has emerged as a leading translational model of TBI, largely due to pig brains’ similarity to human brains in terms of cytoarchitecture, anatomy, metabolic rates, and inflammatory responses [
14]. However, a dearth of knowledge regarding behavioral sequelae following TBI in the pig exists, spanning various injury severities, assessed modalities, and behavioral tests [
13]. Particularly apparent are the lack of investigations into social and sensory changes following mTBI, a condition in which diffuse brain pathologies prevail [
15].
Therefore, this study assessed approachability and mechanical nociception in adult male and female minipigs up to 1w following a diffuse mTBI. Approachability, a component of sociability, was assessed using an in-pen, forced human approach task (FHAT), in which the minipig’s response to a forced interaction with either a known or unknown human was evaluated. Changes in mechanical nociception were evaluated using von Frey monofilaments of various strengths, a commonly used approach in both experimental and clinical assessments of tactile sensitivity [
16,
17,
18,
19,
20]. All assessments were performed on freely moving minipigs in their home runs. We hypothesized a reduction in approachability following mTBI, with a more severe aversion to forced interaction with an unknown versus known investigator. Based on consistent findings of increased sensitivity to von Frey mechanical stimulation in rodent studies [
16,
18,
20,
21,
22], we further hypothesized that minipigs would exhibit hypersensitivity to mechanical nociception sub acutely following mTBI.
4. Discussion
This study aimed to investigate changes in social behaviors and somatosensation over a 1w, subacute timeframe following cFPI-induced TBI in male and female minipigs. A porcine model was selected for its higher degree of similarity to human brain anatomy, cytoarchitecture, ratios of grey matter to white matter, and neuroinflammation, which promotes increased translatability [
27,
28,
29,
30,
31,
32]. Approachability, a component of sociability, was assessed via modified in-pen FHAT, and changes in mechanical somatosensation were assessed via the von Frey monofilament test.
The FHAT was used as an invasive measure of approachability in which human investigators entered the minipigs’ space and initiated contact with the animals. This interaction could create an avoidance reaction, thus providing insight into an animal’s overall emotional valence [
33]. A similar test has been used by farmers to assess animal welfare and the human-animal relationship in which minipigs were assessed for their degree of withdrawal from the experimenter, characterized by distance retreated and loci the minipig could be touched on [
33,
34,
35]. Our FHAT in these studies was similar to the farm paradigm, but modified depending on our physical restrictions. While each run individually housed a single minipig for testing, the neighboring runs were separated by bars and were all in the same room, which allowed minipigs to see and hear other animals. Additionally, due to single run size constraints, we could not feasibly assess the distance withdrawn. Rather, minipigs were assessed based on the experimenter’s ability to make physical contact with them and the minipig’s behavior in response to this contact on three distinct loci: neck, back, and rump.
Female TBI minipigs demonstrated a significant reduction in approachability when interacting with an unknown experimenter at 1w post-cFPI relative to female sham animals interacting with an unknown experimenter at the same 1w timepoint (
Figure 2B). This response was not seen in female TBI minipigs interacting with a known experimenter (
Figure 2A) or in male minipigs regardless of experimenter familiarity (
Figure 2). It was hypothesized that FHAT scores would significantly decrease during interactions with an unknown compared to known experimenter, regardless of sex or injury. While our findings did not fully support this hypothesis, the data does provide indications that TBI and experimenter familiarity can concomitantly result in a sex-specific decrease in approachability at more subacute timepoints after injury.
In clinical studies, mTBI has been associated with discernible negative changes in mental health [
36]. A study published in the American Journal of Psychiatry surveyed patients with mTBI, found that injury was associated with elevated odds of developing a panic disorder, social phobia, and/or agoraphobia [
7]. Additional studies have demonstrated a higher prevalence of affective disorders such as major depressive disorder in mTBI patients compared to the general population [
37]. Dysfunctions of social behavior have been noted to be prevalent in TBI patients, with studies reporting that patients encountered problems with social behaviors across all TBI severities [
8,
9]. In the current study, we found reduced approachability following TBI, but only in female cohorts at 1w post-injury. A potential explanation for this observation is that, over the 1w testing period following injury, minipigs may have become more acclimated to the unknown experimenter. And while all sham minipigs and male TBI minipigs may have become acclimated to the unknown experimenter, our findings would suggest an impairment in this acclimation in female TBI minipigs. Regardless, this sex-specific difference in approachability warrants further investigation to validate and potentially expand on.
In patients suffering with TBI, aggression is a prevalent behavior, with one study classifying up to 25% of assessed patients as aggressive [
8] while another identified verbal aggression and inappropriate social behavior as the most prominent maladaptive behavior in patients with brain injury [
38]. In our study, while some minipigs did demonstrate aggressive behaviors during FHAT, the majority of animals demonstrated more evasive behaviors. This lack of aggression could be attributed to the animal strain itself, as Yucatan minipigs are bred for smaller size and affability. Additionally, male animals were castrated, also potentially influencing aggressive tendencies. Therefore, while hormonal fluctuations might play a role in TBI-induced aggression and other social changes after injury, further studies would be needed to interrogate this possibility.
In addition, external stressors could have influenced approachability independent of injury, sex, or experimenter familiarity. While each cohort of three animals was uniform in sex, cohorts consisted of a mixture of injured and sham animals to try and mitigate potential environmental confounders cohort to cohort. However, the possibility of variability in environmental stressors across female and male cohorts cannot be excluded. It is also possible that the diffuse nature of this mild cFPI model itself could account for more variable responses. Further, the relatively short (1w) time course of assessment coupled with approachability changes seen only at the 1w timepoint suggest that farther out from injury could be where more substantial behavioral changes start to occur.
We also performed von Frey monofilament sensitivity assessments on the pinna of the ear and on the base of the tail in awake, freely moving minipigs. The home-pen environment was selected to reduce anticipated stress from moving the minipigs to a separate testing environment, potentially impacting approachability assessments. The ear and tail were chosen due to their independent mobility, physical accessibility to researchers, and functional relevance to swine behavior [
39,
40]. The ear has been shown to be a locus of nociceptive response in prior studies, with a higher occurrence of ear flicking or flapping in piglets following ear-notching [
41]. Ear flicking has also been identified as a positive response during assessments of tactile allodynia in the porcine model of neurofibromatosis type 1[
42]. Tail posture and movement are also salient indicators for swine’s emotional valence and state of arousal, namely based on tail curling, tucking, and/or wagging [
39]. Variability in ear and tail movements was beneficial in discriminating responses specific to the monofilament rather than baseline tail-wagging or ear movement or response to external stimulus, e.g., a loud noise.
Because minipigs were freely moving in their home pens during behavioral assessments, the von Frey test required two experimenters: one to perform monofilament testing and another to feed the minipig treats to keep its head and body in a relatively consistent location. While this approach afforded some consistency in positioning for the duration of testing, eating the treats would occasionally result in some rapid movements of the head and tail wagging. To avoid this potential confound experimenters waited until the ear or tail was not moving before applying the mechanical stimulation. All stimulations were done in triplicate, and a response was recorded when the animal responded at least two out of three times to that monofilament strength. While a definitive ear flick was defined as a response to mechanical stimulus, experimenters noted that some minipigs would ‘brace’ their ears backwards and closer to their heads after the filament was tested rather than flick them. Such a response was documented but not counted as a response to stimulation. Backward-pointed ear positioning has been shown to occur at higher rates in response to aversive stimulation, indicating a negative valence [
40], thus indicating a potentially different but aversive response to von Frey stimulation that would require more investigation in future studies.
Male and female TBI minipigs had significantly reduced sensitivity scores at all assessed post-injury timepoints compared to pre-injury scores in a location- and sex-specific manner. Male minipigs displayed hyposensitivity in the ear while female minipigs displayed hyposensitivity in the tail following TBI (Figure 4). While this study was not powered to specifically investigate sex differences, the current data indicate a potential influence of sex in TBI-induced sensitivity changes, thus warranting further studies powered to resolve such potential effects.
The observed reduction in minipig ear and tail sensitivity following TBI is contrary to findings in rodent models of TBI, in which sensitivity in multiple loci increases following injury [
18,
20]. Somatosensory hypersensitivity observed in rodents post-TBI has been associated with increased inflammation in the sensory cortex [
30,
31,
32,
43]. In clinical studies, the consequences of TBI include varied atypical sensory sensitivities [
10]. Changes in sensitivity span a wide range of modalities such as light, auditory, olfactory, gustatory, and vestibular perception, with a majority of studies identifying hypersensitivity following injury [
44,
45,
46,
47,
48]. Brain injury has also been linked to changes in mechanical nociception, with reports of post-injury hypersensitivity and tactile anomalies [
11,
12]. However, there is still a gap in the literature regarding changes in tactile sensitivity following TBI, especially in mTBI with primarily diffuse pathologies.
Due to the high degree of variability between pigs (
Figure S2), it is difficult to draw firm conclusions regarding the observed decreased von Frey sensitivities in the current study. Interestingly, a study that utilized the von Frey test to examine the impact of prenatal stress on nociception in juvenile pigs found that stressed groups exhibited less sensitivity to mechanical stimulus compared to non-stressed controls [
49]. Coupled with our findings, this suggests hyposensitivity to mechanical nociception could be a common and expected response in minipigs exposed to injury and/or stress. Due to the lack of publications using von Frey sensitivity assessments in minipigs, however, the findings remain difficult to validate.
It is further possible that the observed reduction in von Frey monofilament responses might not signify reductions in sensitivity. It is possible that during stimulation of the ear, the monofilaments could have contacted hairs growing in the ear, potentially causing the response rather than the monofilament contact with the pinna itself. If this were the case, however, we would have expected observed hypersensitivity rather than hyposensitivity. Additionally, the lack of response to high gauge Von Frey filaments in both the ear and tail of TBI pigs could indicate that the pig’s attention was more easily diverted away from the sensory stimulus and toward the experimenter feeding them snacks. A lack of response to von Frey monofilament stimulation could potentially also indicate an indifference to a stimulus that is perceived as non-dangerous, however, the fact that this was not seen in sham animals reduces the likelihood that this is a normal response. Future studies could be designed to eliminate potential confounds of attention diversion by restraining the pig during von Frey testing. Future studies could also investigate additional stimulus loci, such as the periorbital region or the tip of the snout, to determine if Von Frey filament responsiveness might differ across other regions in a sex-dependent manner as well.