Preprint
Article

Effectiveness of Psychomotor Therapy among Children with Graphomotor Impairment with and without DCD-Diagnosis

Altmetrics

Downloads

277

Views

132

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

02 May 2023

Posted:

03 May 2023

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
In Switzerland, psychomotor therapy (PMT) is a standard treatment for children with graphomotor impairments, but scientific evidence of its effectiveness is rare. To investigate the effectiveness of PMT, we conducted a randomized field trial (RFT). The sample consisted of 121 first and second graders with graphomotor impairments, some of whom met the criteria of developmental coordination disorder, while the remaining suffered from developmental dysgraphia. The treatments lasted over 5 months. Handwriting fluency and consistency were measured five times on a digitizing tablet. All participating children completed a self-concept interview, and a standardized fine motor performance test twice. Psychomotor therapy significantly improved the fine motor skills of the therapy group compared to those of the waiting group. However, there was no evidence that the treated children improved more than the waiting children in terms of their graphomotor skills such as frequency, automaticity, and consistency of forming letters. Finally, the children of the therapy group showed partial improvements in their handwriting self-concept, while those of the waiting group children remained stable. This short-term RFT demonstrated the effectiveness of PMT in terms of fine motor skills and some aspects of the handwriting self-concept but showed no effects on handwriting fluency and consistency.
Keywords: 
Subject: Social Sciences  -   Education

1. Introduction

Handwriting research in children

Current scientific models conceptualize handwriting as a complex neuromotor skill, which involves both cognitive and motor processes that are at least partially executed synchronically in a parallel mode [1,2,3]. Furthermore, the interferences between central processes and ongoing movement executions revealed in recent studies demonstrate that handwriting functions in a cascading manner [4,5,6,7,8,9]. As soon as the child learns to write by hand, he or she has to acquire the allographs of the respective language, the corresponding phoneme sounds, and the hand and finger movements needed to execute the allographs. Subsequently, the acquisition of spelling and orthographic coding, as well as the execution of handwriting movements improve continuously to become automated, fluent, and efficient [10,11,12]. Although developmental increases in handwriting speed are well documented [13], research on the development of handwriting fluency in terms of automaticity is less prominent (for exceptions see e.g., [14,15]).
Novice writers slowly write single movements with visual control (i.e., feedback control), and their speed profile shows constant acceleration and deceleration pulses [16]. Due to the physiological limitation of eye tracking movements, this is possible, at most, up to a frequency of approximately 2 hz [17]. On the contrary, skilled writers show only one movement pulse per stroke unit. In analysis, automated movement patterns show a smooth, regular velocity profile with only one velocity reversal per upward and downward stroke. Familiar patterns, allographs, and words are expected to be performed by older children and adults through this much more automated process (feed-forward control) [18,19,20,21]. Advanced handwriting fluency, in terms of increased stroke frequency and automaticity, has been shown to be an important prerequisite for speed in third and fourth graders [15].

Developmental coordination disorder and handwriting difficulties

According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) [22]) and the International clinical practical guidelines [23], developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is diagnosed in children suffering from significant difficulties with motor and fine-motor coordination that are not due to medical or neurological conditions (e.g., muscular dystrophy or cerebral palsy), or intellectual disabilities.
To be diagnosed, the disorder must severely limit the activities of daily living and/or academic achievement, with a negative impact on activities, such as dressing, feeding, and catching a ball, as well as on fine-motor movements, such as handwriting, although handwriting problems can affect academic success in many ways.
Similarly, the International Classification of Diseases ICD [24] characterizes DCD as “a significant delay in the acquisition of gross and fine motor skills and impairment in the execution of coordinated motor skills that manifest in clumsiness, slowness, or inaccuracy of motor performance.” According to the ICD-10, the child’s motor skills are substantially reduced compared to their typically developing peers, causing significant and persistent limitations in functioning since early childhood.
Depending on the cut-off criteria used, the estimated prevalence of DCD amounts to between 3% and 10% of all school-aged children. Many children with DCD have co-occurring conditions such as attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [25], learning difficulties [26], dyslexia [27], or language disorder [28]; however, these conditions do not exclude the DCD diagnosis. Moreover, DCD can co-occur with pervasive developmental disorders (autistic spectrum, Rett syndrome). While the DSM-5 criteria would exclude the DCD diagnosis in this case, the Leeds Consensus Statement [29] argues for a dual diagnosis given that DCD is a separate neurodevelopmental disorder.
DCD presents with a wide variety of impairments, including affected gross-motor skills, balance problems [30,31], impaired fine-motor skills, which emerge among the majority of DCD children at the time when handwriting is acquired [32]. As the proposed research focuses on the therapy of handwriting difficulties among children with DCD, we address these problems in more detail in the following sections.
According to previous studies, most children with DCD have poor handwriting skills [33,34], particularly poor letter arrangements (i.e., writing less legibly), high spatial variability caused by a lack of control of spatial accuracy [35,36,37], or prominent difficulties relating to the temporal aspect of handwriting [38]. Additionally, children with DCD write fewer letters than non-DCD controls when copying [39], show greater disfluency [40], and spend more time pausing compared to non-DCD controls [41].

Developmental dysgraphia without DCD

Hamstra-Bletz and Blote [42] defined developmental dysgraphia (DD) as a difficulty in the production of written language mainly related to the mechanics of handwriting. A broader understanding of DD refers to an impairment of the transcription process of writing; for instance, a child with DD struggles more with retrieving and producing letters and words than their typically developing peers [43]. Accordingly, handwriting difficulties in children with DD are not merely due to (fine-) motor processes. Respective difficulties also rely on cognitive processes (internal representation of letter formation and written words, retrieval of respective items from memory, and spelling) as outlined by McCloskey and Rapp [44]) and by Berninger [45] who titles handwriting as “language by hand”(p. 39). The term developmental dysgraphia refers to a condition in which difficulties arise during the acquisition of handwriting despite opportunities to learn, and in the absence of a known neuropathology or sensory-motor dysfunction.
Children with DD have a wide range of conditions compared to their typically developing peers, especially the inability to form correct letters [46]. By implementing the Dynamic Time Warping (DTW) method, Di Brina et al. [47] found much higher variability in the consistency of letter forms (defined as “the stability in time and space … while the same letter is repeatedly written”, [38](p. 2037), among poor writers, which was independent of other kinematic results of, for instance, larger trajectories or faster movements. Interestingly, the deviation decreased when the children were asked to write faster. Additionally, lack of legibility, dysfluent handwriting [48], erasing and overwriting of letters [49], lack of endurance and velocity [50], and exerting too much pressure on the paper are often found among children with DD.
Prunty and Barnett [46] reported very similar findings for DD and DCD but did not discover a reduced handwriting speed in children with DD and DCD; however, they did report that both groups paused for a longer time during free writing compared to the typically developing controls. One reason for the longer pauses could be a strong spelling-motor interaction in children with DD given that difficulties with spelling automaticity negatively affect word writing [51].
In a Swiss study among 60 children attending school-based PMT and suffering from graphomotor difficulties [52,53] only 46% of the involved children fulfilled the cut-off criteria for DCD (M-ABC-2 below the 16th percentile) with respect to motor impairment, which is consistent with the results of Overvelde and Hulstijn [54], who found 17% of the Dutch second graders to be dysgraphic, a prevalence that is much higher than that for DCD.

Consequences of poor handwriting

Irrespective of its aetiology, poor handwriting has a negative impact on the physical and emotional well-being of children [49] as well as on their academic achievement (e.g., as poor handwriting is slow, these children have difficulty completing academic tasks within a given time). According to Graham et al. [55], lower order skills (transcription) interfere with higher order skills (translational processes); thus, children with poor handwriting compose shorter and worse texts compared to their typically developing peers [56]. Even if the text quality is comparable, low legibility is associated with worse ratings of text quality by teachers [57].

Therapeutic approaches for children with DCD

Over the last 15 years, several evidence-based interventions for children with DCD have been developed and published, mostly focusing on the training of specific tasks [58].
Neuromotor task training (NTT) is related to motor learning theories and focuses on the interaction between the child, task, and environment. The child choses their preferred task, the therapist teaches via observation, precise feedback, and appropriate variation of the task and environment [59,60]. The Cognitive Orientation to Daily Occupational Performance programme (CO-OP) is another approach that has been shown to be successful by using cognitive problem-solving strategies to learn targeted skills [61]. The cognitive motor approach of Henderson and Sugden [62] has been extended by including the educational systems around the child and is termed ecological intervention (EI). Further, there is some evidence that neuropsychological treatments like motor imaginary training [63] help to develop the automatization of movements.

Therapeutic approaches for children with handwriting difficulties

According to the current German-Austrian-Swiss health care guidelines on the definition, diagnosis, treatment, and psychosocial aspects of circumscribed developmental disorders of motor functions [64], therapies should be based on an individualised planning. In addition, for severe graphomotor impairment, a medical examination is necessary to exclude neurological diseases, such as tremor, that might cause similar problems, and to decide whether direct task training is purposeful. Notably, the therapy should help children to cope with the tasks of daily life; therefore, the training must focus on what is important for the child and their family.
In the case of graphomotor development, parents and teachers mostly recognise the importance of handwriting but place too much emphasis on neatness [65]. Broader objectives should be the improvement in movement skills, including an increase in handwriting fluency and automaticity of written letterforms, increased self-confidence, and motivation for written communication, and to play and express oneself by painting, drawing, and writing [66,67]. The individualised setting of reachable goals and the self-evaluation of the process are efficient features of therapy title [68] and [67](pp. 41–44). It is important to involve teachers to establish commitment and identify appropriate methods for both schools and therapy [69].
As this study investigates the situation in the German-speaking part of Switzerland, PMT should be briefly explained.
PMT is influenced by different approaches and theoretical concepts [70] (pp. 32). According to Eggert and Lütje-Klose [71], among the hypotheses of therapeutic effects, because of the training of the targeted skills, not only the direct improvement has to be considered, but also the transfer effects due to the improvement in executive functions and the children’s experiences of stabilization in their personality owing to improved self-confidence after successful motor learning. Therefore, the therapists often prefer the child-centred approach by Zimmer [72], which places an emphasis on the improvement of competencies and self-competence [73]. Regarding handwriting, graphomotor skills are trained to allow children to express themselves by drawing and writing [74]. In Switzerland, first graders learn a print letter alphabet and are taught to develop their own personal fluent and legible handwriting over the following years [75] based on evidence-based teaching material, which is also partly used in therapy. Progress in better automated, more fluent, and more legible handwriting may also improve achievement at school, largely because better automated skills use less working memory capacities and more resources are released for text production [76].
PMT is part of the school-based services for children with special needs provided by the cantonal ministries of education. In contrast to medical therapeutic measures, PMT is generally paid by the state and not health assurances; therefore, there is no need to obtain a medical diagnosis of DCD in advance. Therapists understand themselves as being part of the educational system and cooperate regularly with teachers [77]. Children usually attend PMT in a specialized room close to their classroom, and in some cases, the therapist will also work in the classroom. Sessions are scheduled during school time, in single settings or in small groups [78].
In summary, PMT is well-known and widely accepted in the field of Swiss special needs education, but only a few studies of its effectiveness have been conducted [79,80]. Particularly, there is a lack of scientific evidence with respect to the effectiveness of PMT in treating the handwriting difficulties of children with DCD or DD despite its nationwide school-based provision in Switzerland. Conversely, several foreign studies have considered the effectiveness of occupational therapy among children with DCD [81]. Therefore, we conducted an experimental field study to investigate the probable improvements of fine-motor and handwriting skills among children with DCD and DD attending a 16-week course of PMT and compared their results to those of a waiting group. With respect to handwriting, we focused on the course characteristics of the respective improvements, including the consistency of written letters and dynamic patterns, such as the automaticity and fluency of handwriting.
According to the theoretical considerations outlined above, we examined the following hypotheses: 1) PMT for children with DCD and DD improves their fine-motor skills compared to the children in the control group; 2) PMT for children with DCD and DD continually improves their handwriting skills with respect to the frequency of up and down strokes, automaticity, and consistency of forming letters compared to the children in the control group; and 3) PMT for children with DCD and DD continually improves their handwriting self-concept compared to the children in the control group.

2. Materials and Methods

Design

To compare the developmental course of children with and without therapy, we employed a randomized waiting-control-group design with pre-, peri-, and post-test measurements. The sample included a therapy group (children attending therapy) and a control group (children waiting for therapy). The participants were first and second graders (n = 121) who were randomly assigned to the therapy or control group (Figure 1). Handwriting process measures were assessed five times, while self-concept and fine motor skills were only assessed twice at pre- and post-test. Over a 3-year period, we examined three independent cohorts. The assessments for each cohort started in August and ended in the following February; therefore, we studied the effectiveness of the treatments over 16 weeks.
The children of the therapy group attended between 9 and 18 therapy sessions (M = 12.7) carried out by experienced psychomotor therapists, with each session lasting 45 min. The therapists reported their interventions using a structured therapy protocol [53,82].
The children of the control group did not attend any therapy sessions, or any other service related to fine motor training, but received the usual handwriting lessons at school.
In February, after the end of the study, the children of the control group were also allowed to start therapy. Regarding the therapy group, the therapists decided on the continuation of the therapy depending on the needs of the particular child.
Figure 1. Study design.
Figure 1. Study design.
Preprints 72496 g001

Participants

All participating children (n = 121) were recruited by psychomotor therapists employed by the city of Zurich. The children had been referred for PMT for the first time due to graphomotor impairments. The sample corresponded to 7.6% of all first and second graders registered in Zurich for a psychomotor assessment because of graphomotor difficulties in the years 2018–2021. To assign the children to the DCD- or the DD-group, the therapists used the German version of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-Ed. 2 (M-ABC-2) [83], a translated version of the DCD- Questionnaire 2007 (DCDQ’07) [84,85] and anamnestic data. Children assigned to the DCD-group met the criteria of ICD-10 yielding a M-ABC-2-score below the 16th percentile, as well as experienced handwriting problems that were serious enough to interfere with their academic performance and social integration. Moreover, their motor performance was poorer than expected given their chronological age and did not arise from neurological disease or mental retardation. A medical check-up was conducted for the children of the DCD group by the medical health services of the City of Zurich or by the families’ pediatrician to guarantee the fulfilment of the ICD-10 exclusion criteria. Children with M-ABC-2- scores on the 16th percentile and greater who also showed graphomotor problems were assigned to the DD group. ADHD or mild learning problems were not used as exclusion criteria. Through a random allocation procedure, the participating children were assigned to the therapy and control groups. Active consent for the participation in the study was given by all parents of the participating children.
The sample consisted of 48 first graders (39.7%) and 73 second graders (60.3%), among whom 74.4% were boys, 87.6% were righthanders, reflecting the general distribution of children attending PMT, 34.7% met the criteria of DCD, and 65.3% showed signs of DD without meeting the criteria of DCD. The proportion of children with DCD was higher among the first graders (58.3%) compared to the second graders (30.1%; χ2 = 9.50, df = 1, p =.002). The mean age measured at t1 was 7 years and 2 months (SD = 7 months, ranging from 6 years to 8 years 9 months).
During the study, one child broke his arm before t4 and had to be excluded for the fine motor and graphomotor examinations of t4 and t5. One child had to be excluded from the second fine motor test because he had started a medical treatment for ADHD before t5. Six children missed the 2nd fine motor test and the self-concept evaluation due to scheduling problems.
Table 1. Description of the sample.
Table 1. Description of the sample.
Therapy Group Control group
Variable n % n % n % χ2 p
121 100 61 50.4 60 49.6
Sex .068 n.s.
- Female 31 25.6 15 24.6 16 26.7
- Male 90 74.4 46 75.4 44 73.3
Handedness .743 n.s.
- Righthanders 106 87.6 55 90.2 51 85
- Lefthanders 15 12.4 6 9.8 9 15
Diagnosis .752 n.s.
- DCD 42 34.7 22 36.1 20 33.3
- DD 79 65.3 39 63.9 40 66.7
Class .089 n.s.
- First graders 48 39.7 25 41 23 38.3
- Second graders 73 60.3 36 59 37 61.7

Material

Handwriting movements were recorded using writing tablets (Wacom Intuos PRO-medium tablet) connected to a notebook. All tasks were written down with the “Wacom Inking Pen” KP130 - an induction pen with ballpoint refill. An extended version of the software CSWin DTW [86] with CSWin DTW plugin [87](Marquardt et al 2021), with a recording frequency of 200 Hz and an accuracy of 0.1 mm in the x- and y-axes, was used for recording and analysis. For the calculation and smoothing of the velocity and acceleration signals, non-parametric regression methods (kernel estimation) were included in the mathematical calculation procedures of CSWin [88].

Procedure

There were 24 trained psychomotor therapists involved, working in the city of Zurich. They treated between one and four children per cohort, whereby some therapists were involved in all three cohorts and others only in one or two cohorts. The therapists did not receive information about the results in between the study cohorts, only at the end. After study completion, an information event was used to collect the therapists’ comments and interpretations on the revealed results.
The tablet recordings were conducted approximately every 4 weeks by trained university staff. Each child was examined individually in a separate room of the school building or in the therapy room. The child sat beside the test administrator. In front of the child was the Wacom tablet with the special pen and a sheet of paper attached to the surface of the tablet. The sequence of the 20-min examination was predetermined by the pre-programmed task sequence of the software. The original items of CSWin have been extended within several studies [15,53,89] to include tasks that are typical for the stages of handwriting development. All participants performed 15 digital handwriting items in the same order, including basic graphomotor movements.
Table 2. Items of handwriting measurement.
Table 2. Items of handwriting measurement.
Item No. Task Item No. Task
1 Scribbling (for trying out, not evaluated) 8 Repetitive letter sequences (writing at least 8 times the letter “a,” no speed specification)
2 Finger movements (no speed specification) 9 Repetitive letter sequences (writing at least 8 times the letter “a,” as fast as possible)
3 Finger movements (fast) 10 Repetitive letter sequences (writing at least 8 times the letter “a,” as precisely as possible)
4 Wrist movements (no speed specification) 11 Patterns (garlands)
5 Wrist movements (fast) 12 Patterns (double loops)
6 Combined finger and wrist movements when circling (no speed specification) 13 Copying the word “neu” (new) three times
7 Combined finger and wrist movements when circling (fast) 14 Copying a sentence “Die Kinder fliegen nach Amerika» («The children fly to America», no speed specification)
15 Copying a sentence “Die Kinder fliegen nach Amerika» («The children fly to America», fast)
For reasons of efficiency, we have limited the statistical analyses to the faster, and usually better, second attempt of basic movements and omitted the first trial and the two difficult patterns (e.g., garlands and double loops cf. the evaluated tasks in the white fields).
The basic movements were directly demonstrated by the experimenter and the children were allowed to try them out on a laminated card; afterwards, the task was performed twice (the second one as fast as possible) to ensure the best possible performance based on the combined visual and tactile-kinaesthetic information.
With respect to writing repetitive letters sequences, many children were not capable of reproducing valid recognisable letters within the set time. Consequently, we offered (starting with measurement point 3) that the letter trace was first retraced with the finger on a laminated card with an enlarged a-shape to ensure that the correct sequence could be reproduced successfully without interruption.
The remaining tasks were presented visually using instruction cards, with ComicSansSerif used as the font for the text. The children were asked to use the writing type that they had learnt at school. Unstructured white paper was used for all items given that, according to Quenzel and Mai [20], visual guidelines have a negative influence on writing speed. Only in the case of the tasks with repetitive letter sequences, was a discrete visual structure in the form of light grey bars provided for measurement purposes. Observations on validity, pen postures, and other difficulties were noted and used to clean the dataset. If a task was solved incorrectly (e.g., if it was aborted too early), a maximum of one repetition was allowed.

Measures

Fine-motor performance

To measure the fine-motor performance, the German version [90] of the Brunininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency BOT-2 [91] was administered by the therapists themselves before the first measurement and after the 5th measurement. This is a standardised, norm-referenced, individually administered measure, specifically of fine manual control, manual coordination, body coordination, and strength and agility, which is often used in clinical and school practice settings. For the purpose of this study, subtest 1 (fine motor precision) and subtest 2 (fine motor integration) were used, both of which were combined in the fine manual control scale. The tests were blindly evaluated by the research assistant.

Process-based handwriting measures

Velocity: Stroke frequency (FREQ) refers to the number of upward and downward strokes per second. To calculate these strokes, the written trace is divided in subsequent up and down segments by CSWin [92]. This measure seems more appropriate than assessing the absolute writing speed (mm/s) as the speed will directly depend on a person’s individual writing size. Children with handwriting difficulties have been found to perform slowly but steadily improve throughout PMT [52]. Additionally, the data provide an insight into the level of motor control already achieved; thus, while visually controlled movements show up in a stroke frequency of approximately 2 hz or lower, values greater than this indicate that handwriting is performed by sufficiently automated movements [17,93]
Automaticity: We measured automaticity by the number of inversions in velocity (NIV). The NIV indicates the average number of velocity changes occurring within writing strokes. In the optimal case, the velocity profile is unimodal (acceleration followed by deceleration), resulting in a value of NIV = 1. A fluent adult handwriter requires nearly one velocity change per stroke (acceleration followed by deceleration) resulting in an NIV score that is close to 1 [88]. Children with handwriting difficulties demonstrate a much higher NIV, indicating a substantial lack of automaticity [52].
Dynamic time warping (DTW): The digital time normalization DTW is a method used for the pattern comparison of different sequences of values to calculate relative difference measures. The DTW analysis for writing [94,95] compares the spatial and temporal similarity of repeatedly written traces. Di Brina et al. [47] used the DTW method to compare the shape of written letters to analyse the spatial properties of the handwriting of children with writing problems. The calculated DTW distance is the average point-to-point distance between the respective written letters and the individually calculated prototype of this letter with a normated size of 1. To better understand the distribution of the deviation, the percentages of coherent letters (named as percentage coherence, d < 0.05) and deviant letters (named as percentage deviation, d > 0.1) are calculated [86,87].

Handwriting self-concept

To assess the handwriting self-concept, an extended version of an instrument we had developed and used previously [53,96]. The children were interviewed by the therapists regarding their self-concept over eight aspects. They were asked whether they considered their handwriting to be nice, fluent, loose, and legible; if they use correct letter sequences; if they feel secure when writing; and if they write with joy and are satisfied with the product. The children were asked to use a token that they positioned on a six-step staircase made of building blocks, where the higher the step the more positive their estimation. The therapists noted the answers on the related 6-point scale. Within this study we used the scale twice, once at the pre-test and once at the post-test.

Therapy aims and therapy protocols

The 61 children attending PMT weekly from August to January were treated according to individual goals set at the beginning. All therapeutic interventions were recorded in terms of content and time by means of a therapy protocol [82]; this yielded between 10 and 18 protocols per child, indicating the interventions chosen per session and the time spent on them. Within each focus area, the therapist indicated the selected sub-areas. The protocol dataset includes 671 protocols referring to comprehensive information on treatment contents and procedures [82].

Data analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 28. For the standardized tests (BOT-2), transformed T-values were used in the analysis.
To examine the intervention effects on fine motor control, a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) using time as a repeated factor, group and diagnosis as between factors, and grade as a covariate was employed.
Because of the young age of our sample and the resulting difficulties of many children with certain handwriting tasks, the respective data contain a significant number of missing data (indicating that a child was unable to perform the task at this trial). In addition, many of the handwriting process variables were skewed and not normally distributed (particularly the NIV) and therefore did not meet the requirements of the traditional ANOVA approach. Consequently, we calculated generalized estimating equations (GEE), which are designed to handle missing data and are also suitable for non-normal distributed variables given that the missing data were distributed completely at random (MCAR) [97,98]. When the MCAR requirement was not fulfilled (in addition to the non-normal distribution), we decided to perform separate non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon) for the therapy and waiting groups, comparing t1 vs. t5 only. The rational of the Wilcoxon analyses was to detect a significant change over time (t1 vs. t5) in one group but not in the other, indicating that there was a group difference.
Similarly to the process variables described above, the handwriting self-concept variables were not normally distributed, and the MCAR-criterion was not met. Therefore, we employed Wilcoxon tests as described previously.

3. Results

3.1. Fine motor skills

The 2 (group) x 2 (diagnosis) x 2 (time) repeated measures ANOVA revealed two significant main effects and one significant two-way interaction (Table 3). Firstly, children with DCD performed poorer in fine manual control than children with DD. Secondly, the therapy group performed better than the waiting group taking both time points into account. Thirdly, children attending therapy improved in fine manual control over time, while this was not the case among the children of the waiting group who stagnated at the initial level (time*group interaction: F = 28.74, df = 1/108, p =.000, Eta2=.210). All other main or interaction terms turned out to be non-significant (including the three-way interaction).
Table 3. Fine-manual control (T-values) from t1 to t5 by group and diagnosis.
Table 3. Fine-manual control (T-values) from t1 to t5 by group and diagnosis.
Therapy
group
Control
group
DD DCD DD DCD Time Group Diagnosis Time*
Group
Time *
Diagnosis
Measures M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
F
df
Eta2
p
F
df
Eta2
p
F
df
Eta2
p
F
df
Eta2
p
F
df
Eta2
p
t1 39.40
(1.43)
31.65
(1.89)
37.41
(1.41)
33.79
(1.96)
2.699
1/108
.024
n.s.
7.190
1/108
.062
.008
7.563
1/108
.065
.007
28.74
1/108
.210
.000
3.082
1/108
.028
.082
t5 45.81
(1.49)
40.38
(1.96)
34.98
(1.46)
34.64
(2.04)
Notes: The covariates in the model were calculated using the following values: Grade = 1.61 (n.s.). n = 113.
Figure 2. (a) Fine manual control (Brunininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency BOT-2, T-values) among children with Developmental Dysgraphia (DD); (b) Fine manual control (Brunininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency BOT-2, T-values) among children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD).
Figure 2. (a) Fine manual control (Brunininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency BOT-2, T-values) among children with Developmental Dysgraphia (DD); (b) Fine manual control (Brunininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency BOT-2, T-values) among children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD).
Preprints 72496 g002

3.2. Process-based handwriting results

In the following sections, we analyse how the process-based handwriting measures develop from t1 to t5 in both groups by means of the GEE procedure if the MCAR condition is fulfilled. If this requirement is not met, we analyse by means of nonparametric Wilcoxon tests only taking t1 and t5 into account. The means and standard deviations for all measures from t1 to t5 are reported in Table A1 and Table A2 in the Appendix, as well as missing data due to invalid attempts, which were more prevalent among the basic movements and over the first trials (t1 and t2).
GEE analyses: The GEE analyses revealed several handwriting fluency (FREQ) improvements over time for both groups; this was the case regarding fast finger movements (Wald-Chi2 = 10.5, df = 4, p <.05), repetitive letter sequences (without speed specification) (Wald-Chi2 = 30.8, df = 4, p <.001), copying a word (Wald-Chi2 = 101.6, df = 4, p <.001), and copying a sentence (without speed specification) (Wald-Chi2 = 116.1, df = 4, p <.001). The same analyses did not reveal any group effects and, with one exception, any group*time interactions. The exception was an unexpected group*time interaction with respect to repetitive letter sequences (without speed specification), indicating more improvement among the waiting group compared to the therapy group (Wald-Chi2 = 10.7, df = 4, p <.05).
Non-parametric analyses (Wilcoxon): Regarding handwriting fluency (FREQ), we found a time effect for both groups for repetitive letter sequences (as fast as possible) (therapy-group: z = –4.41, p <.001; waiting group: –4.23, p <.001). Only the waiting group but not the therapy group improved from t1 to t5 in wrist movements (fast) (z = –2.532, p =.011), combined finger and wrist movements when circling (z = –2.621, p =.009), and repetitive letter sequences (precisely) (z = 4.493, p <.001).
In contrast, the therapy group improved in automaticity (NIV) in combined finger and wrist movements (fast) (z = –2.634, p =.008).
Also with respect to automaticity, both groups improved from time 1 to time 5 in repetitive letter sequences (without speed specification) (therapy-group: z = –2.026, p =.043; waiting group: z = –4.163, p <.001), repetitive letter sequences (as fast as possible) (therapy-group: z = –4.597, p <.001; waiting group: z = –3.959, p <.001), repetitive letter sequences (as precisely as possible) (therapy-group: z =–2.121, p =.034; waiting group: z = –4.289, p <.001), in copying a word (therapy-group: z = –4.030, p <.001; waiting group: z = –5.103, p <.001), in copying a sentence without speed specification (therapy-group: z = –5.278, p <.001; waiting group: z = –5.530, p <.001), and under fast condition (therapy-group: z = –3.818, p <.001; waiting group: z = 4.803, p <.001).
Regarding the measures of DTW, for the criteria of distance, consistency, and deviance, we did not find any change over time for both groups, among all three conditions (repetitive letter sequences without speed specification, as fast as possible, and as precisely as possible), with one exception: Children of the waiting group wrote more coherent letters at t5 compared to t1 when writing repetitive letter sequences as fast as possible (z=-2.359, p= .018), whereas the children of the therapy group did not improve in this respect.
Despite several improvements over time (as reported above), a narrower inspection of the performance at t5 made it clear that, depending on the task, the children did not achieve the target of sufficient frequency (i.e., a frequency of 2 hz or greater). More than 75% of all children managed the basic movements in an automated manner, while in the remaining tasks 70–100% of the children were not able to do so (Figure 4).
Figure 3. Performance regarding frequency over all items at t5 (freq.3 = Finger movements (fast); freq.5 = Wrist movements (fast); freq.7= Combined finger and wrist movements (fast); freq.8 = Repetitive letter sequences (no speed specification); freq.9 = Repetitive letter sequences (as fast as possible); freq.10 = Repetitive letter sequences (as precisely as possible); freq.13 = Copying the word “neu”; freq.14 = Copying a sentence (no speed specification); freq.15 = Copying a sentence (fast). Notes: The bar at 2 hz marks the threshold at which controlled movement changes to automated execution [17].
Figure 3. Performance regarding frequency over all items at t5 (freq.3 = Finger movements (fast); freq.5 = Wrist movements (fast); freq.7= Combined finger and wrist movements (fast); freq.8 = Repetitive letter sequences (no speed specification); freq.9 = Repetitive letter sequences (as fast as possible); freq.10 = Repetitive letter sequences (as precisely as possible); freq.13 = Copying the word “neu”; freq.14 = Copying a sentence (no speed specification); freq.15 = Copying a sentence (fast). Notes: The bar at 2 hz marks the threshold at which controlled movement changes to automated execution [17].
Preprints 72496 g003

3.3. Handwriting-self-concept

Because some of the children did not answer all questions, the number of participants of each item differs slightly (range: 108–114 children).
Compared to the baseline assessment (t1), the children of the therapy group rated their handwriting as more beautiful (z = –2.70, p =.007), more legible (z = –2.24, p =.025), more skilful in terms of letter sequences (z = –2.26, p =.024), and were more satisfied with their handwriting (z = –2.41, p =.016) at t5. In contrast, the respective ratings did not change over time among the waiting group children. Regarding the remaining self-concept variables, the ratings of both groups did not change over time.

4. Discussion

We found a significant treatment effect with respect to fine-motor control. PMT significantly improved fine motor skills in children with DCD and DD over a 5-month period compared to those in the waiting group regardless of diagnosis.
In contrast, we found no evidence, that the treated children improved more than the waiting children (regardless of diagnosis DD or DCD) with respect to their graphomotor skills, such as fluency (frequency), automaticity, and consistency of forming letters over the 16-week period, but we did find several time effects for both groups.
In relation to the self-concept of handwriting, the treated children rated some aspects better at t5 compared to t1, while the ratings of the children of the waiting group remained stable over time.
The results regarding fine motor control are in line with the fact that PMT often starts to work on fine motor development as a precursor skill to writing by hand, e.g., the strengthening and mobility of the fingers is built up in this way [74]. The reasons for the increase in fine motor skills still need to be substantiated by the differentiated analysis of the protocol data, but they seem comprehensible. As the initial results of our therapy protocol analyses reveal [82], the focus was indeed on directly handwriting-related precursor motor skills such as pen posture and finger movement control (25% of the total therapy time). Additionally, children usually respond very well to the game-centred approach of PMT to promote fine motor skills. The approach superficially allows for attractive choices and seems less school-related and less performance-oriented than training graphomotor skills; therefore, it can be worked with a high level of intrinsic motivation, as recommended by the medical guidelines [64].
When it comes to the treatment of graphomotor difficulties, task-oriented therapy approaches are assumed to be more successful than process-oriented ones [58]. The initial results of our therapy protocol analysis [82] demonstrated that the pencil-and-paper-based promotion of visuomotor skills (11%) and direct handwriting training (15.5%) took up a quarter of all training units. Therefore, our zero finding regarding handwriting skills is unexpected, especially given the fact that even short-term task-based interventions improve handwriting fluency among struggling and typically developing young handwriters [99]. These findings may have several explanations.
First, the duration of some therapies did not reach the intended duration in the 16 weekly sessions, as there were failures due to illness, holidays, and school projects. With at least 10 sessions among the shortest interventions, this was just below the recommended threshold of guidelines. According to the meta study of Smits-Engelsmann et al. [58], most of the investigated interventions that were successful lasted longer than 10 weeks.
Second, during the intervention time, the pencil was used daily in class and explicit handwriting was taught and practiced several times a week. Therefore, the teaching effects probably supressed the therapy effects.
Third, learning processes in handwriting are known as non-linear; for example, children with ADHD initially show deterioration in their handwriting fluency when they obtain the perfect mix of medication and therapeutical treatment, at which time they are finally able to focus and learn, and therefore tend to write slower and in a less automated manner [100]. This could be the case here as well. When children learn to focus during the first therapy weeks, they will be ready to learn, even in terms of handwriting, but the increase will not be immediately visible. Fortunately, most of PMTs last longer, so some starting difficulties or even regressions can be absorbed. Due to ethical constraints, it was not possible to extend the duration of the study time for each cohort, even though the usual therapies last longer.
Furthermore, it is possible that these children were too young for a purely task-oriented approach to promoting graphomotor skills. Fine and gross motor movement opportunities, such as those available in the therapy room, are more in line with the fundamental need for play at this age. It is known that children who lacked play opportunities for a variety of reasons show a pronounced need to catch up [101] (p.254), [102]. However, targeted work on handwriting corresponds less to the child's intrinsic motivation but is usually a concern of parents and teachers. The therapists cannot resolve this conflict of goals, at most they can steer it in a constructive direction by making agreements with the children, and they rely on the fact that playing creates an essential foundation for further development.
Regarding our data so far, we can conclude that all the children were making progress in terms of handwriting, but their performance at t5 was still far below the target range. As normally developed second graders can write common short words in a fast and almost automatized manner [103], the children in our sample showed a lower speed (below 2 hz), indicating controlled fine motor steering even at t5 over all items that were more complex than basic movements. According to the cognitive load theory [104] this lack of automaticity is unfavorable because children need to master basic handwriting and spelling skills in a fluent way to obtain more free resources in their working memory for the higher demands of writing.
The newly created DTW tasks unfortunately turned out to be too difficult for many children. Compared to the performance of a pilot study, many children of this sample of struggling handwriters were unable to write a sufficient number of valid letters in the given time or to reproduce non-recognizable or wrong letter forms. Additionally, due to the teaching material used in many classes, the children had surprisingly no experience with lowercase letters, not even with a common “a.” Therefore, our zero finding should be considered with caution. More research using a simpler task is necessary to come to a more comprehensive conclusion.
A comparison with the performance of normally developing second graders [103] can reveal areas in which children with DCD or DD still require therapy and whether this can lead to values in the target range in the longer term. Further research is necessary to analyse the outcome of the therapy, which will be possible due to the therapy protocols that are planned to be examined in detail next.
The finding that the children in therapy improved their self-concept more than the waiting group children is not surprising and in line with our respective hypothesis as PMT has an explicit focus on individual progresses that are discussed at several instances with the child [105,106]. Regarding the Reciprocal Effect Model [107], in which self-concept and performance influence each other, this improvement is important for the further course of therapy.
Finally, we emphasize that even regarding these limitations, the increase in a positive self-concept is a crucial first step to gain more joy in writing and motivation for a therapy that may simply take more time..

5. Conclusions

The present study successfully demonstrates that PMT improves fine-motor skills, which are assumably prerequisites of handwriting, among young children, as well as improves some aspects of the handwriting self-concept, which is expected to support further learning and training. Thus, despite the short-term limitations of this study, our results provide scientific evidence for the current PMT services in Switzerland.
As handwriting acquisition implies complex learning processes, PMT can be considered a long-term endeavor. Our study children who attended PMT for half a year remain below the level of automatized handwriting movements and clearly need additional therapeutic support. However, given the limited results with respect to handwriting fluency and consistency, more research, taking longer time periods into account to observe respective improvements, is necessary.

Limitations

Due to challenges associated with the pandemic, the data collection time was extended from two to three cohorts to meet the required sample size. Although there was no complete data loss, the precise rhythm of monthly examinations was difficult to fulfil e.g., due to illness of children, therapist, or teachers.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, W.W. and S.H.; methodology, W.W. and S.H.; software, C.M. and C.B.; validation, S.H., M.N. and C.B.; formal analysis, W.W.; investigation, S.H., M.N. and S.W.; data curation, S.H.; writing—original draft preparation, S.H., W.W. and M.N.; writing—review and editing.; S.H., W.W., A.B., C.M., C.B., S.W. and M.N.; visualization, S.H. and M.N.; supervision, W.W.; project administration, M.N. and S.W.; funding acquisition, W.W., S.H. and A.B.

Funding

Research was funded by the SWISS NATIONAL SCIENCE FOUNDATION, grant number 100019_185467 / 1+2, and the APC is funded by ChronosHub/SNF. Check carefully that the details given are accurate and use the standard spelling of funding agency names at https://search.crossref.org/funding. Any errors may affect your future funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study according to the ethical guidelines and regulations of the Swiss Psychological Society (https://www.swisspsychologicalsociety.ch/ ).”

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are openly available. Hurschler Lichtsteiner, Sibylle, Nideröst, Melanie, Wicki, Werner & Wyss, Stefanie (2023). Wirksamkeit Psychomotoriktherapie Grafomotorik (2019-2022) (1.0.0) [Dataset]. FORS Datenservice. https://doi.org/10.48573/gka9-vq56

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participating children, the head of the psychomotricity department of the city of Zurich, all participating therapists, support services and teachers.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix

Table A1. Process-based handwriting descriptives: Frequency and automaticity.
Table A1. Process-based handwriting descriptives: Frequency and automaticity.
t1 t2 t3 t4 t5
Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group
Measures M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD)
FREQ 3 2.41 (.99) 2.29 (.94) 2.53 (1.03) 2.61 (.96) 2.72 (.97) 2.50 (.95) 2.73 (1.06) 2.78 (1.02) 2.69 (1.12) 2.77 (.97) 2.80 (.94) 2.74 (1.02) 2.69 (.83) 2.76 (.82) 2.62 (.85)
NIV 3 1.87 (1.05) 1.90 (.99) 1.84 (1.28) 1.68 (.94) 1.52 (.78) 1.84 (1.06) 1.47 (.77) 1.43 (.82) 1.51 (.69) 1.51 (.94) 1.38 (.65) 1.65 (1.17) 1.57 (1.52) 1.43 (.96) 1.74 (1.99)
FREQ 5 3.44 (1.00) 3.60 (.98) 3.26 (1.01) 3.71 (.98) 3.86 (1.00) 3.55 (.94) 3.69 (1.01) 3.73 (1.04) 3.65 (.99) 3.68 (1.11) 3.79 (1.11) 3.58 (1.10) 3.67 (.96) 3.70 (.87) 3.64 (1.05)
NIV 5 1.26 (.99) 1.21 (1.09) 1.31 (.87) 1.12 (.33) 1.13 (.31) 1.11 (.35) 1.11 (.26) 1.10 (.19) 1.12 (.32) 1.51 (.45) 1.17 (.55) 1.13 (.34) 1.10 (.31) 1.09 (.37) 1.11 (.24)
FREQ 7 2.62 (.96) 2.65 (.98) 2.58 (.95) 2.75 (.96) 2.89 (.96) 2.62 (.95) 2.84 (.90) 2.92 (.81) 2.75 (.98) 2.83 (.97) 2.95 (.92) 2.71 (1.01) 2.80 (.88) 2.83 (.81) 2.77 (.94)
NIV 7 1.86 (1.58) 1.82 (1.55) 1.90 (1.63) 1.75 (1.73) 1.61 (1.55) 1.89 (1.89) 1.47 (.83) 1.39 (.85) 1.56 (.81) 1.70 (1.78) 1.49 (1.20 1.91 (2.20) 1.47 (.84) 1.43 (.64) 1.52 (1.02)
FREQ 8 1.12 (.38) 1.20 (.42) 1.06 (.32) 1.33 (.49) 1.32 (.52) 1.34 (.46) 1.32 (.44) 1.24 (.38) 1.42 (.49) 1.42 (.54) 1.35 (.49) 1.50 (.58) 1.38 (.51) 1.27 (.45) 1.50 (.55)
NIV 8 5.47 (2.37) 5.06 (2.15) 5.86 (2.53) 4.57 (2.76) 4.85 (1.07) 4.26 (2.37) 4.13 (2.10) 4.41 (1.99) 3.82 (2.20) 4.05 (2.64) 4.38 (2.71) 3.71 (2.55) 4.17 (2.54) 4.50 (2.45) 3.79 (2.61)
FREQ 9 1.70 (.54) 1.74 (.55) 1.66 (.54) 2.00 (.68) 1.99 (.71) 2.01 (.65) 0.99 (.39) 1.89 (.74) 2.07 (.76) 2.05 (.76) 2.09 (.77) 2.01 (.76) 2.15 (.76) 2.08 (.69) 2.22 (.83)
NIV 9 3.03 (1.67) 2.91(1.54) 3.15 (1.81) 2.51 (1.49) 2.51 (1.48) 2.51 (1.52) 2.54 (1.52) 2.57 (1.30) 2.50 (1.77) 2.46 (1.69) 2.27 (1.23) 2.65 (2.06) 2.15 (1.12) 2.12 (1.06) 2.19 (1.20)
FREQ 10 0.89 (.28) 0.90 (.31) 0.87 (.24) 1.09 (.41) 1.10 (.45) 1.07 (.38) 0.99 (.39) 0.95 (.37) 1.05 (.42) 1.07 (.42) 0.98 (.36) 1.16 (.45) 1.10 (.41) 1.01 (.38) 1.21 (.43)
NIV 10 8.03 (3.51) 8.09 (3.80) 7.98 (3.23) 6.22 (3.21) 6.24 (3.43) 6.19 (2.99) 7.11 (3.82) 7.29 (3.42) 6.89 (4.28) 6.53 (4.12) 7.30 (4.74) 5.71 (3.17) 6.22 (3.71) 6.97 (4.08) 5.40 (3.07)
FREQ 13 1.03 (.45) 1.08 (.44) 0.99 (.46) 1.16 (.56) 1.21 (.57) 1.11 (.56) 1.25 (.56) 1.23 (.54) 1.26 (.60) 1.37 (.63) 1.39 (.59) 1.35 (.67) 1.45 (.63) 1.46 (.63) 1.43 (.65)
NIV 13 6.98 (5.99) 6.49 (6.20) 7.50 (5.76) 5.99 (4.98) 5.60 (4.43) 6.40 (5.49) 5.30 (4.03) 4.99 (3.06) 5.63 (4.85) 4.53 (3.68) 4.11 (2.79) 4.94 (4.37) 3.98 (3.44) 3.74 (2.53) 4.23 (4.17)
FREQ 14 1.15 (.52) 1.15 (.48) 1.14 (.56) 1.36 (.59) 1.37 (.58) 1.34 (.60) 1.46 (.61) 1.46 (.57) 1.45 (.65) 1.57 (.69) 1.60 (.65) 1.56 (.74) 1.57 (.59) 1.57 (.49) 1.57 (.68)
NIV 14 5.41 (3.57) 5.20 (3.60) 5.64 (3.56) 4.50 (3.59) 4.36 (3.17) 4.64 (3.99) 3.97 (3.28) 3.67 (2.23) 4.27 (4.07) 3.42 (2.21) 3.17 (1.82) 3.68 (2.54) 3.32 (2.41) 3.04 (1.86) 3.61 (2.85)
FREQ 15 1.63 (.68) 1.64 (.71) 1.63 (.65) 1.78 (.72) 1.85 (.76) 1.72 (.68) 1.90 (.80) 1.97 (.81) 1.82 (.78) 2.00 (.84) 2.06 (.77) 1.95 (.90) 2.07 (.86) 2.03 (.82) 2.11 (.90)
NIV 15 3.42 (2.58) 3.56 (2.93) 3.28 (2.18) 2.91(2.01) 2.83 (2.06) 3.00 (1.96) 2.78 (2.22) 2.59 (1.96) 2.98 (2.46) 2.50 (1.76) 2.28 (1.50) 2.72 (1.97) 2.37 (1.57) 2.36 (1.61) 2.38 (1.55)
Notes: Descriptive statistics of writing characteristics. Legend: 3 = Finger movements (fast); 5 = Wrist movements (fast); 7= Combined finger and wrist movements (fast); 8 = Repetitive letter sequences (no speed specification); 9 = Repetitive letter sequences (as fast as possible); 10 = Repetitive letter sequences (as precisely as possible); 13 = Copying the word “neu” freq.; 14 = Copying a sentence (no speed specification) freq.; 15 = Copying a sentence (fast). The sample varied because of invalid attempts: Item 3 range n = 59–100, Item 5 range n = 98–115, Item 7 range n = 105–116, Item 8 range n = 69–100, Item 9 range n = 77–100, Item 10 range n = 79–108; Item 13 range n = 113–120, Item 14 range n = 116–121, Item 15 range n = 91–114.
Table A2. Process-based handwriting descriptives: Measures of dynamic time warping.
Table A2. Process-based handwriting descriptives: Measures of dynamic time warping.
t1 t2 t3 t4 t5
Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group Total Therapy group Waiting group
Measures M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
DTW 8 Distance 0.08
(.04)
0.08
(.03)
0.09
(.04)
.08
(.33)
.08
(.03)
.09
(.04)
.08
(.03)
.08
(.03)
.08
(.03)
.08
(.08)
.08
(.02)
.08
(.04)
.08
(.07)
.08
(0.3)
.08
(.03)
DTW 8 Consistency 35.57
(22.44)
37.95
(21.99)
33.26
(22.95)
29.81
(24.04)
31.92
(20.43)
27.48
(20.24)
29.91
(17.63)
32.09 (19.11) 27.49
(15.73)
30.81
(21.02)
32.92
(21.97)
28.59
(20.03)
31.33
(19.47)
34.11
(20.14)
28.19
(18.39)
DTW 8
Deviance
20.05
(18.05)
20.24
(18.42)
19.86
(17.94)
23.51
(18.79)
22.58
(16.92)
24.53
(20.86)
22.23
(17.62)
22.23 (19.40) 22.23
(15.67)
21.37
(17.36)
19.56
(16.29)
23.27
(18.42)
21.30
(20.27)
19.93
(16.99)
22.84
(23.52)
DTW 9 Distance 0.10
(.05)
0.09
(.04)
0.10
(.05)
.09
(.04)
.09
(.03)
.09
(.04)
.10
(.04)
.10
(.04)
.10
(.40)
.10
(.04)
.09
(.03)
.10
(.04)
.10
(.03)
.09
(.03)
0.10
(0.3)
DTW 9 Consistency 26.17
(18.21)
27.25
(21.50)
26.17
(18.21)
23.89
(18.24)
24.63
(18.70)
23.89
(18.24)
20.13
(16.01)
23.84 (19.00) 20.13
(16.01)
20.19
(15.83)
22.85
(18.14)
20.19
(15.83)
19.19
(14.67)
22.58
(18.42)
19.19 (
14.67)
DTW 9
Deviance
28.75
(21.68)
26.77
(22.02)
28.75
(21.68)
32.01
(26.05)
28.48
(21.33)
32.01
(26.05)
31.93
(22.52)
29.59 (24.94) 31.93
(22.52)
32.44 (
22.79)
29.82
(21.59)
32.44
(22.79)
32.68
(20.37)
27.84
(21.25)
32.68
(20.37)
DTW 10 Distance 0.10
(.05)
0.08
(.04)
0.10
(.05)
.09
(.04)
.08
(.03)
.09
(.04)
.08
(.05)
.08
(.04)
.08
(.05)
.09
(.04)
.08
(.04)
.09
(.04)
.08
(.03)
.07
(.03)
0.08
(.03)
DTW 10 Consistency 25.76
(19.27)
35.25
(24.94)
25.76
(19.27)
29.16
(17.79)
31.54
(23.39)
29.16
(17.79)
29.61
(19.88)
32.66 (21.50) 29.61
(19.88)
25.96
(16.00)
34.90
(22.07)
25.96
(16.00)
29.13
(17.75)
36.53
(20.76)
29.13
(17.75)
DTW 10
Deviance
27.46
(23.44)
22.47
(19.18)
27.46
(23.44)
25.71 (
19.92)
17.72
(16.05)
25.71
(19.92)
20.08
(22.23)
19.89 (20.57) 20.08
(22.23)
24.23
(19.80)
19.06
(19.38
24.23
(19.80)
23.79
(18.97)
16.32
(15.98)
23.79
(18.97)
Notes: Descriptive statistics of DTW writing characteristics. Legend: 8 = Repetitive letter sequences (no speed specification); 9 = Repetitive letter sequences (as fast as possible); 10 = Repetitive letter sequences (as precisely as possible). The sample varied because of invalid attempts: Item 8 range n = 69–100, Item 9 range n = 77–100, Item 10 range n = 79–108.

References

  1. Kandel, S.; Peereman, R.; Grosjacques, G.; Fayol, M. For a psycholinguistic model of handwriting production: Testing the syllable-bigram controversy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2011, 37, 1310–1322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. van Galen, G.P. Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory. Hum. Mov. Sci. 1991, 10, 165–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. van Galen, G.P.; Meulenbroek, R.G.J.; Hylkema, H. On the simultaneous monitoring of words, letter and strokes in handwriting: Evidence for a mixed linear and parallel model. In Graphonomics: Contemporary research in handwriting; Kao, H.S.R., van Galen, G.P., Hoosain, R., Eds.; Elsevier Science Ltd: Amsterdam, New York, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  4. Delattre, M.; Bonin, P.; Barry, C. Written spelling to dictation: sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 2006, 32, 1330–1340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Fayol, M.; Lété, B. Contributions of online studies to understanding translation from ideas to written text. In Translation of thought to written text while composing: Advancing theory, knowledge, methods, and applications; Fayol, M., Alamargot, D., Berninger, V.W., Eds.; Psychology Press: East Sussex, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  6. Kandel, S.; Peereman, R.; Ghimenton, A. Further evidence for the interaction of central and peripheral processes: the impact of double letters in writing English words. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Olive, T.; Alves, R.A.; Castro, S.L. Cognitive processes in writing during pause and execution periods. Europe Journal of Cognitive Psychology 2009, 21, 758–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Olive, T.; Kellogg, R.T. Concurrent activation of high- and low-level production in written composition. Memory & Cognition 2002, 30, 594–600. [Google Scholar]
  9. Roux, S.; McKeff, T.J.; Grosjacques, G.; Afonso, O.; Kandel, S. The interaction between central and peripheral processes in handwriting production. Cognition 2013, 127, 235–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. 1Abbott, R.D.; Berninger, V.W.; Fayol, M. Longitudinal relationships of levels of language in writing and between writing an reading in grades 1 to 7. Journal of Educational Psychology 2010, 102, 281–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. 1Bosga-Stork, I.M.; Bosga, J.; Meulenbroek, R. Intentional control and biomechanical exploitation in preparatory handwriting. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2011, 30, 687–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bosga-Stork, I.M.; Bosga, J.; Meulenbroek R, G.J. Developing movement efficiency between 7 and 9 years of age. Motor Control 2014, 18, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Graham, S.; Berninger, V.W.; Weintraub, N.; Schafer, W. Development of handwriting speed and legibility in grades 1-9. The Journal of Educational Research 1998, 92, 42–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Meulenbroek, R.G.J.; van Galen, G.P. Foreperiod duration and the analysis of motor stages in a line-drawing task. Acta Psychologica 1988, 69, 19–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Wicki, W.; Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S.; Saxer Geiger, A.; Müller, M. Handwriting fluency in children: Impact and correlates. Swiss Journal of Psychology 2014, 73, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Nottbusch, G. Handschriftliche Sprachproduktion. Sprachstrukturelle und ontogenetische Aspekte; Max Niemeyer: Tübingen, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  17. Marquardt, C.; Gentz, W.; Mai, N. Visual control of automated handwriting movements. Experimental Brain Research 1999, 128, 224–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Fitzpatrick, P.; Vander Hart, N.; Cortesa, C. The Influence of Instructional Variables and Task Constraints on Handwriting Performance. The Journal of Educational Research 2013, 106, 216–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Khalid, P.I.; Yunus, J.; Adnan, R. Extraction of dynamic features from hand drawn data for the identification of children with handwriting difficulty. Research in Developmental Disabilites 2010, 31, 256–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Quenzel, I.; Mai, N. Kinematische Analyse von Schreibbewegungen im Erstschreibunterricht. Unterrichtswissenschaft : Zeitschrift für Lernforschung 2000, 28, 290–303. [Google Scholar]
  21. Rosenblum, S.; Parush, S.; Weiss, P.L. Computerized temporal handwriting characteristics of proficient and non-proficient handwriters. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 2003, 57, 129–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Association, A.P. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5 ®), 5th; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, D. C, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  23. Blank, R.; Barnett, A.L.; Cairney, J.; Green, C.; Kirby, A. ; Polatajko, H…; Rosenblum, S.; Smits-Engelsman, B.C.M.; Sudgen, D.; Wilson, P.; et al. International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2019, 242–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. WHO. Internationale Klassifikation psychischer Störungen. ICD-10 Kapitel V (F). Diagnostische Kriterien für Forschung und Praxis; Hogrefe: Bern, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  25. Visser, L.; Röschinger, J.; Barck, K.; Büttner, G.; Hasselhorn, M. Learning Difficulties in Children with Symptoms of DCD And/or ADHD: Analyses from a Categorical and a Continuous Approach. International Journal of Disability Development and Education 2022, 69, 1505–1521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gomez, A.; Piazza, M.; Jobert; Dehaene-Lambertz, G. ; Dehaene, S.; Huron, C. Mathematical difficulties in developmental coordination disorder: Symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing. Research in developmental disabilities 2015, 43-44, 167–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Biotteau, M.; Péran, P.; Vayssière, N.; Tallet, J.; Albaret, J.-M.; Chaix, Y. Neural changes associated to procedural learning and automatization process in Developmental Coordination Disorder and/or Developmental Dyslexia. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2017, 21, 286–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Visser, J. Developmental coordination disorder: a review of research on subtypes and comorbidities. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2003, 22, 479–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Leeds Consensus Statement: Developmental Coordination Disorder as a Specific Learning Difficulty; Sugden, D.A. (Ed.) DCD-UK/Dyscovery Centre: Leeds, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  30. Jover, M.; Schmitz, C.; Centelles, L.; Chabrol, B.; Assaiante, C. Anticipatory postural adjustments in a bimanual loadlifting task in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2010, 52, 850–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Asonitou, K.; Koutsouki, D.; Kourtessis, T.; Charitou, S. Motor and cognitive performance differences between children with and without developmental coordination disorder (DCD). Research in Developmental Disabilities 2012, 33, 996–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Vaivre-Douret, L.; Lalanne, C.; Ingster-Moati, I.; Boddaert, N.; Cabrol, D.; Dufier, J.L.; Golse, B.; Falissard, B. Subtypes of Developmental Coordination Disorder: Research on Their Nature and Etiology. Developmental Neuropsychology 2011, 36, 614–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Missiuna, C.; Rivard, L.; Pollock, N. They’re Bright but Can’t Write: Developmental Coordination Disorder in school aged children. TEACHING Exceptional Children Plus 2004, 1. [Google Scholar]
  34. Biotteau, M.; Danna, J.; Baudou, E.; Puyjarinet, F.; Velay, J.L.; Albaret, J.M.; Chaix, Y. Developmental coordination disorder and dysgraphia: signs and symptoms, diagnosis, and rehabilitation. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2019, 15, 1873–1885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Smits-Engelsman, B.C.M.; van Galen, G.P. Dysgraphia in children: Lasting psychomotor deficiency or transient developmental delay? Journal of experimental child psychology 1997, 67, 164–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Rosenblum, S.; Livneh-Zirinski, M. Handwriting process and product characteristics of children diagnosed with developmental coordination disorder. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008, 27, 200–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Huau, A.; Velay, J.-L.; Jover, M. Graphomotor skills in children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD): Handwriting and learning a new letter. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2015, 42, 318–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Bo, J.; Colbert, A.; Lee, C.-M.; Schaffert, J.; Oswald, K.; Neill, R. Examining the relationship between motor assessments and handwriting consistency in children with and without probable Developmental Coordination Disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2014, 35, 2035–2043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Prunty, M.M.; Barnett, A.L.; Wilmut, K.; Plumb, M. Handwriting speed in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: Are they really slower? Research in developmental disabilities 2013, 34, 2927–2936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Jolly, C.; Huron, C.; Albaret, J.M.; Gentaz, É. Analyse comparative des tracés de lettres cursives d’une enfant atteinte d’un trouble d’acquisition de la coordination et scolarisée en CP avec ceux d’enfants ordinaires de GSM et de CP. Psychologie Française, 2010, 55, 145–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Prunty, M.M.; Barnett, A.L.; Wilmut, K.; Plumb, M. An examination of writing pauses in the handwriting of children with developmental coordination disorder. Research in developmental disabilities 2014, 35, 2894–2905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hamstra-Bletz, L.; Blote, A.W. A longitudinal study on dysgraphic handwriting in primary school. Journal of Learning Disabilities 1993, 26, 689–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Berninger, V.W. Understanding the “Graphia” in Developmental Dysgraphia. In Developmental motor disorders: A neuropsychological perspective; Dewey, D., Tupper, D.E., Eds.; Guilford press: New York, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  44. McCloskey, M.; Rapp, B. Developmental dysgraphia: An overview and framework for research. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2017, 34, 65–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Berninger, V.W.; Abbott, R.D.; Abbott, S.P.; Graham, S.; Todd, R. Writing and reading: Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities 2002, 35, 39–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Prunty, M.M.; Barnett, A.L. Understanding handwriting difficulties: A comparison of children with and without motor impairment. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2017, 34, 205–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Di Brina, C.; Niels, R.; Overvelde, A.; Levi, G.; Hulstijn, W. Dynamic time warping: A new method in the study of poor handwriting. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2008, 27, 242–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Rosenblum, S.; Weiss, P.L.; Parush, S. Product and process evaluation of handwriting difficulties. Educational Psychology Review 2003, 15, 41–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Engel-Yeger, B.; Nagauker-Yanuv, L.; Rosenblum, S. Handwriting Performance, Self-Reports, and Perceived Self-Efficacy Among Children With Dysgraphia. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 2009, 63, 182–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Graham, S.; Struck, M.; Santoro, J.; Berninger, V.W. Dimensions of good and poor handwriting legibility in first and second graders: Motor programs, visual-spatial arrangement, and letter formation parameter setting. Developmental Neuropsychology. Special Issue on Writing 2006, 29, 43–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kandel, S.; Lassus-Sangosse, D.; Grosjacques, G.; Perret, C. The impact of developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia on movement production during word writing. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2017, 34, 219–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Wicki, W.; Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S. Improvement of handwriting automaticity among children treated for graphmotor difficulties over a period of six months. Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention 2018, 11, 148–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S.; Wicki, W. Kinematische Untersuchung der Handschrift mit STREGA CSWin: Ein Verfahren zur Förderplanung und zur Wirksamkeitsforschung. Empirische Sonderpädagogik 2017, 4, 406–425. [Google Scholar]
  54. Overvelde, A.; Hulstijn, W. Handwriting development in grade 2 and grade 3 primary school children with normal, at risk, or dysgraphic characteristics. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2011, 32, 540–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Graham, S.; Berninger, V.W.; Abbott, R.D.; Abbott, S.P.; Whitaker, D. Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology 1997, 89, 170–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Prunty, M.M.; Barnett, A.L.; Wilmut, K.; Plumb, M. The impact of handwriting difficulties on compositional quality in children with developmental coordination disorder. British Journal of Occupational Therapy 2016, 79, 591–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Graham, S.; Harris, K.R.; Hebert, M. It Is More Than Just the Message: Presentation Effects in Scoring Writing. Focus on Exceptional Children 2011, 44, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Smits-Engelsman, B.C.M.; Blank, R.; van der Kaay, A.-C.; Mosterd - van der Meijs, R.; Vlugt - van der Brand, E.; Polatajko, H…; Wilson, P. H. Efficacy of interventions to improve motor performance in children with developmental coordination disorder: a combined systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2013, 55, 229–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Niemeijer, A.S. Neuromotor Task Training: physiotherapy for children with developmental coordination disorder. Dissertation; Groningen, Rijkuniversiteit, Groningen, 2007.
  60. Schoemaker, M.M.; Niemeijer, A.S.; Reynders, K.; Smits-Engelsman, B.C.M. Effectiveness of Neuromotor Task Training for Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: A Pilot Study. Neural Plasticity 2003, 10, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Polatajko, H.J.; Mandich, A.D. Enabling Occupation in Children. The Cognitive Orientation to Daily Occupational Performance (CO-OP) Approach; CAOT Publications ACE: Ottawa, ON, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  62. Henderson, S.E.; Sugden, D.A. Ecological Intervention for Children with Movement Difficulties. Pearson, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  63. Adams, I.L.J.; Lust, J.M.; Steenbergen, B. Development of motor imagery ability in children with developmental coordination disorder - A goal-directed pointing task. British Journal of Psychology 2018, 109, 187–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Blank Rainer, Vinçon, Sabine. Deutsch-österreichisch-schweizerische (DACH) Versorgungsleitlinie zu Definition, Diagnostik, Behandlung und psychosozialen Aspekten bei Umschriebenen Entwicklungsstörungen motorischer Funktionen (UEMF), 2020.: Kurzfassung. Available online: https://www.awmf.org/uploads/tx_szleitlinien/022-017k_S3_Umschriebene-Entwicklungsstoerungen-motorischer-Funktionen-UEMF_2020-08_01.pdf.
  65. Graham, S. Want to improve children’s handwriting? Don’t neglect their handwriting. American Educator 2009, 20–40. [Google Scholar]
  66. Berninger, V.W.; Vaughan, K.B.; Abbott, R.D.; Abbott, S.P.; Rogan, L.W.; Brooks, A.; Reed, E.; Graham, S. Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers: Transfer from handwriting to composition. Journal of Educational Psychology 1997, 89, 652–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Fuchs, M. Grafomotoriktherapie mit jüngeren und mit älteren Kindern - eine Fallstudie.
  68. Jongmans, M.J.; Linthorst-Bakker, E.; Westenberg, Y.; Smits-Engelsman, B.C.M. Use of a task-oriented self-instruction method to support children in primary school with poor handwriting quality and speed. Human Movement Science. Special Issue: Developmental Coordination Disorder: Mechanisms, Measurement and Management 2003, 22, 549–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Vetter, M. Bildungspolitische Entwicklungen in der Schweiz und die Organisation des Angebotes der Psychomotorik in der Stadt Zürich: Ein nachahmenswerter Prozess? motorik 2016, 39, 19–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Kuhlenkamp, S. Lehrbuch Psychomotorik; Ernst Reinhardt: München, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  71. Eggert, D.; Lütje-Klose, B. Theorie und Praxis der psychomotorischen Förderung, 7., verb. Aufl.; Borgmann: Dortmund, 2008; ISBN 3861453029. [Google Scholar]
  72. Zimmer, R. Handbuch der Psychomotorik. Theorie und Praxis der psychomotorischen Förderung von Kindern; Herder: Freiburg i.B, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  73. Vetter, M.; Sandmeier, A. Psychomotricity: Effects of psychomotor interventions from the perspective of teachers. European Psychomotricity Journal 2020, 30–42. [Google Scholar]
  74. Häusler, M. Spielen, zeichnen, schreiben. Grafomotorik - das Schreibenlernen verstehen, Lernschwierigkeiten behandeln. In Psychomotorik-Therapie und individuelle Entwicklung; Buchmann, T., Ed.; Schweizerische Zentralstelle für Heilpädagogik (SZH): Luzern, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  75. Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S. Erfahrungen mit der Basisschrift. Eine grosse Erleichterung - und wie weiter? Die Grundschulzeitschrift 2013, 265/266, 28–31. [Google Scholar]
  76. Sturm, A.; Nänny, R.; Wyss, S. Entwicklung hierarchieniedriger Schreibprozesse. In Handbuch Schriftspracherwerb und weiterführendes Lesen und Schreiben; Philipp, M., Ed.; Beltz Juventa: Weinheim, 2017; ISBN 978-3-7799-3130-0. [Google Scholar]
  77. Jucker, D. Die Professionalisierung der Psychomotoriktherapie in der Deutschschweiz; Universität, Zürich, 2012.
  78. Psychomotorik-Therapie und individuelle Entwicklung; Buchmann, T. , Ed.; Schweizerische Zentralstelle für Heilpädagogik (SZH): Luzern, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  79. Kranz, I.; Sammann, K.; Amft, S.; Vetter, M. Effekte einer grafomotorisch ausgerichteten psychomotorischen Intervention bei Kindern im Vorschulalter. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht 2011, 58, 139–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Avet L’Oiseau-Tissot, S.; Senn, B.; Balligand Lecompte, K. Evaluation des effets de la thérapie psychomotrice: Résultats de recherche principaux. Revue suisse de pédagogie spécialisée 2011, 2, 48–53. [Google Scholar]
  81. Hoy, M.M.P.; Egan, M.Y.; Feder, K.P. A systematic review of interventions to improve handwriting. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy 2011, 78, 13–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Wyss, S.; Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S.; Wicki, W. Wirksamkeit der Psychomotoriktherapie: Sekundäranalyse Therapieziele und Protokolle. Zwischenbericht. Forschungsgruppe Sprachen und Schrift., 2022. Available online: https://www.phlu.ch/7050.
  83. Petermann, F. Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (M-ABC-2; deutsche Version); Pearson Assessment: Frankfurt/M, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  84. Wilson, B.N.; Crawford, S.G.; Green, D.; Roberts, G.; Aylott, A.; Kaplan, B. Psychometric Properties of the Revised Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics 2009, 29, 182–202. [Google Scholar]
  85. Kennedy-Behr, A.; Wilson, B.N.; Rodger, S.; Mickan, S. Cross-cultural adaptation of the developmental coordination disorder questionnaire 2007 for German-speaking countries: DCDQ-G. Neuropediatrics 2013, 44, 245–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Marquardt, C. CSWin DTW. Computerunterstützte Analyse der Bewegungsabläufe beim Schreiben. Bedienungshandbuch; Verlag MedCom: München, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  87. Marquardt, C., Di Brina, C., Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S., Nideröst, M., Wicki, W. CSWin DTW plugin; Pädagogische Hochschule Luzern, 2021.
  88. Marquardt, C.; Mai, N. A computational procedure for movement analysis in handwriting. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 1994, 52, 39–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S.; Wicki, W.; Falmann, P. Impact of handwriting training on fluency, spelling and text quality among third graders. Reading and Writing 2018, 31, 1295–1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Blank, R.; Jenetzky, E.; Vinçon, S. Bruininks-Oseretzky Test der motorischen Fähigkeiten. 2. Ausgabe. Deutschsprachige Version; Pearson: Frankfurt a.M, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  91. Bruininks, R.H.; Bruininks, B.D. Brunininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency (BOT-2), 2nd; Pearson Assessment: Minneapolis, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  92. Mai, N.; Marquardt, C. CSWin - computergestützte Analyse der Bewegungsabläufe beim Schreiben. Bedienungshandbuch; MedCom: München, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  93. Freund, H.J. , Büttner, U., Cohen, B., Noth, J. Time control of hand movements: (1986). Progress in Brain Research 64, 287–294. Progress in Brain Research 1986, 64, 287–294. [Google Scholar]
  94. Niels, R. Allograph based writer identification, handwriting analysis and character recognition. PhD; University, Radboud, Nijmegen, 2010.
  95. Vuori, V. Adaptive methods for on-line recognition of isolated handwritten characters. PhD; Technology, University of, Helsinki, 2002.
  96. Hurschler, S.; Saxer, A.; Wicki, W. Schreibmotorische Leistungen im frühen Primarschulalter in Abhängigkeit vom unterrichteten Schrifttyp. Forschungsbericht Nr. 18 der Pädagogischen Hochschule Zentralschweiz, Luzern; PHZ Luzern: Luzern, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  97. Hubbard, A.E.; Ahern, J.; Fleischer, N.L.; van der Laan, M.; Lippman, S.A.; Jewell, N.; Bruckner, T.; Satariano, W.A. To GEE or not to GEE: comparing population average and mixed models for estimating the associations between neighborhood risk factors and health. Epidemiology 2010, 21, 467–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Ghisletta, P.; Spini, D. An Introduction to Generalized Estimating Equations and an Application to Assess Selectivity Effects in a Longitudinal Study on Very Old Individuals. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics 2004, 29, 421–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. López-Escribano, C.; Martín-Babarro, J.; Pérez-López, R. Promoting Handwriting Fluency for Preschool and Elementary-Age Students: Meta-Analysis and Meta-Synthesis of Research From 2000 to 2020. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 841573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Tucha, O.; Lange, K.W. The effect of conscious control on handwriting in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders 2005, 9, 323–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Jenni, O. Die kindliche Entwicklung verstehen: Praxiswissen über Phasen und Störungen, 1. Auflage; Springer, 2021, ISBN 9783662624470.
  102. Pellegrini, A.D.; Huberty, P.D.; Jones, I. The Effects of Recess Timing on Children’s Playground and Classroom Behaviors. American Educational Research Journal 1995, 32, 845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Hurschler Lichtsteiner, S. bst du noch oder schreibst du schon? Eine Untersuchung zum Handschrifterwerb in der zweiten Klasse mit Anregungen für den Unterricht. In Schreibunterricht - Schreibdidaktische Perspektiven auf Medien, Flüssigkeit, Impulse, Arrangements und Diagnostik; Kruse, N., Lemke, V., Steinhoff, T., Sturm, A., Eds.; Waxmann, in press.
  104. McCutchen, D. Knowledge, Processing, and Working Memory: Implications for a Theory of Writing. Educational Psychologist 2000, 35, 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Ruploh, B.; Martzy, F.; Bischoff, A.; Matschulat, N.; Zimmer, R. Veränderungen im Selbstkonzept nach psychomotorischer Förderung: Eine Pilotstudie im Mixed Methods Design. motorik 2013, 36, 180–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Fischer, K. Einführung in die Psychomotorik: Mit 11 Tabellen, 3., überarb. und erw. Aufl.; Reinhardt; UTB: München, Stuttgart, 2009; ISBN 9783497019915. [Google Scholar]
  107. Marsh, H.W. Self-concept theory, measurement and research into practice: The role of self-concept in educational psychology; British Psychological Society: Leicester, 2006; ISBN 9781854334480. [Google Scholar]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated