Introduction
Darwin [
1] first postulated that natural selection would favor the evolution of trait divergence that minimizes competition between individuals, leading to the evolution of new forms, and even new species. While evolutionary biologists following Darwin generally viewed this as a process occurring via interactions between different populations or species (i.e., ecological and reproductive character displacement; reviewed in [
2]), intense competition for vital resources within populations can also lead to the evolution of intraspecific variation by disruptive selection [
3,
4,
5,
6]. Resource polymorphism ⎯ the occurrence of discrete intraspecific morphs with differential resource use within a population ⎯ is a potential evolutionary outcome of such disruptive selection [
7]. Examples of resource polymorphism are plentiful in many taxa. Nevertheless, the level of divergence between morphs can vary tremendously between polymorphic species. In some cases, phenotypic differences (e.g., morphological, behavioral) can be very subtle and easily overlooked (e.g., pumpkinseed sunfish
Lepomis gibbosus) while in other cases, differences are so dramatic that some morphs were initially described as separate species (e.g., Arctic char
Salvelinus alpinus, tiger salamander
Ambystoma tigrinum, spadefoot toads
Spea multiplicata, and African finches
Pyrenestes ostrinus) (reviewed in [
7]).
Theory and empirical data suggest that intraspecific competition may result in disruptive selection when: (i) phenotypic variation is linked to resource use; (ii) competition is more intense between phenotypically similar individuals using the same limited resources; (iii) competition is density and frequency dependent; and (iv) underused resources are available (i.e. ecological opportunity). Thus, via negative frequency- and density-dependent selection, individuals with extreme resource-use traits that specialize in less common but underused resources exhibit a fitness advantage due to less intense competition with more common phenotypes [
3,
4,
5,
8]. Alternatively, it has also been argued that disruptive selection can also occur when resources are discrete (i.e., bimodal) and functional resource-use tradeoffs between them are strong (e.g., mobile and vulnerable vs. armored and sessile prey), resulting in higher fitness for specialists compared to generalists [
9,
10,
11,
12]. Importantly, these mechanisms can operate together in driving disruptive selection [
5]. Although the role of intraspecific competition in disruptive selection and resource polymorphism is broadly acknowledged, competition can take several forms, a fact mostly disregarded in previous research [
13,
14]. In exploitative competition, individuals compete for shared, limited resources indirectly, while interference competition implies direct competition, involving antagonistic interactions that vary in intensity and consequences. Previous studies on disruptive selection have generally assumed competition to be exploitative or have not distinguished between the possible forms of competition, as exploitative competition is believed to be stronger than interference competition, particularly in less aggressive species that use more dispersed ecological resources [
13,
15]. Nevertheless, it has been recently suggested that interference competition may play a greater role in disruptive selection than previously considered, as it can have rapid effects on competing individuals and could modulate the negative frequency-dependent effects that arise from resource depletion and exploitative competition [
13]. Interference competition may force less aggressive conspecifics to exploit underused resources, particularly with territorial species [
16]. Furthermore, aggressive interactions should be more acute when extreme phenotypes not only compete with other phenotypes, but also predate upon them [
17]. Although cannibalism itself may not contribute greatly to disruptive selection (but see [
18]), the perceived threat of being consumed or injured by a conspecific might have more powerful ecological and evolutionary consequences than previously thought.
Insights can be drawn from studies of predator-prey or intraguild relationships. Predator-prey relations have traditionally been viewed from the perspective of lethality (i.e., predator captures and consumes prey) and density-mediated interactions. However, theoretical and empirical evidence demonstrate that non-consumptive, trait-mediated interactions may have stronger effects on prey fitness than consumptive interactions [
19,
20]. Fear ecology theory thus predicts that the predator affects prey more than just through consumptive interactions, as prey species are under selection to avoid predators [
19]. Indeed, in the presence of predators, prey species or beta-predators (in intraguild relationships) can exhibit considerable phenotypic (i.e. behavioral, physiological, developmental or morphological) alterations to reduce their risk of mortality [
21,
22,
23]. However, these alterations reduce the prey’s overall fitness, as they carry considerable costs, either through the direct ‘cost’ of producing said alterations, or decreases in their reproductive investment and resource acquisition [
24,
25]. A meta-analysis conducted by Preisser et al. [
20] showed that trait-mediated interactions are a major component in predator-prey relationships, comprising at least 50% of the total predator effects, by greatly increasing trophic cascades, and particularly in aquatic environments. While the factor of fear has received much attention in predator-prey and intraguild relations [
20], its role in intraspecific resource competition, and hence in resource polymorphism and disruptive selection, has received little attention despite its potential importance.
Here we experimentally investigated the effects of interference competition between resource-use morphs of the Mexican spadefoot toad (
Spea multiplicata) tadpoles.
Spea tadpoles plastically develop into alternative omnivore or a carnivore morphs in response to diet [
26]. We focused on phenotypically intermediate tadpoles (in relation to the two morphs) that typically have low survival in natural ponds when resource competition is strong, as they are inferior competitors to both omnivore and carnivore morphs [
5,
14,
27]. As intermediate tadpoles both compete with and are potentially consumed by the carnivore morph tadpoles, and omnivores compete with intermediates and are consumed by the carnivores [
5], the system mirrors an inter-specific intraguild relationship. We first ran behavioral trials where we asked if tadpoles would significantly alter their behavior (with regards to resource acquisition – foraging, swimming, or resting) in the presence of carnivore or omnivore tadpoles. We predicted that focal tadpoles would reduce foraging in the presence of carnivore competitors. Finally, we ran a competition experiment, allowing either exploitative or exploitative + interference competition for intermediate tadpoles with either omnivore morph or carnivore morph competitors. We predicted that interference competition would have the greatest effect during competition with carnivore morph competitors.
Materials and Methods
Study system
The Mexican spadefoot toad (
Spea multiplicata) is a nocturnal, predominantly fossorial amphibian species that inhabits arid areas within the southwestern United States and Mexico. They remain underground during the dry season, emerging to feed and breed during the summer monsoons. Breeding within a given ephemeral pond takes place on a single night, and development from egg to metamorphosis can occur in under two weeks [
26,
28]. Their tadpoles display a well-known form of resource polymorphism, as they develop into one of two environmentally-induced ecomorphs: an ‘omnivore’ or ‘carnivore’ morph [
26,
28]. While the former morph is a dietary generalist that feeds predominantly on detritus and algae along with small zooplankton, the latter mostly consumes fairy shrimp, and sometimes other tadpoles, including conspecific ones [
28,
29]). The two morphs dramatically differ in a series of morphological traits, such as body shape, gut length, jaw muscle size, and mouthpart keratinization [
28,
30]. The distribution of phenotypes within a pond is determined by frequency-dependent disruptive selection driven by competition for resources [
5,
31]. While phenotypically intermediate tadpoles exist, they have low survival in ponds with high densities of spadefoot tadpoles [
5,
32]. Disruptive selection disfavors intermediates, as they are outcompeted by the two specialized ecomorphs [
5]. This polymorphism is a plastic response, as the development of a tadpole into a carnivore requires an environmental cue, namely the ingestion of fairy shrimp or other tadpoles [
26,
28].
Behavioral trials
To explore the potential behavioral responses of intermediate tadpoles to interference competition from carnivore tadpoles, we ran a series of experimental behavioral trials. We first collected live
S. multiplicate tadpoles (~10 days old) by dip net from a temporary pond in Price Canyon, Arizona (‘Eagle’s Cry’) and transported them to the live animal holding facility at the Southwestern Research Station in Portal, Arizona. Prior to the experiment we determined whether a tadpole was an omnivore morph, carnivore morph or intermediate by visual inspection of mouthparts [
26]. For each trial in the experiment we introduced two intermediate tadpoles into a plastic tank (412.75 mm x 285.75 mm x 171.45 mm) filled with ~ 10,000 ml of aged water. We offered food in the tank, both in the form of fairy shrimp (
n=10) and ground dry fish food (detritus substitute) and allowed the tadpoles to acclimate for five minutes. We then recorded the tadpoles’ behavior every 30 seconds for five minutes, and logged if they were swimming, feeding or resting. Next we added a carnivore tadpole into the tank and proceeded to record the intermediate tadpoles’ behavior for another five minutes, as previously mentioned. We then removed the carnivore tadpole, rinsed the tank and replaced the water in the tank, reintroduced the intermediate tadpoles, added food, and allowed them to acclimate for another five minutes. Subsequently, we introduced an omnivore tadpole into the tank and recorded the focal tadpoles’ behavior for five more minutes. We ran a total of 21 trials and focal tadpoles were not reused across trials.
Competition experiment
We aimed to disentangle and measure the effects of exploitative competition alone from the combined effects of exploitative and interference competition using a microcosm laboratory experiment. For this experiment, we collected live
S. multiplicata tadpoles (~7 days old) by dip net from a temporary pond near Portal, Arizona (‘Horseshoe’). We again transported them to the live animal holding facility at the Southwestern Research Station in Portal, Arizona. We measured body mass with a digital balance to the nearest 0.001g both prior to and after the experiment, and determined whether a tadpole was an omnivore morph, carnivore morph or intermediate by visual inspection of mouthparts [
26]. On completion of the experiment, all tadpoles were euthanized by immersion in a buffered solution of MS-222 and preserved in 95% ethanol.
In order to disentangle the effects of purely exploitative (indirect) from exploitative and interference (direct) competition between the intermediate and specialized tadpoles, we measured growth as a proxy for fitness during a 10-day experiment, in which intermediate tadpoles competed for food against either omnivore or carnivore tadpoles, in both mixed (interference + exploitative) or single (exploitative) treatments, as follows:
To simulate conditions of both exploitative and interference competition acting together, we reared intermediate tadpoles (
n=32), together with either a carnivore (
n=16), or an omnivore (
n=16) for the entire duration of the experiment. We housed the tadpole pairs of both treatments in plastic tanks (343 mm x 209.5 mm x 120.65 mm) filled with 3250 ml of water. As spadefoot toad tadpoles are known to accelerate development in rapidly evaporating ponds [
33], we inspected water levels daily and topped up when needed in order to maintain the initial volume throughout the experiment. Carnivore treatment tadpole pairs were fed 30 fairy shrimp every 24 hours, while the omnivore treatment pairs were fed 20 mg of ground, dry cichlid pellets (as a substitute for detritus) every 48 hours. We fed the omnivore treatment pairs less frequently, as the consumption of the cichlid food is slower than that of fairy shrimp. Consequently, tadpoles within this treatment competed for food while being exposed to potential antagonistic interactions and/ or fear induced behavioral changes.
For simulating purely exploitative competition, we reared intermediate (n=32), carnivore (n=16) and omnivore (n=16) tadpoles alone in individual plastic tanks. We assigned each intermediate tadpole a specific carnivore (carnivore competition treatment; n=16), or an omnivore (omnivore competition treatment; n=16) tadpole competitor with which they competed for food in an exploitative manner. Specifically, carnivore treatment intermediates and carnivores were each fed 15 fairy shrimp every 24 hours, while omnivore treatment intermediates and omnivore tadpoles were fed 10 mg of dry cichlid food every 48 hours. After the introduction of food, tadpoles were allowed to forage for one hour, after which each tadpole was removed from its tank and introduced in its competitor’s tank, thus having access to the resources remaining after one hour of competitor foraging. Tadpoles thus competed for the same resources without experiencing direct interaction. For this treatment, we filled the tanks with 1625 ml of water, representing half of the volume used in the interference treatment, as only one tadpole was present at any given time in a tank, as opposed to two in the interference treatment. The water level was inspected and maintained constant, as for the previous treatment. It is important to note, that while this design minimized interference competition, it could not remove all potential sources of interference competition. There was no possibility for direct, physical or visual interactions between competing tadpoles, however we could not eliminate the possibility of interference competition via chemical interactions.
Data analysis
Behavioral trials: To analyze the data from our behavioral trials we fit separate generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) with a binomial error structure for each of the three measured behaviors: foraging, swimming, or resting (coded as ‘1’ if a behavior was observed at each timepoint and ‘0’ if not). Each model included treatment (control, carnivore added, omnivore added) as a fixed effect, and focal pair ID as a random effect. The mixed models were fit with the
glmer function from the {lme4} library [
34] in R (version 3.6.1; [
35]). We used analysis of deviance to assess the statistical significance of treatment. We ran post-hoc tests using the
emmeans function from the {emmeans} library [
36] to evaluate the pairwise treatment comparisons.
Competition experiment: To compare the effects of purely exploitative (indirect) from the combined effects of exploitative and interference (direct) competition between the intermediate and specialized tadpoles, we fit a linear model using the lm function in R with the difference in growth of the focal (intermediate morph) tadpoles’ mass minus the growth of their competitor as the response variable. We included competition treatment, competitor type and their interaction as predictors. We used F-ratios and calculated Type 3 sums of squares to assess the statistical significance of these effects. The emmeans function was again used to evaluate the pairwise treatment by competitor type comparisons.
Discussion
Resource competition is an important cause of evolutionary diversification [
6,
37]. Fitness declines when individuals of the same or separate species compete for shared, limited resources. Individuals who exploit alternative resources, however, can escape competition and gain a fitness benefit, driving divergent selection and potentially, evolutionary diversification within and between species [
37,
38]. Most empirical and theoretical research into this process however, focuses, often implicitly, on the role of exploitative competition. Consequently, how interference competition affects competitively driven disruptive selection and ecological diversification is not well understood [
13,
14]. We addressed this question in polymorphic spadefoot toad tadpoles (
S. multiplicata). First, using a behavioral experiment, we found that the focal phenotypically intermediate tadpoles foraged less when a third tadpole was introduced, and that the introduction of carnivore morph tadpoles caused the greatest decrease in foraging (
Figure 1,
Table 1). By experimentally manipulating the forms of competition tadpoles experienced, we next found that the focal tadpoles grew more slowly than carnivore morph competitors, but only when they were allowed to interact directly (e.g., when both exploitative and interference competition was possible) compared to when the interference competition was minimized. Together, these results suggest that interference competition plays an important role in the evolution and maintenance of the resource polymorphism in spadefoot toad tadpoles, and perhaps in other systems as well.
Why was competition more intense when our focal tadpoles directly interacted with carnivore competitors (
Figure 2,
Table 2)? The responses to carnivores in the behavioral experiment point to indirect effects of fear leading to decreased foraging behavior (
Figure 1,
Table 1). While there was no direct predation within our competition experiment, carnivore morph tadpoles do engage in cannibalism in nature and in experimental settings [
39]. In both of our current experiments, it seems likely that focal tadpoles were responding to visual and/or tactile cues of carnivores. Tadpoles in other species respond to these cues in response to both predators and conspecifics [
40,
41,
42,
43,
44]. While tadpoles [
41], including
Spea tadpoles [
29,
39,
45,
46], also respond to chemical cues, It is unlikely that such cues mediated the responses we found in our experiments since both treatments were exposed to potential chemical cues.
Results from previous studies also suggest that interference and exploitative competition act together in
S. multiplicata tadpoles.
Spea multiplicata tadpoles exhibit kin recognition and can act altruistically towards kin [
29,
39,
45,
47,
48]. In a competition experiment manipulating both relatedness and resource availability, tadpoles competed less with siblings than non-siblings when reared together. However, this only occurred when alternative resources were available to them [
49], suggesting that unrelated competitors were engaging in interference competition while siblings avoided doing so. It is perhaps surprising then that exploitative competition had little effect on its own in the current competition experiment. Previous studies have found that exploitative competition is generally strong among spadefoot toad tadpoles [
5,
14,
27,
31]. Potentially, we found little effect of exploitative competition here either because resources were not limiting in the experiment or because we did not allow individual competitors enough time to exploit their conspecifics’ resources within the exploitative competition treatment.
Fear responses and other forms of interference competition could allow some individuals to monopolize profitable resources and force others to use less preferred alternatives [
16]. For
Spea tadpoles, interference competition could enable carnivores to discourage competitors from using the more profitable shrimp resource [
14,
50], in addition to the advantages carnivores already possess in exploitative competition for fairy shrimp [
5]. Because the distribution of fairy shrimp within ponds is clumped, carnivore morph tadpoles can behaviorally monopolize this resource, even when carnivores are at relatively low frequency in the population. This in turn could increase competition among less carnivore-like tadpoles for alternative resources - for which phenotypically intermediate tadpoles are poorer competitors compared with omnivore morph tadpoles - and strengthen disruptive selection [
14]. Indeed, disruptive selection is both widespread and of strong magnitude in
S. multiplicata populations [
32]. Fear as a form of interference competition might be expected to be common within resource polymorphisms where cannibalism occurs between morphs, however this possibility has not been widely explored [
14,
18].
Rather than the fear of predation, how might cannibalism itself affect competition, disruptive selection, and the evolution of resource polymorphism? There are several possibilities. First, by consuming other conspecifics, cannibalism could weaken exploitative competition for resources [
18,
51,
52,
53,
54]. For example,
Spea carnivores tend to target smaller, omnivore morph tadpoles [
28,
55]. As a consequence, cannibalism in this system might be predicted to reduce competition by providing an additional resource for carnivores and eliminating omnivore competitors. However, by reducing the range of resource-use phenotypes in the population by targeting omnivores, cannibalism might instead increase negative frequency-dependent competition and thereby strengthen disruptive selection [
5]. Moreover, within fish planktivore-benthivore resource polymorphisms, cannibalism between age cohorts can stabilize consumer-resource dynamics in populations, thereby promoting the evolution and maintenance of resource polymorphism [
18,
53,
54].
While the role of interference competition in disruptive selection or resource polymorphism is rarely investigated, there is evidence of its potential importance from other systems. For example, the determinants of resource specialization are complex in the oystercatcher,
Haematopus ostralegus [
56]. Individuals specialize on different prey items (e.g., mussels vs. worms) and use different feeding strategies to specialize within a given prey (e.g. hammering vs. stabbing mussels) [
56,
57]. Both learning and beak morphology/beak wear contribute to the choice of resource use in oystercatchers [
56], yet young and subordinate individuals can be excluded from their preferred, profitable diets by more dominant individuals [
58].