Preprint
Article

Factor in Fear? Interference Competition in Polymorphic Spadefoot Toad Tadpoles and Its Potential Role in Disruptive Selection

Altmetrics

Downloads

135

Views

45

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

This version is not peer-reviewed

Submitted:

11 March 2023

Posted:

14 March 2023

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Disruptive selection arises when extreme phenotypes have a fitness advantage compared to more intermediate phenotypes. Theory and evidence suggest that intraspecific resource competition is a key driver of disruptive selection. However, while competition can be indirect (exploitative) or direct (interference), the role of interference competition in disruptive selection has not been tested, and most models of disruptive selection assume exploitative competition. We experimentally investigated whether the type of competition affects the outcome of competitive interactions using a system where disruptive selection is common: Mexican spadefoot toads (Spea multiplicata). Spea tadpoles develop into alternative resource-use phenotypes: carnivores, which consume fairy shrimp and other tadpoles, and omnivores, which feed on algae and detritus. Tadpoles intermediate in phenotype have low fitness when competition is intense, as they are outcompeted by the specialized tadpoles. Our experiments revealed that the presence of carnivores significantly decreased foraging behavior in intermediate tadpoles, and that intermediate tadpoles had significantly lower growth rates in interference competition treatments with carnivores but not with omnivores. Interference competition may therefore be important in driving disruptive selection. As carnivore tadpoles are also cannibalistic, the ‘fear’ effect may have a greater impact on intermediate tadpoles than exploitative competition alone, similarly to non-consumptive effects in predator-prey or intraguild relationships.
Keywords: 
Subject: Biology and Life Sciences  -   Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics

Introduction

Darwin [1] first postulated that natural selection would favor the evolution of trait divergence that minimizes competition between individuals, leading to the evolution of new forms, and even new species. While evolutionary biologists following Darwin generally viewed this as a process occurring via interactions between different populations or species (i.e., ecological and reproductive character displacement; reviewed in [2]), intense competition for vital resources within populations can also lead to the evolution of intraspecific variation by disruptive selection [3,4,5,6]. Resource polymorphism ⎯ the occurrence of discrete intraspecific morphs with differential resource use within a population ⎯ is a potential evolutionary outcome of such disruptive selection [7]. Examples of resource polymorphism are plentiful in many taxa. Nevertheless, the level of divergence between morphs can vary tremendously between polymorphic species. In some cases, phenotypic differences (e.g., morphological, behavioral) can be very subtle and easily overlooked (e.g., pumpkinseed sunfish Lepomis gibbosus) while in other cases, differences are so dramatic that some morphs were initially described as separate species (e.g., Arctic char Salvelinus alpinus, tiger salamander Ambystoma tigrinum, spadefoot toads Spea multiplicata, and African finches Pyrenestes ostrinus) (reviewed in [7]).
Theory and empirical data suggest that intraspecific competition may result in disruptive selection when: (i) phenotypic variation is linked to resource use; (ii) competition is more intense between phenotypically similar individuals using the same limited resources; (iii) competition is density and frequency dependent; and (iv) underused resources are available (i.e. ecological opportunity). Thus, via negative frequency- and density-dependent selection, individuals with extreme resource-use traits that specialize in less common but underused resources exhibit a fitness advantage due to less intense competition with more common phenotypes [3,4,5,8]. Alternatively, it has also been argued that disruptive selection can also occur when resources are discrete (i.e., bimodal) and functional resource-use tradeoffs between them are strong (e.g., mobile and vulnerable vs. armored and sessile prey), resulting in higher fitness for specialists compared to generalists [9,10,11,12]. Importantly, these mechanisms can operate together in driving disruptive selection [5]. Although the role of intraspecific competition in disruptive selection and resource polymorphism is broadly acknowledged, competition can take several forms, a fact mostly disregarded in previous research [13,14]. In exploitative competition, individuals compete for shared, limited resources indirectly, while interference competition implies direct competition, involving antagonistic interactions that vary in intensity and consequences. Previous studies on disruptive selection have generally assumed competition to be exploitative or have not distinguished between the possible forms of competition, as exploitative competition is believed to be stronger than interference competition, particularly in less aggressive species that use more dispersed ecological resources [13,15]. Nevertheless, it has been recently suggested that interference competition may play a greater role in disruptive selection than previously considered, as it can have rapid effects on competing individuals and could modulate the negative frequency-dependent effects that arise from resource depletion and exploitative competition [13]. Interference competition may force less aggressive conspecifics to exploit underused resources, particularly with territorial species [16]. Furthermore, aggressive interactions should be more acute when extreme phenotypes not only compete with other phenotypes, but also predate upon them [17]. Although cannibalism itself may not contribute greatly to disruptive selection (but see [18]), the perceived threat of being consumed or injured by a conspecific might have more powerful ecological and evolutionary consequences than previously thought.
Insights can be drawn from studies of predator-prey or intraguild relationships. Predator-prey relations have traditionally been viewed from the perspective of lethality (i.e., predator captures and consumes prey) and density-mediated interactions. However, theoretical and empirical evidence demonstrate that non-consumptive, trait-mediated interactions may have stronger effects on prey fitness than consumptive interactions [19,20]. Fear ecology theory thus predicts that the predator affects prey more than just through consumptive interactions, as prey species are under selection to avoid predators [19]. Indeed, in the presence of predators, prey species or beta-predators (in intraguild relationships) can exhibit considerable phenotypic (i.e. behavioral, physiological, developmental or morphological) alterations to reduce their risk of mortality [21,22,23]. However, these alterations reduce the prey’s overall fitness, as they carry considerable costs, either through the direct ‘cost’ of producing said alterations, or decreases in their reproductive investment and resource acquisition [24,25]. A meta-analysis conducted by Preisser et al. [20] showed that trait-mediated interactions are a major component in predator-prey relationships, comprising at least 50% of the total predator effects, by greatly increasing trophic cascades, and particularly in aquatic environments. While the factor of fear has received much attention in predator-prey and intraguild relations [20], its role in intraspecific resource competition, and hence in resource polymorphism and disruptive selection, has received little attention despite its potential importance.
Here we experimentally investigated the effects of interference competition between resource-use morphs of the Mexican spadefoot toad (Spea multiplicata) tadpoles. Spea tadpoles plastically develop into alternative omnivore or a carnivore morphs in response to diet [26]. We focused on phenotypically intermediate tadpoles (in relation to the two morphs) that typically have low survival in natural ponds when resource competition is strong, as they are inferior competitors to both omnivore and carnivore morphs [5,14,27]. As intermediate tadpoles both compete with and are potentially consumed by the carnivore morph tadpoles, and omnivores compete with intermediates and are consumed by the carnivores [5], the system mirrors an inter-specific intraguild relationship. We first ran behavioral trials where we asked if tadpoles would significantly alter their behavior (with regards to resource acquisition – foraging, swimming, or resting) in the presence of carnivore or omnivore tadpoles. We predicted that focal tadpoles would reduce foraging in the presence of carnivore competitors. Finally, we ran a competition experiment, allowing either exploitative or exploitative + interference competition for intermediate tadpoles with either omnivore morph or carnivore morph competitors. We predicted that interference competition would have the greatest effect during competition with carnivore morph competitors.

Materials and Methods

Study system

The Mexican spadefoot toad (Spea multiplicata) is a nocturnal, predominantly fossorial amphibian species that inhabits arid areas within the southwestern United States and Mexico. They remain underground during the dry season, emerging to feed and breed during the summer monsoons. Breeding within a given ephemeral pond takes place on a single night, and development from egg to metamorphosis can occur in under two weeks [26,28]. Their tadpoles display a well-known form of resource polymorphism, as they develop into one of two environmentally-induced ecomorphs: an ‘omnivore’ or ‘carnivore’ morph [26,28]. While the former morph is a dietary generalist that feeds predominantly on detritus and algae along with small zooplankton, the latter mostly consumes fairy shrimp, and sometimes other tadpoles, including conspecific ones [28,29]). The two morphs dramatically differ in a series of morphological traits, such as body shape, gut length, jaw muscle size, and mouthpart keratinization [28,30]. The distribution of phenotypes within a pond is determined by frequency-dependent disruptive selection driven by competition for resources [5,31]. While phenotypically intermediate tadpoles exist, they have low survival in ponds with high densities of spadefoot tadpoles [5,32]. Disruptive selection disfavors intermediates, as they are outcompeted by the two specialized ecomorphs [5]. This polymorphism is a plastic response, as the development of a tadpole into a carnivore requires an environmental cue, namely the ingestion of fairy shrimp or other tadpoles [26,28].

Behavioral trials

To explore the potential behavioral responses of intermediate tadpoles to interference competition from carnivore tadpoles, we ran a series of experimental behavioral trials. We first collected live S. multiplicate tadpoles (~10 days old) by dip net from a temporary pond in Price Canyon, Arizona (‘Eagle’s Cry’) and transported them to the live animal holding facility at the Southwestern Research Station in Portal, Arizona. Prior to the experiment we determined whether a tadpole was an omnivore morph, carnivore morph or intermediate by visual inspection of mouthparts [26]. For each trial in the experiment we introduced two intermediate tadpoles into a plastic tank (412.75 mm x 285.75 mm x 171.45 mm) filled with ~ 10,000 ml of aged water. We offered food in the tank, both in the form of fairy shrimp (n=10) and ground dry fish food (detritus substitute) and allowed the tadpoles to acclimate for five minutes. We then recorded the tadpoles’ behavior every 30 seconds for five minutes, and logged if they were swimming, feeding or resting. Next we added a carnivore tadpole into the tank and proceeded to record the intermediate tadpoles’ behavior for another five minutes, as previously mentioned. We then removed the carnivore tadpole, rinsed the tank and replaced the water in the tank, reintroduced the intermediate tadpoles, added food, and allowed them to acclimate for another five minutes. Subsequently, we introduced an omnivore tadpole into the tank and recorded the focal tadpoles’ behavior for five more minutes. We ran a total of 21 trials and focal tadpoles were not reused across trials.

Competition experiment

We aimed to disentangle and measure the effects of exploitative competition alone from the combined effects of exploitative and interference competition using a microcosm laboratory experiment. For this experiment, we collected live S. multiplicata tadpoles (~7 days old) by dip net from a temporary pond near Portal, Arizona (‘Horseshoe’). We again transported them to the live animal holding facility at the Southwestern Research Station in Portal, Arizona. We measured body mass with a digital balance to the nearest 0.001g both prior to and after the experiment, and determined whether a tadpole was an omnivore morph, carnivore morph or intermediate by visual inspection of mouthparts [26]. On completion of the experiment, all tadpoles were euthanized by immersion in a buffered solution of MS-222 and preserved in 95% ethanol.
In order to disentangle the effects of purely exploitative (indirect) from exploitative and interference (direct) competition between the intermediate and specialized tadpoles, we measured growth as a proxy for fitness during a 10-day experiment, in which intermediate tadpoles competed for food against either omnivore or carnivore tadpoles, in both mixed (interference + exploitative) or single (exploitative) treatments, as follows:
To simulate conditions of both exploitative and interference competition acting together, we reared intermediate tadpoles (n=32), together with either a carnivore (n=16), or an omnivore (n=16) for the entire duration of the experiment. We housed the tadpole pairs of both treatments in plastic tanks (343 mm x 209.5 mm x 120.65 mm) filled with 3250 ml of water. As spadefoot toad tadpoles are known to accelerate development in rapidly evaporating ponds [33], we inspected water levels daily and topped up when needed in order to maintain the initial volume throughout the experiment. Carnivore treatment tadpole pairs were fed 30 fairy shrimp every 24 hours, while the omnivore treatment pairs were fed 20 mg of ground, dry cichlid pellets (as a substitute for detritus) every 48 hours. We fed the omnivore treatment pairs less frequently, as the consumption of the cichlid food is slower than that of fairy shrimp. Consequently, tadpoles within this treatment competed for food while being exposed to potential antagonistic interactions and/ or fear induced behavioral changes.
For simulating purely exploitative competition, we reared intermediate (n=32), carnivore (n=16) and omnivore (n=16) tadpoles alone in individual plastic tanks. We assigned each intermediate tadpole a specific carnivore (carnivore competition treatment; n=16), or an omnivore (omnivore competition treatment; n=16) tadpole competitor with which they competed for food in an exploitative manner. Specifically, carnivore treatment intermediates and carnivores were each fed 15 fairy shrimp every 24 hours, while omnivore treatment intermediates and omnivore tadpoles were fed 10 mg of dry cichlid food every 48 hours. After the introduction of food, tadpoles were allowed to forage for one hour, after which each tadpole was removed from its tank and introduced in its competitor’s tank, thus having access to the resources remaining after one hour of competitor foraging. Tadpoles thus competed for the same resources without experiencing direct interaction. For this treatment, we filled the tanks with 1625 ml of water, representing half of the volume used in the interference treatment, as only one tadpole was present at any given time in a tank, as opposed to two in the interference treatment. The water level was inspected and maintained constant, as for the previous treatment. It is important to note, that while this design minimized interference competition, it could not remove all potential sources of interference competition. There was no possibility for direct, physical or visual interactions between competing tadpoles, however we could not eliminate the possibility of interference competition via chemical interactions.

Data analysis

Behavioral trials: To analyze the data from our behavioral trials we fit separate generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with a binomial error structure for each of the three measured behaviors: foraging, swimming, or resting (coded as ‘1’ if a behavior was observed at each timepoint and ‘0’ if not). Each model included treatment (control, carnivore added, omnivore added) as a fixed effect, and focal pair ID as a random effect. The mixed models were fit with the glmer function from the {lme4} library [34] in R (version 3.6.1; [35]). We used analysis of deviance to assess the statistical significance of treatment. We ran post-hoc tests using the emmeans function from the {emmeans} library [36] to evaluate the pairwise treatment comparisons.
Competition experiment: To compare the effects of purely exploitative (indirect) from the combined effects of exploitative and interference (direct) competition between the intermediate and specialized tadpoles, we fit a linear model using the lm function in R with the difference in growth of the focal (intermediate morph) tadpoles’ mass minus the growth of their competitor as the response variable. We included competition treatment, competitor type and their interaction as predictors. We used F-ratios and calculated Type 3 sums of squares to assess the statistical significance of these effects. The emmeans function was again used to evaluate the pairwise treatment by competitor type comparisons.

Results

Behavioral trials

We used a behavioral trial to examine how tadpoles intermediate in phenotype would respond to omnivore or carnivore morph tadpoles. Introducing a third tadpole competitor significantly changed the frequency of foraging (χ2= 40.86, P <0.0001), swimming (χ2= 7.16, P = 0.0278), and resting behaviors (χ2= 15.75, P = 0.0003). Post-hoc tests showed that focal tadpoles decreased foraging in the presence of an additional competitor and that this effect was greatest for carnivore competitors (Table 1, Figure 1). In contrast, swimming behavior increased in the presence of carnivores, and only when compared to the control but not the omnivore treatment (Table 1, Figure 1). Finally, the focal tadpoles more frequently rested at the bottom of the tank after the introduction of a second competitor, but this did not differ between omnivore and carnivore competitors (Table 1, Figure 1).

Competition experiment

We aimed to disentangle the effects of interference from exploitative competition using a laboratory microcosm experiment. We found a significant interaction between the competition treatment and competitor type on the growth of our focal intermediate tadpoles (competition treatment: F1,60 = 40.33, P <0.0001; competitor type: F1,60 = 0.00, P = 0.999; treatment x type: F1,60 = 28.14, P <0.0001). Post-hoc tests revealed that the focal tadpoles grew significantly worse than their carnivore competitors when both exploitative and direct interference competition were possible, but that the effects of competition did not differ among the other treatment combinations (Table 2, Figure 2).

Discussion

Resource competition is an important cause of evolutionary diversification [6,37]. Fitness declines when individuals of the same or separate species compete for shared, limited resources. Individuals who exploit alternative resources, however, can escape competition and gain a fitness benefit, driving divergent selection and potentially, evolutionary diversification within and between species [37,38]. Most empirical and theoretical research into this process however, focuses, often implicitly, on the role of exploitative competition. Consequently, how interference competition affects competitively driven disruptive selection and ecological diversification is not well understood [13,14]. We addressed this question in polymorphic spadefoot toad tadpoles (S. multiplicata). First, using a behavioral experiment, we found that the focal phenotypically intermediate tadpoles foraged less when a third tadpole was introduced, and that the introduction of carnivore morph tadpoles caused the greatest decrease in foraging (Figure 1, Table 1). By experimentally manipulating the forms of competition tadpoles experienced, we next found that the focal tadpoles grew more slowly than carnivore morph competitors, but only when they were allowed to interact directly (e.g., when both exploitative and interference competition was possible) compared to when the interference competition was minimized. Together, these results suggest that interference competition plays an important role in the evolution and maintenance of the resource polymorphism in spadefoot toad tadpoles, and perhaps in other systems as well.
Why was competition more intense when our focal tadpoles directly interacted with carnivore competitors (Figure 2, Table 2)? The responses to carnivores in the behavioral experiment point to indirect effects of fear leading to decreased foraging behavior (Figure 1, Table 1). While there was no direct predation within our competition experiment, carnivore morph tadpoles do engage in cannibalism in nature and in experimental settings [39]. In both of our current experiments, it seems likely that focal tadpoles were responding to visual and/or tactile cues of carnivores. Tadpoles in other species respond to these cues in response to both predators and conspecifics [40,41,42,43,44]. While tadpoles [41], including Spea tadpoles [29,39,45,46], also respond to chemical cues, It is unlikely that such cues mediated the responses we found in our experiments since both treatments were exposed to potential chemical cues.
Results from previous studies also suggest that interference and exploitative competition act together in S. multiplicata tadpoles. Spea multiplicata tadpoles exhibit kin recognition and can act altruistically towards kin [29,39,45,47,48]. In a competition experiment manipulating both relatedness and resource availability, tadpoles competed less with siblings than non-siblings when reared together. However, this only occurred when alternative resources were available to them [49], suggesting that unrelated competitors were engaging in interference competition while siblings avoided doing so. It is perhaps surprising then that exploitative competition had little effect on its own in the current competition experiment. Previous studies have found that exploitative competition is generally strong among spadefoot toad tadpoles [5,14,27,31]. Potentially, we found little effect of exploitative competition here either because resources were not limiting in the experiment or because we did not allow individual competitors enough time to exploit their conspecifics’ resources within the exploitative competition treatment.
Fear responses and other forms of interference competition could allow some individuals to monopolize profitable resources and force others to use less preferred alternatives [16]. For Spea tadpoles, interference competition could enable carnivores to discourage competitors from using the more profitable shrimp resource [14,50], in addition to the advantages carnivores already possess in exploitative competition for fairy shrimp [5]. Because the distribution of fairy shrimp within ponds is clumped, carnivore morph tadpoles can behaviorally monopolize this resource, even when carnivores are at relatively low frequency in the population. This in turn could increase competition among less carnivore-like tadpoles for alternative resources - for which phenotypically intermediate tadpoles are poorer competitors compared with omnivore morph tadpoles - and strengthen disruptive selection [14]. Indeed, disruptive selection is both widespread and of strong magnitude in S. multiplicata populations [32]. Fear as a form of interference competition might be expected to be common within resource polymorphisms where cannibalism occurs between morphs, however this possibility has not been widely explored [14,18].
Rather than the fear of predation, how might cannibalism itself affect competition, disruptive selection, and the evolution of resource polymorphism? There are several possibilities. First, by consuming other conspecifics, cannibalism could weaken exploitative competition for resources [18,51,52,53,54]. For example, Spea carnivores tend to target smaller, omnivore morph tadpoles [28,55]. As a consequence, cannibalism in this system might be predicted to reduce competition by providing an additional resource for carnivores and eliminating omnivore competitors. However, by reducing the range of resource-use phenotypes in the population by targeting omnivores, cannibalism might instead increase negative frequency-dependent competition and thereby strengthen disruptive selection [5]. Moreover, within fish planktivore-benthivore resource polymorphisms, cannibalism between age cohorts can stabilize consumer-resource dynamics in populations, thereby promoting the evolution and maintenance of resource polymorphism [18,53,54].
While the role of interference competition in disruptive selection or resource polymorphism is rarely investigated, there is evidence of its potential importance from other systems. For example, the determinants of resource specialization are complex in the oystercatcher, Haematopus ostralegus [56]. Individuals specialize on different prey items (e.g., mussels vs. worms) and use different feeding strategies to specialize within a given prey (e.g. hammering vs. stabbing mussels) [56,57]. Both learning and beak morphology/beak wear contribute to the choice of resource use in oystercatchers [56], yet young and subordinate individuals can be excluded from their preferred, profitable diets by more dominant individuals [58].

Conclusions

If the role of interference competition has been understudied for its potential role in causing disruptive selection in resource use, other areas of research suggest that interference competition could be important in the evolution and maintenance of diversity. Within species, interference competition can, in part, drive fitness tradeoffs between alternative male reproductive strategies, resulting in negative frequency-dependent selection and the maintenance of multiple strategies [59]. Between species, competition for mates, territories or other aggressive interspecific interactions can lead to evolutionary divergence and coexistence (i.e., character displacement) [2,60,61]. And for plants and microbes inhabiting patchy habitats, interference competition can promote greater species diversity [62,63,64,65]. An emergent outcome of these studies and our own suggests that interference competition may be an important, if understudied, mechanism in the evolution and maintenance of inter- and intra-specific variation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.S. and R.A.M.; Methodology, A.S. and R.A.M.; Formal Analysis, R.A.M.; Investigation, A.S. and R.A.M.; Resources, R.A.M.; Data Curation, R.A.M.; Writing – Original Draft Preparation, A.S. and R.A.M.; Writing – Review & Editing, A.S. and R.A.M.

Funding

A.S. was supported by the Romanian-U.S. Fulbright Commission through the Fulbright Visiting Scholar Program (grant no. 626) and by the Romanian Executive Agency for Higher Education, Research, Development and Innovation Funding (UEFISCDI) through the grant PN-III-P1-1.1-TE-2021-1452.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The Case Western Reserve University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approved all methods, and collections were approved by the Arizona Game and Fish Department.

Data Availability

Data is publicly archived on The Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/bnm4h/?view_only=231d270db0b14879982293d33ac82789.

Acknowledgements

We thank the staff and interns at the Southwestern Research Station for their logistical support during this study. Special thanks to Thomas Eldib, Iulian Gherghel and Alice Gadau for field and laboratory assistance and to Sarah Diamond for helpful comments on the manuscript.

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  1. Darwin, C. On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. 1st edition.; John Murray: London, 1859. [Google Scholar]
  2. Pfennig, K.S.; Pfennig, D.W. Character Displacement: Ecological and Reproductive Responses to a Common Evolutionary Problem. Q. Rev. Biol. 2009, 84, 253–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Abrams, P.A.; Matsuda, H.; Harada, Y. Evolutionarily Unstable Fitness Maxima and Stable Fitness Minima of Continuous Traits. Evol. Ecol. 1993, 7, 465–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bolnick, D.I. Can Intraspecfic Competition Drive Disruptive Selection? An Experimental Test in Natural Populations of Sticklebacks. Evolution 2004, 58, 608–618. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  5. Martin, R.A.; Pfennig, D.W. Disruptive Selection in Natural Populations: The Roles of Ecological Specialization and Resource Competition. Am. Nat. 2009, 174, 268–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Rueffler, C.; Van Dooren, T.J.M.; Leimar, O.; Abrams, P.A. Disruptive Selection and Then What? Trends Ecol. Evol. 2006, 21, 238–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Smith, T.B.; Skúlason, S. Evolutionary Significance of Resource Polymorphisms in Fishes, Amphibians, and Birds. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1996, 27, 111–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Rosenzweig, M.L. Competitive Speciation. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 1978, 10, 275–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Maynard Smith, J. Disruptive Selection, Polymorphism and Sympatric Speciation. Nature 1962, 195, 60–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Schluter, D. Adaptive Radiation in Sticklebacks: Trade-Offs in Feeding Performance and Growth. Ecology 1995, 76, 82–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Robinson, B.W.; Wilson, D.S.; Shea, G.O. Trade-Offs of Ecological Specialization: An Intraspecific Comparison of Pumpkinseed Sunfish Phenotypes. Ecology 1996, 77, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Hendry, A.P.; Huber, S.K.; De León, L.F.; Herrel, A.; Podos, J. Disruptive Selection in a Bimodal Population of Darwin’s Finches. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2008, 276, 753–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Dijkstra, P.D.; Border, S.E. How Does Male–Male Competition Generate Negative Frequency-Dependent Selection and Disruptive Selection during Speciation? Curr. Zool. 2018, 64, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Martin, R.A.; Pfennig, D.W. Field and Experimental Evidence That Competition and Ecological Opportunity Promote Resource Polymorphism. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2010, 100, 73–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Dubois, F.; Giraldeau, L.A. Fighting for Resources: The Economics of Defense and Appropriation. Ecology 2005, 86, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bolnick, D.I.; Svanbäck, R.; Fordyce, J.A.; Yang, L.H.; Davis, J.M.; Hulsey, C.D.; Forister, M.L. The Ecology of Individuals: Incidence and Implications of Individual Specialization. Am. Nat. 2003, 161, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Rudolf, V.H.W. The Interaction of Cannibalism and Omnivory: Consequences for Community Dynamics. Ecology 2007, 88, 2697–2705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Andersson, J.; Byström, P.; Claessen, D.; Persson, L.; Roos, A.M.D. Stabilization of Population Fluctuations Due to Cannibalism Promotes Resource Polymorphism in Fish. Am. Nat. 2007, 169, 820–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Brown, J.S.; Laundré, J.W.; Gurung, M. The Ecology of Fear: Optimal Foraging, Game Theory, and Trophic Interactions. J. Mammal. 1999, 80, 385–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Preisser, E.L.; Bolnick, D.I.; Benard, M.E. Scared to Death? The Effects of Intimidation and Consumption in Predator-Prey Interactions. Ecology 2005, 86, 501–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Werner, E.E.; Peacor, S.D. A Review of Trait-Mediated Indirect Interactions in Ecological Communities. Ecology 2003, 84, 1083–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Brock, K.M.; Bednekoff, P.A.; Pafilis, P.; Foufopoulos, J. Evolution of Antipredator Behavior in an Island Lizard Species, Podarcis Erhardii (Reptilia: Lacertidae): The Sum of All Fears? Evolution 2015, 69, 216–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. du Preez, B.; Hart, T.; Loveridge, A.J.; Macdonald, D.W. Impact of Risk on Animal Behaviour and Habitat Transition Probabilities. Anim. Behav. 2015, 100, 22–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Peckarsky, B.L.; Abrams, P.A.; Bolnick, D.I.; Dill, L.M.; Grabowski, J.H.; Luttbeg, B.; Orrock, J.L.; Peacor, S.D.; Preisser, E.L.; Schmitz, O.J.; et al. Revisiting the Classics: Considering Nonconsumptive Effects in Textbook Examples of Predator-Prey Interactions. Ecology 2008, 89, 2416–2425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Sheriff, M.J.; Peacor, S.D.; Hawlena, D.; Thaker, M. Non-Consumptive Predator Effects on Prey Population Size: A Dearth of Evidence. J. Anim. Ecol. 2020, 89, 1302–1316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Pfennig, D. The Adaptive Significance of an Environmentally-Cued Developmental Switch in an Anuran Tadpole. Oecologia 1990, 85, 101–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Pfennig, D.W.; Rice, A.M.; Martin, R.A. Field and Experimental Evidence for Competition’s Role in Phenotypic Divergence. Evolution 2007, 61, 257–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Pomeroy, L.V. Developmental Polymorphism in the Tadpoles of the Spadefoot Toad Scaphiopus Multiplicatus. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of California, Riverside 1981.
  29. Pfennig, D.W.; Frankino, W.A. Kin-Mediated Morphogenesis in Facultatively Cannibalistic Tadpoles. Evolution 1997, 51, 1993–1999. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  30. Pfennig, D.W. Proximate and Functional Causes of Polyphenism in an Anuran Tadpole. Funct. Ecol. 1992, 6, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Pfennig, D.W. Polyphenism in Spadefoot Toad Tadpoles as a Logically Adjusted Evolutionarily Stable Strategy. Evolution 1992, 46, 1408–1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Martin, R.A.; Pfennig, D.W. Widespread Disruptive Selection in the Wild Is Associated with Intense Resource Competition. BMC Evol. Biol. 2012, 12, 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Morey, S.; Reznick, D. A Comparative Analysis of Plasticity in Larval Development in Three Species of Spadefoot Toads. Ecology 2000, 81, 1736–1749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat Softw. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Found. Stat. Comput. 2019.
  36. Lenth, R.V.; Bolker, B.; Buerkner, P.; Giné-Vázquez, I.; Herve, M.; Jung, M.; Love, J.; Miguez, F.; Riebl, H.; Singmann, H. Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, Aka Least-Squares Means 2018.
  37. Schluter, D. The Ecology of Adaptive Radiation; Oxford University Press. 2000.
  38. Pfennig, D.; Pfennig, K. Evolution’s Wedge: Competition and the Origins of Diversity; University of California Press. 2012.
  39. Pfennig, D.W.; Reeve, H.K.; Sherman, P.W. Kin Recognition and Cannibalism in Spadefoot Toad Tadpoles. Anim. Behav. 1993, 46, 87–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Benard, M.F. Predator-Induced Phenotypic Plasticity in Organisms with Complex Life Histories. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2004, 35, 651–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Relyea, R.A. Competitor-Induced Plasticity in Tadpoles: Consequences, Cues, and Connections to Predator-Induced Plasticity. Ecol. Monogr. 2002, 72, 523–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Yu, T.L.; Lambert, M.R. Conspecific Visual Cues: The Relative Importance of Interference and Exploitation Competition among Tadpoles of Rana Kukunoris. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2017, 29, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Rot-Nikcevic, I.; Taylor, C.N.; Wassersug, R.J. The Role of Images of Conspecifics as Visual Cues in the Development and Behavior of Larval Anurans. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2006, 60, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Sutherland, M.; Gouchie, G.; Wassersug, R. Can Visual Stimulation Alone Induce Phenotypically Plastic Responses in Rana Sylvatica Tadpole Oral Structures. J. Herpetol. 2009, 43, 165–1681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Pfennig, D.W. Kin recognition among spadefoot toad tadpoles: a side-effect of habitat selection? Evolution. 1990, 44, 785–798. [Google Scholar]
  46. Pfennig, K.S.; Pfennig, D.W. Dead Spadefoot Tadpoles Adaptively Modify Development in Future Generations: A Novel Form of Nongenetic Inheritance? Copeia 2020, 108, 116–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Pfennig, D.W. Cannibalistic Tadpoles That Pose the Greatest Threat to Kin Are Most Likely to Discriminate Kin. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1999, 266, 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Dugas, M.B.; McCormack, L.; Gadau, A.; Martin, R.A. Choosy Cannibals Preferentially Consume Siblings with Relatively Low Fitness Prospects. Am. Nat. 2016, 188, 124–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Martin, R.A.; Garnett, S.C. Relatedness and Resource Diversity Interact to Influence the Intensity of Competition. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2013, 110, 689–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Frankino, W.A.; Pfennig, D.W. Condition-Dependent Expression of Trophic Polyphenism: Effects of Individual Size and Competitive Ability. Evol. Ecol. Res. 2001, 3, 939–951. [Google Scholar]
  51. Polis, G.A. The Evolution and Dynamics of Intraspecific Predation. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 1981, 12, 225–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Pfennig, D.W.; Sherman, P.W.; Collins, J.P. Kin Recognition and Cannibalism in Polyphenic Salamanders. Behav. Ecol. 1994, 5, 225–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Svanbäck, R.; Persson, L. Individual Diet Specialization, Niche Width and Population Dynamics: Implications for Trophic Polymorphisms. J. Anim. Ecol. 2004, 973–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Persson, L.; Bystrom, P.; Wahlstrom, E. Cannibalism and Competition in Eurasian Perch: Population Dynamics of an Ontogenetic Omnivore. Ecology 2000, 81, 1058–1071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. David W Pfennig Evolution,Development,and Behavior of Alternative Amphibian Morphologies.; Ph.D. Diss.: Univ. Texas, Austin, 1989.
  56. Sutherland, W.J. Why Do Animals Specialize? Nature 1987, 325, 483–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Goss-Custard, J.D.; dit Durell, S.E.A. LE. V. Individual and Age Differences in the Feeding Ecology of Oystercatchers Haematopus Ostralegus Wintering on the Exe Estuary, Devon. Ibis 1983, 125, 155–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Goss-Custard, J.D.; Clarke, R.T.; dit Durell, S.E.A. Le V. Rates of Food Intake and Aggression of Oystercatchers Haematopus Ostralegus on the Most and Least Preferred Mussel Mytilus Edulis Beds of the Exe Estuary. J. Anim. Ecol. 1984, 53, 233–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sinervo, B.; Lively, C.M. The Rock–Paper–Scissors Game and the Evolution of Alternative Male Strategies. Nature 1996, 380, 240–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Grether, G.F.; Losin, N.; Anderson, C.N.; Okamoto, K. The Role of Interspecific Interference Competition in Character Displacement and the Evolution of Competitor Recognition. Biol. Rev. 2009, 84, 617–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Ledón-Rettig, C.C.; Pfennig, D.W. Antipredator Behavior Promotes Diversification of Feeding Strategies. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2012, 52, 53–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Amarasekare, P. Interference Competition and Species Coexistence. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2002, 269, 2541–2550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Greig, D.; Travisano, M. Density-Dependent Effects on Allelopathic Interactions in Yeast. Evolution 2008, 62, 521–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Rendueles, O.; Amherd, M.; Velicer, G.J. Positively Frequency-Dependent Interference Competition Maintains Diversity and Pervades a Natural Population of Cooperative Microbes. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 1673–1681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Czárán, T.L.; Hoekstra, R.F.; Pagie, L. Chemical Warfare between Microbes Promotes Biodiversity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2002, 99, 786–790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Proportion of focal intermediate tadpoles’ behaviors after introduction of an additional omnivore or carnivore competitor. Filled symbols represent mean proportion for each behavior and vertical lines represent 95% confidence intervals from the fitted model. See Table 1 for contrasts.
Figure 1. Proportion of focal intermediate tadpoles’ behaviors after introduction of an additional omnivore or carnivore competitor. Filled symbols represent mean proportion for each behavior and vertical lines represent 95% confidence intervals from the fitted model. See Table 1 for contrasts.
Preprints 69690 g001
Figure 2. Differences in growth rate between intermediate morph focal tadpoles and their competitor. Filled symbols represent predicted mean growth and vertical lines represent 95% confidence intervals from the fitted model. See Table 2 for contrasts.
Figure 2. Differences in growth rate between intermediate morph focal tadpoles and their competitor. Filled symbols represent predicted mean growth and vertical lines represent 95% confidence intervals from the fitted model. See Table 2 for contrasts.
Preprints 69690 g002
Table 1. Post-hoc analyses of the effect of competitor type on tadpole behavior are provided including estimates of the contrast, their standard errors and test statistics and p-values for the pairwise differences between factor levels. Statistically significant p-values at the 0.05 level are indicated in bold font. P-values were adjusted using the false discovery rate for multiple tests. Values are presented to three significant digits.
Table 1. Post-hoc analyses of the effect of competitor type on tadpole behavior are provided including estimates of the contrast, their standard errors and test statistics and p-values for the pairwise differences between factor levels. Statistically significant p-values at the 0.05 level are indicated in bold font. P-values were adjusted using the false discovery rate for multiple tests. Values are presented to three significant digits.
Behavior Contrast Estimate SE z P
Foraging carnivore- control -0.217 0.0349 -6.24 < 0.0001
carnivore- omnivore -0.0685 0.0324 -2.11 0.0347
control- omnivore -0.149 0.0352 4.23 < 0.0001
Swimming carnivore- control 0.0778 0.300 2.58 0.0289
carnivore- omnivore 0.0247 0.309 0.801 0.423
control- omnivore -0.0531 0.029 -1.82 0.102
Resting carnivore- control 0.135 0.0346 3.89 0.003
carnivore- omnivore 0.0448 0.0355 1.26 0.206
control- omnivore -0.0902 0.0339 -2.65 0.0118
Table 2. Post-hoc analyses of the interaction between competitor type and competition treatment are provided including estimates of the contrast, their standard errors and test statistics and p-values for the pairwise differences between factor levels. Statistically significant p-values at the 0.05 level are indicated in bold font. P-values were adjusted using the false discovery rate for multiple tests. Values are presented to three significant digits.
Table 2. Post-hoc analyses of the interaction between competitor type and competition treatment are provided including estimates of the contrast, their standard errors and test statistics and p-values for the pairwise differences between factor levels. Statistically significant p-values at the 0.05 level are indicated in bold font. P-values were adjusted using the false discovery rate for multiple tests. Values are presented to three significant digits.
Contrast Estimate SE t ratio P
e,c – i,c 0.575 0.0906 6.35 < 0.0001
e,c – e,o -0.0000625 0.0906 -0.001 0.999
e,c – i,o -0.104 0.0906 -1.15 0.305
i,c – e,o -0.575 0.0906 -6.35 < 0.0001
i,c – i,o -0.679 0.0906 -7.50 < 0.0001
e,o – i,o -0.104 0.0906 -1.15 0.304
* e = exploitative treatment; i = interference & exploitative treatment; c = carnivore competitor; o = omnivore competitor.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated