1. Introduction
Scientists have long been intrigued by the specialized adaptations of climbing plants that enable them to compete for necessary resources such as sunlight [
1]. But, despite this prolonged fascination, we know surprisingly little about how climbers make ‘decisions’ with regard to stimulus searching and attachment behaviors. Indeed, climbing plants can be an ideal model system to study the decision-making in plants because they show rapid changes in response to environmental cues [
2]. For them, finding a suitable support upon which they can climb is among the most important factors affecting their growth and development [
3].
The study of climbing plant behavior is chiefly based on Darwin’s observations on the oscillatory movements of exploring stems and tendrils (i.e.,
circumnutation) [
4]. He noted that vines are not only able to locate potential supports and grow towards them, but they can even show aversion towards them [
4]. He first described this effect with regard to
Bignonia capreolata L. tendrils that initially seized and then let go of sticks that were inappropriate in terms of size. If, because of its thickness a stimulus was perceived as ‘inadequate’, after initially seizing it, the tendrils let go of it [
4]. A similar phenomenon was observed when herbaceous twining vines met a very thick trunk. Instead of winding around the tree trunk, they wound around themselves. As far as annual vines were concerned, Darwin commented that, independently of diameter constraints, it would have been maladaptive for the vines to wrap around thick, hence large, trees, as they would improbably reach higher light levels by the end of the growing season [
4].
The cases cited above provide a degree of support to speculative claims that some climbing plants can judge the thickness of potential supports and modify their
circumnutation patterns to a greater or lesser extent depending on features of potential supports with respect to what would be expected by chance movement. Experimental evidence demonstrating that this might indeed be the case has been forthcoming from recent studies that used kinematic analysis to characterize the movements of the tendrils of pea plants (
Pisum sativum L.) [5-9]. Guerra and colleagues demonstrated that pea plants are able to perceive a support and modulate the kinematics of the tendrils’ aperture depending on its thickness [
7]. The aperture of the tendrils refers to the maximum distance between the tips of the tendrils reached during movements leaning towards a support. The average and the maximum velocity of the tendrils were found to be higher for thinner supports compared to thicker ones. In temporal terms, it took more time for the tendrils to reach peak velocity and maximum aperture when the supports were thinner [
5,
7]. Further, they modulate the production of a number of secondary velocity peaks (i.e., submovements) as a function of the support’s thickness [
6]. The frequency of submovements tends to increase when the support is thick. This signifies that they need to make more adjustments in order to establish contact points along the support [
6].
These results are in line with the above evidence highlighting that for climbing plants thinner and thicker supports are different [4,10-13] with the grasping of thick supports being more ‘difficult’ since it is more energy-consuming with respect to grasping thinner ones. In fact, it implies that the plant not only needs to increase the length of its tendrils in order to efficiently wrap itself around the stimulus [
14] but also has to strengthen tensional forces to counteract gravity [
2,
15] and modulate kinematics [
7].
In light of these considerations, the aim of the current study is twofold. First, to ascertain what pea plants do when confronted with differently sized supports. To test this, after germination pea plants have been exposed to both a thin and a thick support. We hypothesized that if pea plants inevitably prefer thinner supports, then we should observe a significantly higher frequency of movements directed toward them. Second, to ascertain whether such a decisional process impacts on the kinematics of tendrils’ circumnutations, we compared a ‘choice’ condition termed as the “double-support” (DS) condition in which a thin and a thick support were present in the environment with a “single-support” (SS) condition where only a thin support was present in the environment. On the basis of the pioneering observations by the Darwin [
4], we expect a preference for thin than thick support. Further we foresee differences across conditions evident at the level of movement kinematics, despite the plants will prefer the thinner support they might still keep into account the thicker one (as a potential option for an everchanging environment) by programming a hybrid kinematical patterning accounting for differently sized supports.
3. Discussion
In the current study, we examined the behavior of pea plants raised in the presence of either a single support or two supports differing in size. The results suggest that their kinematical patterning differs depending on whether they are exposed to either one or two potential supports. And that when they are put in the position to choose between a thin or a thick support, they manifest a clear preference for the former. These findings support Darwin’s [
4] and others’ [
3,
11,
19] observations suggesting that when support diameter increases beyond a certain point, climbing plants are unable to maintain tensional forces that facilitate coiling and attachment to the support. Thus, a support with a large diameter appears to be unsuitable for coiling and climbing.
But how climbing plants do avoid an unsuitable host and choose a suitable one? A common believe is that the physiological mechanisms underlying behavioral responses in plants tend to be caused by non-integrated, local reactions [
20]. As proposed by Saito, these ‘reactions’ might also be at the basis of the decision-making processes related to support diameter characterizing tendrils’ coiling [
12]. In this view, changes in the coiling responses may be caused by local reactions in the tendrils. For instance, in many climbing plants, the coiling of tendrils is thought to be caused by the contraction of the gelatinous fibers (G fibers) after stimuli have been contacted [
12,
21]. Put simply, at the basis of climbers’ support selection there might be a passive and automatic mechanism that makes it possible to select a support with an appropriate diameter.
A point worth noting is that most studies on how climbers select a support based on diameter information focus on the final coiling response, with no or little reference to the choreography assumed by the tendrils during the approach phase [
7,
8,
22]. To fill this gap, we have measured the kinematics of tendrils’ circumnutation from the start of their growth until they grasped the support. The emerging picture might suggest a trade-off in terms of metabolic use. Grasping a thicker support would imply the growth of longer tendrils, which in turn would be more demanding in terms of energy consumption. This metabolically based decision would also reflect on movement kinematics as the movement towards thicker supports is much slower than for thinner supports [
7] and implies a great deal of on-line adjustments [
6]. Therefore, a certain degree of information processing is required to integrate, interpret, and compute the relative information that determines a preference for thin supports.
These aspects are particularly evident when comparing kinematics for the one and the two supports conditions of the present study, signifying that plants can perceive their surroundings and generate circumnutation patterns accordingly. When comparing circumnutation between the thin support for the SS and the DS conditions plants move faster and execute less but larger circumnutations for the latter than for the former. This signifies that despite they are aiming at supports of a similar size, being exposed to an alternative (the thicker support for the DS condition) determines a decisional complexity that is played out in the kinematics of circumnutation. Most importantly, our findings suggest that pea plants’ movement seems planned on the basis of the isochrony principle [
16]. The isochrony principle refers to a spontaneous tendency to increase the velocity of a movement depending on the linear extent of its trajectory to maintain the execution time approximately constant [
23]. In our circumstances, plants maintain movement duration constant and scale velocity in order to cover longer distances as witnessed by the longer circumnutation paths. This appears to be the easiest and most readily chosen organizational option by the plant to program the circumnutational patterning when a decision based on alternatives has to be taken.
Decision-making has been customarily considered as a people-centric process [
24,
25], implying the making of a choice from a number of alternatives to achieve a desired result [
26]. In recent years, decision-making has been studied on a variety of organisms [
27], including plants [
28,
29]. Dener and colleagues investigated decision-making in the root development of pea plant (
Pisum sativum) using the risk sensitivity theory (RST) [
28]. According to RST, the rational decision is the one that maximizes fitness [
30]. In the study, root growth displayed both risk-prone and risk-averse behaviors, which better support the RST hypothesis than previous animal testing. It appears that pea plants make more "rational" economic decisions than species such as birds and humans in terms of risk sensitivity [
28,
31]. Plant decision-making is also explored in the context of the social environment. Gruntman and colleagues compared the responses of
Potentilla reptans centered on their ability to outcompete their neighbors for accessing light [
29]. Observed shifts in the responses between vertical growth, shade tolerance, and lateral growth suggest that plants can choose adaptively from several alternatives under light-competition scenarios [
29].
Altogether, these findings suggest that plants possess the ability to make decisions and adjust their behavior in response to their surroundings. Our findings add this to the literature demonstrating that plant behavior is flexible, as opposed to rigid and mechanical [
32], reinforcing the idea that plants are open systems with a remarkable ability to deal with the complexities of an ever-changing environment [
33].
At this stage, the natural question is how and at which level pea plants implement such decisions which translate into specific behavioral patterns. One possible mechanism could be light acquisition at the level of the stomata [
34,
35], which might allow them to distinguish the light reflections determined by differently sized supports. Alternatively, Souza and colleagues introduced the concept of “plant electrome” describing the totality of the ionic dynamics at different scales of plant organization, engendering a constant electrical activity [
36,
37]. Souza and colleagues demonstrated that, rather than pure random noise, the amount of complexity characterizing environmental stimuli might alter several characteristics of the temporal dynamics of the plant electrome [
36,
38,
39]. It was reported that some frequencies (the higher ones) exhibited by non-stimulated plants faded after stimulation. Only the lowest frequencies remain, allowing for low-energy-cost long-distance signaling [
37]. In this view, the electrome could be considered as a unifying factor of whole plant reactivity in a constantly changing environment and therefore might be a good candidate to understand the flexible behavior of plants [
37].
In conclusion, the results of this study offer a contextual framework for the different well-known responses of climbing plants when searching for a support. More importantly, we have demonstrated a decision-making ability in plants, which allows them to adaptively ‘choose’ between responses, according to the relative structure of available supports. Overall, the results of our study suggest that plants are capable of acquiring and integrating complex information about their environment in order to adaptively modify their extent of plastic responses. Such complex decision-making in plants could have important implications for our understanding of the processes that govern plant behavior. And open to the possibility that plants may deploy a higher level of ‘cognitive’ complexity than previously thought, providing further evidence against traditional views considering plants as passive organisms.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, Q.W. and U.C.; methodology, Q.W.; software, Q.W. and V.S.; validation, Q.W.; formal analysis, Q.W.; investigation, Q.W., S.G and B.B.; resources, M.B.; data curation, Q.W.; writing—original draft preparation, Q.W. and U.C.; writing—review and editing, U.C. and Q.W.; visualization, Q.W.; supervision, U.C.; project administration, Q.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.