Preprint
Brief Report

Rich and Easy to Deceive? The Influence of Money Priming on the Ability to Detect Deception

Altmetrics

Downloads

197

Views

91

Comments

1

This version is not peer-reviewed

Submitted:

18 January 2024

Posted:

19 January 2024

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Based on recent popular money priming research results, which conclude that money makes self-sufficient (e.g. less interest in other people), we assumed that people are less interested in finding out whether others are lying or telling the truth. In a laboratory experiment, 163 students (85 women, 78 men, MAge = 23.08, ranging from 18 to 36 years) were primed by actively handling money (versus paper sheets). Afterwards, they classified 24 video statements as true or deceptive (senders describing their most/least favorite movie), rated their classification confidence for each decision and then answered control questions. Results revealed no influence of priming condition on judgmental bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence. Also the level of self-reported motivation to find out who lied or told the truth did not differ between conditions. Higher motivation was correlated to higher classification confidence. Additionally, and in line with Reinhard (2010) and Reinhard et al. (2011), higher classification accuracy correlated to a higher use of verbal content cues for classification decisions. Thus, while we were able to replicate these findings, our results contradict the assumption of a money prime influence on lie detection ability. Concluding, our results make self-sufficiency in this context questionable and offer next steps for research.
Keywords: 
Subject: Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Introduction

“Money makes people self-sufficient”, a discussed assumption based on results of the pioneer research by Vohs and colleagues regarding the influence of a money prime on social orientation [1]. They define self-sufficiency “as an insulated state wherein people put forth effort to attain personal goals and prefer to be separate from others” [1] (p. 1). The results of nine experiments, in which the type of the money prime and the type of the dependent variable were varied, supported their assumption. The authors primed with descrambling tasks, reading an essay in front of a video camera, playing monopoly, having play money, imagination, looking at a screensaver or a poster. The dependent measures for self-sufficiency varied from persistence on the problem before asking for help (time), or volunteering for help (number of solutions), donating money, or sitting with someone (distance between the chairs), to preference for activities alone vs. together (survey) see also [2,3,4]. In 2020, [5] published results which replicated the finding by [1] that being primed with money (screensaver showing money bills vs. fish floating through water) encouraged to prefer rather solitude activities (Cohen’s d = 0.37, but the results might be reliant on the kind of data analysis). [6] measured self-focus on trial basis with the imitation-inhibition task and found higher congruency in the control condition and so more self-focus under money priming (dz = .19). Another pro argument for a higher self-sufficiency when priming with money is provided by the results of [7] on Chinese participants that suggested an instrumentality orientation in social interactions then (Experiment 1 and 2). In Experiment 1, the money prime manipulation was facilitated by a picture evaluation task, and the dependent variable was measured with a 20-item objectification scale (see also [8]). The results showed higher objectification when participants were primed with money (compared to the control group). In Experiment 2, a sentence-descrambling task was utilized as the money prime. After the money prime manipulation, a goal for the participants was set. They were instructed to complete a task that requires mathematics skills and logical thinking. Then participants read a profile of an anonymous student. Depending on the condition, that student was described as majoring in math, being a fan of math, and planning to become a financial analyst after graduation (instrumental) or as majoring in Chinese, loving to read and write, and planning to become a writer after graduation (non-instrumental). Next, the approach intention of the participants was measured by asking them to what extent they would choose the student as their work partner on the mathematical task, feel good about the student and make friends with the student. The measurement of the perceived instrumentality followed with two items, asking the participants how much the student could help on the task and how useful the student is. Under the money prime, approach intention was increased when the perceived instrumentality was high. The effect did not occur under high competence of achieving a goal self-employed (Experiment 3). In Experiment 3, participants received feedback about their Sudoku achievement (manipulation of the competence for achieving a goal self-employed by different benchmarks) and were asked to collaborate for a next Sudoku with an anonymous student who was described as being good in math. Therefore, under money priming, people seem interested in others only when they need them to reach an own goal. In accordance, people also seem to be less distressed about social exclusion under the money prime [9], what refers to independence and self-sufficiency.
Beside the effect of the competence of achieving a goal self-employed on the outcomes of being primed with money, there also exists research regarding the feeling of socioeconomic status. [10] found that especially in the money prime condition, the feeling of a high economic status led to justifying the existing socioeconomic system (USA) more strongly (Study 1), and people believed more in the justness of its social outcomes (Study 2). In both studies, the authors used a word-descrambling task as money priming method and measured the socio economic status with the Mac Arthur Scale of subjective SES [11]. In Study 1, an 8-item System Justification Scale [12,13] was conducted as the dependent measure. In Study 2, the dependent variable was measured with the 20-item Belief in a Just World Scale [14]. Results therefore suggested, that money primed people seem to be more trustful when a feeling of higher economic status is activated and therefore would be more susceptible to deception. In line with our reasoning, a new study developed theoretical arguments that power (inducible by money) could be correlated with less deception detection [15]. To sum up, under the money prime, subjects show less interest in others, what others do, how they feel (self-sufficiency) and trust more; following, less interest whether others tell the truth or lie is assumable.

1.1. Deception detection in everyday life

In general, humans tend to have a low ability to detect deception. Overall, we are slightly above chance level in accurately judging the veracity of true or invented statements [16-18]. Further, based on meta-analysis results by [16], people seem more accurately identifying the truth as non-deceptive than lies as deceptive (Truth Bias). Research showed that neither education, sex, age, nor confidence with the judgment are significantly related to accuracy of truth and lie classification [19]. Nevertheless, the use of verbal cues in contrast to nonverbal cues for the judgment can enhance the accuracy rate [17]. In line with this, [20] tested the theoretical assumption that high task involvement (Experiment 1) and high cognitive capacity increase the use of verbal (therefore content) information in credibility judgments (Experiment 2 and 3). Based on research findings regarding what kind of verbal and nonverbal cues people use to evaluate the credibility of a statement [21-27], the authors manipulated four versions of a short film that displayed a social interaction between two persons (same film in Experiment 1 and 2, another version in Experiment 3). One person was shown in the video, whereas the other person could only be heard. Four versions were created: Deceptive verbal and deceptive nonverbal vs. deceptive verbal and truthful nonverbal vs. truthful verbal and truthful nonverbal vs. truthful verbal and deceptive verbal behavior. Participants were assigned to one of these conditions. Beforehand, in Experiment 1, task involvement was manipulated by the information on how important the participation was for science (high vs. not, between-subjects). In the high-involvement condition, participants differentiated between the deceptive and non-deceptive cues for their credibility judgment (higher credibility for non-deceptive cues and lower for deceptive cues), while in the low-involvement condition, there was no differentiation. In Experiment 2 and 3, the authors manipulated cognitive load by assigning the participants either to a distracting task (high cognitive load) or no task (low cognitive load) before watching the videos. In both experiments, while nonverbal cues were used under high cognitive load, the verbal cues were only used by individuals with low cognitive load, and so higher cognitive capacity. Moreover, in Experiment 3, the authors additionally asked for reasons for the credibility judgments; their analysis yielded results in line with the assumption that participants in the low cognitive load condition mainly used verbal cues.
Accordingly, in following studies, the authors further confirmed that the use of verbal cues was correlated to higher lie detection accuracy [28,29]. To summarize, using verbal cues (that automatically focus on the content compared to nonverbal cues) predicts better deception detection accuracy. People use these verbal cues more often when they are highly involved in the detection task and expend their cognition. When people are less interested in others (self-sufficient), they should be less involved in the task and, according to dual process theories, consequently use more effortless ways (nonverbal behavior) to arrive at a judgment [30,31]. Combining these arguments, we hypothesized that the money prime decreases classification accuracy of truths and lies concerning other peoples’ statements (in comparison to a control group).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

In 2014, a total of 163 students from a German University participated in the study (83 in the treatment group, 80 in the control group). A bar of chocolate and the possibility to win a game console were offered as incentives. The age of the participants ranged from 18 to 36 years (MAge = 23.09, SDAge = 4.14); two participants did not report their age. 85 women (52.15%) and 78 men (47.85%) participated.

2.2. Design and conditions

We tested our hypothesis with a 2 (Prime: Money vs. Paper) x 2 (Message type: Truth vs. Lie) Mixed-Methods-Design in a laboratory experiment, with prime and set of the messages as between-subject factors and message type as within-subject factor. In reference to [19], we included the variables gender and classification confidence to our analysis and according to [32] also the control variable set of the messages.
A sensitivity power analysis (G*Power; [33]) for the given sample size of N = 163 (Manova: Repeated measures, between factors; correlation between repeated measures of r = .039, α = .05) showed that a minimum effect size of Cohen’s d = 0.32 could be detected with a power of 80%.

2.3. Procedure and stimuli

At first, we thanked for the participation and participants were instructed that the study investigates the ability to recognize deception and truth. An informed consent followed. Afterwards, participants were randomized to one of two priming conditions (between-subjects, Money vs. Paper). We operationalized the money prime with the method of actively handling money due to it yielding the largest money priming effects (see meta-analysis by [34]). Participants were instructed to assess the value of the banknotes that were presented filled in a jar. They had to grab into the jar and touch the banknotes twice. After the first time, they wrote down the estimated value. After the second time, they wrote down the estimated number of bank notes. For the control group (paper prime condition), instead of money (banknotes), now participants were presented a jar filled with paper sheets and the same procedure followed. Here, in the first task, participants were instructed to estimate the weight of the paper sheets (by touching it). The next step of the experiment, the truth and lie detection task, followed on the monitor (see Appendix A). After the task, the demographic data of the participants was collected.

2.3.1. Stimulus material for the veracity judgments

Due to us wanting to present a basic scenario of social interactions without offering an instrumentality [7], we used videos about a personal statement regarding an individual attitude as stimulus material. The material was taken from a study by [28] (Experiment 3): 36 female and 36 male students were filmed, so that the head and upper body was to be seen, while they described for about one minute a movie they liked or disliked. In the truth condition, they actually liked or disliked the described movie. In the lie condition, they stated to like (or dislike) the described movie when they actually disliked (or liked) it. All participants were instructed to make their statement as truthful as possible. There was the opportunity to receive an extra reward of five Euros if the interviewer (blind to the experimental conditions) believed that they indeed liked or disliked the movie. In accordance to [28], we created three sets with 24 messages (see also [32]). Each set contained 12 truthful and 12 deceptive messages (balanced valence of senders’ attitudes and gender, no difference in video length across conditions).

2.3.2. Deception detection task

Participants were instructed that out of 24 videos of students that talk about films that they like and that they do not like, they have to evaluate who is telling the truth and who is lying. The videos were presented one after another. After each video, participants judged if the report was true or lied. As a control variable according to [19], they rated their confidence for each decision (on the next page) via a percentage scale (from 0% = not at all sure to 100% = completely sure) right after their judgment. Then, we measured their task motivation, (Cronbach’s α = .72; “It was important to me not to judge people who tell the truth as liars”, “It was important to me not to overlook people who are lying.”, “It was important to me to properly assess people who tell the truth”, “It was important to me to recognize liars as such”) which were assessed on a 7-point scale (from 1 = does not apply at all to 7 = applies exactly). Parallel to [28] and [29], we also asked, if participants used more verbal or nonverbal behavioral cues for their classification decision as true or deceptive, with two items (Cronbach’s α = .85; “I based my judgment more on …” “In watching the messages, I tended to pay more attention to …”; scale from 1 = nonverbal behaviour to 7 = verbal content).

3. Results

3.1. Judgmental bias

Overall, participants judged 52.30% (SD = 11.53%) of the video-statements as true. This differed with a small effect size from 50%, t(162) = 2.55, p = .012, 95% CI [0.52, 4.08], dCohen = 0.20, resulting in a truth bias (see also Table 1). An one-way ANOVA with priming condition (Money vs. Paper) as independent variable and number of truth judgments as dependent variable revealed no effect of priming condition, F(1, 162) = 1.88, p = .172, ηp2 = .01, 95% CI [-6.03, 1.09] (see also Table 2). So, the participants’ classification decision regarding the messages as true or deceptive did not depend on the priming condition. An additional univariate analysis of variance revealed no main effect of gender of the judges on the judgmental bias. Further, no interaction with the priming condition was found. We also found no interaction between set of the messages and priming condition, so the priming results did not depend on the set of the messages (see Appendix B).

3.2. Classifcation accuracy

The overall classification accuracy of 55.78% (SD = 10.72%) differed with a medium effect size from chance level (50%), t(162) = 6.88, p < .001 , 95% CI [4.12, 7.44], dCohen = 0.54. True statements were classified more accurately (M = 58.08%, SD = 53.48%) with a small effect size than false statements (M = 53.48%, SD = 17.97%), F(1, 162) = 6.40, p = .012, ηp2 = .04 (see also Table 1). To test our hypothesis, a 2 (Prime: Money vs. Paper) x 2 (Message type: Truth vs. Lie deceptive) mixed-design ANOVA with classification accuracy (in %) as the dependent variable was run. Against our hypothesis, the analysis revealed no effect of priming condition, F(2, 161) = 0.10, p = .758, ηp2 = .00, 95% CI [-2.81, 3.85], so the money prime did not decrease classification accuracy compared to the paper prime (see also Table 2). An additional univariate analysis of variance revealed no main effect of gender of the judges and no with the priming condition. Moreover, no interaction between set of the messages and priming condition on the classification accuracy was found (see Appendix B).
Based on that our results did not match the assumption, we additionally tested whether the power hypothesis or the null hypothesis is more likely to explain our findings. The Bayesian analysis found that the BF01 value of 7.82 was a moderate evidence for the null hypothesis. From this can be concluded that the probability is 7.82 times higher for that the null hypothesis occurs compared to our hypothesis.

3.3. Classification confidence

Overall classification confidence was M = 70.23% (SD = 12.43%). Confidence ratings regarding true statements (M = 70.80%, SD = 13.10%) did not differ to confidence ratings regarding false statements (M = 69.67%, SD = 13.15%), F(1, 162) = 2.96, p = .087, ηp2 = .02 (see also Table 1). A 2 (Prime: Money vs. Paper) x 2 (Message type: Truth vs. Lie) mixed-design ANOVA with classification confidence (in %) as the dependent variable yielded no effect of priming condition on classification confidence, F(1, 162) = 0.28, p = .595, ηp2 = .00, 95% CI [-4.90, 2.82] (see also Table 2). An additional univariate analysis of variance revealed no main effect of gender of the judges and no interaction with priming condition, and further no interaction between set of the messages and priming condition on the classification confidence (see Appendix B).

3.4. Self-reported task motivation

In an univariate ANOVA with priming condition as independent variable and self-reported task motivation as the dependent variable, there was no main effect of money prime, F(2, 161) = 0.37, p = .546, ηp2 = .00, 95% CI [4.80, 5.31]. Against our assumption, participants in the money prime condition did not report less task motivation (M = 5.05, SD = 1.18) compared to participants in the paper prime condition (M = 4.94, SD = 1.15). Further, neither the judgmental bias, r = .03, p = .715, nor classification accuracy, r = .07, p = .367, was correlated with the task motivation. However, higher classification confidence was correlated with higher self-reported motivation to correctly classify the statements as true or lied, r = .21, p = .007.

3.5. Self-reported use of verbal content versus nonverbal information

In an univariate ANOVA with prime condition as independent variable and self-reported use of nonverbal versus verbal content information as the dependent variable, no main effect of money prime was found, F(2, 161) = 1.78, p = .184, ηp2 = .01, 95% CI [-0.15, 0.75]. In contrast to our assumption, participants in the money prime condition did not report less use of verbal content information (M = 3.53, SD = 1.38) than participants in the paper prime condition (M = 4.04, SD = 1.51). Judgmental bias was not correlated to verbal content use, r = .01, p = .907. In line with the findings of [26] and [27], higher classification accuracy was low correlated with more self-reported use of verbal content (versus nonverbal) information, r = .16, p = .039. The negative correlation between classification confidence and verbal content use was only small, r = -.10, p = .196.

4. Discussion

In this study, we hypothesized that people are less accurate in their classification of video-messages as true or deceptive under a money prime compared to a neutral prime (paper). While the participants’ overall accuracy (of 55.78%) was a medium effect size better than chance (see also [16]), the results did not support our hypothesis that classification accuracy depends on the priming condition. Against our theoretical reasoning, the analysis of the control questions revealed that priming with money did not lead to less motivation and less use of (the more valid) verbal content information. So, the results of our control questions did not indicate a lower interest in detecting lies of others about a personal theme when participants were primed with money.
We found a small effect that true statements were classified as accurate more often than lied statements, which might be caused by a small judgmental bias. In line with the meta-analysis results by [19], classification accuracy was also not influenced by the control variable gender (just as judgmental bias and classification confidence). The overall classification confidence was 70.23%.
We investigated our research question in a laboratory experimental study with the money priming method that recently offered the highest self-sufficiency effects, actively handling money [34]. Furthermore, we presented 24 stimuli to each person (3 sets), [32] showed that with a minimum of 20 stimuli, the classification accuracy measurement becomes valid. Respectively, we found no interaction between set of the messages and prime condition, for neither judgmental bias, classification accuracy, nor classification confidence what ensures the reliability of our stimulus material. While our method was appropriate, we did not find an effect regarding our assumption.

4.1. Limitations

Recent research by [35] suggested that the consequences effected by priming with money depended on the constitution of the bank note, in detail if it is an unused new one versus a used one. A new banknote enhanced feelings of empathy regarding co-workers (Study 2) and decreased self-serving behavior compared to a used one and a general money priming (without information of the newness, Study 3). Further, priming with new money led to perceived stronger norms of social conscientiousness and higher helping intentions (Study 4). The authors conclude, priming with new money induces peoples’ warmth (prosocial behavior).
One could argue that the money prime manipulation did not work, no manipulation check was assessed. Nevertheless, also without a manipulation check, we could have found effects if they actually existed. From a methodological perspective, we did not differ in manipulating the money priming to recent research that found the money priming effect and have even chosen the most powerful method of actively handling money. In the research of the pioneer paper by [1], no manipulation checks are reported either. Also the available research papers listed in the meta-analysis by [34], with the priming method actively handling money, did not check for an effective manipulation.
Based on the fact that people show low deception detection ability in general [16-18] and our sensitivity analysis presuming an effect size of a minimum Cohen’s d = 0.32, it is possible that the effect actually exists but was too small to be found with our sample size of 163 participants. [34] however detected in their meta-analysis, which included papers from the published status until 2017, a Hedges’ g of 0.58 for the priming with actively handling money. Our sample size was big enough to reveal an effect of that size and nevertheless, with a rather small effect of Cohen’s d = 0.32, we were able to find a difference between the overall accuracy means (treatment vs. control) of minimum 3.5 percent if it exists. From a perspective that focuses on applied settings, a smaller difference would not have as much relevance, for example in organizational contexts. Meta-analysis results by [36] found an influence of money priming on the organizational-relevant outcomes performance (Cohen’s d = .38) and selfishness (Cohen’s d = .33). Either way, the results of our control questions showed an increased classification accuracy by a higher use of verbal content cues than nonverbal cues in the classification decision. Further, higher task motivation led to an increase in classification confidence. The results of both control measurements were independent from the priming condition.
An indirect argument for the nonexistence of an effect of money priming on deception detection are the results by [15] that exposed an increase in deception detection under power (asymmetric control over valued resources in social relationships, see [37]). Money implicates having power, being independent from others and, as explained before, seems to let people interact with others only when it is needed to instrumentalize them for reaching an own goal [7]. However, [15] found in their experiments that having power over others enhanced the accuracy of one’s veracity assessment. In line, in the present study, we did not find a decrease of classification accuracy by money priming. In accordance and contrasting [7] (Experiment 3), [38] revealed that enhancing self-affirmation (writing an essay explaining the personal importance of a core value) weakened the influence of money priming on self-sufficiency regarding different dependent measures: Such as donating more money under high self-affirmation, being more likely to request help, choosing more leisure experiences to be shared with others (versus individually focused ones) and feeling distressed after social exclusion (Cyberball, see [39]).
While our study and data collection in 2014 was based on the theoretical assumptions and the concerning published research findings at that time, revealing an existing money priming effect on self-sufficiency, nowadays published critiques by several experts point out the possibility of an absence of the money priming effect in general. For example, only one study out of 36 replications [40] was able to replicate the money priming effect by [12]. In line, [41] detected a selective re-porting by [12]. Furthermore, we want to highlight that in the meta-analysis by [34], only published results until 2017 were included. In terms of the publication bias, it was rather untypical to publish null-effects in that time. In accordance, integrated were rather less precise studies with lower sample sizes than more precise studies with higher sample sizes. These tendencies stay in line with our results (already in 2014), both display the assumption that money priming makes self-sufficient as quiet questionable. Otherwise, the null-effects in our study would implicate deception detection being a separate paradigm with which it is not possible to depict “self-sufficiency” what could be found with other dependent measurements.
However, someone could critically argue that our data collection might be “too old” to verify our conclusions for nowadays because crises in society like COVID-19 happened later on and had influences on customer experiences (e.g. [42]). Nevertheless, the year of data collection is transparent and the theory regarding the effects of money priming is not time sensitive, it rather follows the standard to be generalizable. In the last 100 years, comparable crises (for example “The 1918–1920 flu pandemic”, “World War II”, “9/11 terrorist attacks”, a. o.) caused by the environment occurred and if the theoretically embedded effect actually exists, it should not be affected by such changes in society. The results of our (reliable laboratory) study more likely display an important contribution in the wake of the replication crisis. The relevance of the publication of studies that found null-effects (see our Bayesian analysis) increased in importance and are especially interesting for meta-analytical reviews that present a progression over time. Therefore, the publication of the current study is in line with the arguments of the open science debate.

4.2. Future research

At first, in further research, manipulation checks should be accessed. According to [5], a possible manipulation check for a primed mindset could be the participants’ indication of their positivity toward words related to the prime, here money, compared to neutral words. Investigations on the information processing level depending on money priming seem to be essential as well. If a money prime makes self-sufficient (therefore being less interested in others), and based on dual-process theories [30,31], one could argue that these participants show lower motivation and so a lower level of attention regarding various social measures. To find out how the process level functions, distraction and time pressure are variables that could be manipulated, counterbalanced in both prime conditions.
The literature shows that the effect varies according to variations in the methodological operationalization and dependent variable (e.g. [5,34,40]). So, it seems relevant to focus on a different operationalization for being primed with money than actively holding it (and estimating its value) to answer the question if money makes self-sufficient. Beyond, it is thinkable that the way this priming was operationalized in its recently most powerful kind, that in fact was not so powerful at all as the lot failed replications represent (e.g. [34,40]), could have rather an influence in the undesirable direction. One could assume that holding the money and estimating its value could activate rather a comparing process with the own financial situation and so possibly the feeling of having less money. For children (3 to 6 years old), that are not in such a comparing position, handling money seems to lead to pro-social helpful behavior (number of crayons retrieved to the experimenter who asked for help) [43]. Against this assumption, research results from 2022 pointed out a buffering effect of money priming on negative emotions, especially for subliminal priming (versus supraliminal priming) and that it occurs automatically and unconsciously while the early phase of perception [44]. Nevertheless, for example, the influence of the present socio-economic status (see also [10]) and how satisfied the participants are with it on lie detection ability could reveal other effects than those found in the current study. Referring, [45] found that the socio-economic status positively influenced financial self-efficacy under the prime of gaining money.
Another variable that might offer some new insights is emotional intelligence. [46] showed in their research that high scoring emotionally intelligent people rather figured out mismatches between facial expression (nonverbal behavior) and verbal content information of the lying or the truth telling senders. Nevertheless, a good liar could even control facial expressions or body movements. As explained in the introduction, nonverbal cues do not seem to be connected to a better classification accuracy [28,29]. In line, while [47] found that emotional intelligence leads to a higher use of nonverbal cues, deception detection was not heightened; emotional intelligence rather seems to lead to an overestimation of the own lie detection ability [48]. [49] concluded that the aspect “perception of emotions” of emotional intelligence supported the deception detection. Moreover, based on findings by [50] that indicate a money priming effect on increased lying and cheating, further research not only regarding deception detection ability but also the actual lying behavior seems to be plausible.

Author Contributions

Research idea und theoretical hypotheses: Main contribution by the second author and additional contribution by the first author. Development of material and data collection: Full contribution by the second author. Data analysis: First and second author made equal contribution. Discussion: Main contribution by the first author and additional contribution by the second author.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in full accordance with the Ethical Guidelines of the German Association of Psychologists (DGPs) and the American Psychological Association (APA). Moreover, by the time the data were acquired it was also not customary at University of Mannheim, nor at most other German universities, to seek ethics approval for simple studies on lie detection ability. Therefore, ethical approval was not required for this study in accordance with the national and institutional guidelines. The study exclusively makes use of anonymous questionnaires. No identifying information was obtained from participants. Every participant had to read (and agree to) an informed consent. They were thereby explicitly informed that all data are treated confidentially and that they may withdraw from the study at any time without giving explanation. Contact information of the researchers was available from the beginning of the study until the end.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available on demand, https://osf.io/sa9w7/.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Appendix A.1. Instruction money prime condition (presented on a printed paper sheet)

Task 1:
In the following we would like to ask you to do an estimation task. The experimenter will show you a jar filled with bank notes.
Please estimate the value of the bank notes.
Studies show that the ability to estimate improves when the goods to be estimated can be touched. Therefore, please reach into the glass to touch the bank notes.
Now please go to the experimenter and reach into the jar.
Write down the estimated value of the bank notes:
Task 2:
Please estimate the number of bank notes.
Please reach into the glass again to enable a more accurate estimate.
Now please go to the experimenter and reach into the jar.
Write down the estimated number of bank notes:
WHEN YOU ARE FINISHED, PLEASE CONTACT THE EXPERIMENTER SO THEY CAN ASSIGN YOU TO A PC!

Appendix A.2. Instruction paper prime condition (presented on a printed paper sheet)

Task 1:
In the following we would like to ask you to do an estimation task. The experimenter will show you a jar filled with paper sheets.
Please estimate the weight of the paper sheets.
Studies show that the ability to estimate improves when the goods to be estimated can be touched. So, to give you a more accurate estimate, you are allowed to put your hands inside the jar.
Now please go to the experimenter and reach into the jar.
Write down the estimated weight of the paper sheets:
Task 2:
Please estimate the number of the paper sheets.
To give you a more accurate guess, you may put your hand in the jar again.
Now please go to the experimenter and reach into the jar.
Write down the estimated number of leaves:
WHEN YOU ARE FINISHED, PLEASE CONTACT THE EXPERIMENTER SO THEY CAN ASSIGN YOU TO A PC!

Appendix B

Appendix B.1. Supplementary material, additional analysis

Appendix B.1.1. Judgmental bias

No influence of the control variable gender on judgmental bias was found, F(1, 162) = 0.27, p = .603, ηp2 = .00, 95% CIfemale [-8.41, 8.21]. There was no interaction between gender and priming condition, F(1, 162) = 1.27, p = .261, ηp2 = .01. Unexpectedly, the analysis yielded a large main effect for the control variable set of the messages, F(2, 161) = 14.38, p < .001, ηp2 = .16 (set 1: M = 56.85%, SD = 11.55%; set 2: M = 45.41%, SD = 9.09%; set 3: M = 53.56%, SD = 10.71%), 95% CIset 1 [-9.95, 8.28], 95% CIset 2 [-12.49, 5.05]. However, no interaction between priming condition and set of the messages on judgmental bias was found, F(1, 162) = 0.77, p = .465, ηp2 = .01. There was no three-way interaction, F(2, 161) = 0.70, p = .501, ηp2 = .01 (see also Table A1 and Table A2).

Appendix B.1.2. Classification accuracy

Further, the analysis yielded no main effect for the control variable gender on classification accuracy, F(1, 162) = 3.48, p = .064, ηp2 = .02, 95% CIfemale [-4.38, 10.66].
Gender and priming condition did not interact, F(1, 162) = 0.32, p = .570, ηp2 = .00. Unexpectedly, the analysis yielded a large main effect of the control variable set of the messages on classification accuracy, F(2, 161) = 21.87, p < .001, ηp2 = .23, 95% CIset 1 [-5.44, 11.05], 95% CIset 2 [7.83, 23.71]. Nevertheless, we found no interaction between priming condition and set of messages on classification accuracy, F(2, 161) = 1.87, p = .158, ηp2 = .02. There also was no three-way interaction, F(2, 161) = 1.21, p = .302, ηp2 = .02 (see also Table A1 and Table A2).

Appendix B.1.3. Classification confidence

Further, for the control variable gender, no main effect on classification confidence was shown, F(1, 162) = 0.32, p = .575, ηp2 = .00, 95% CIfemale [-12.78, 7.01]. Further, no interaction between gender and priming condition emerged, F(1, 162) = 0.59, p = .444, ηp2 = .00. There was also no main effect for the control variable set of messages on classification confidence, F(2, 161) = 0.08, p = .926, ηp2 = .00, 95% CIset 1 [-12.64, 9.06], 95% CIset 2 [-15.51, 5.38]. In line, the analysis further yielded no interaction between priming condition and set of the messages on classification confidence, F(2, 161) = 1.81, p = .167, ηp2 = .02. There was no three-way interaction, F(2, 161) = 0.93, p = .396, ηp2 = .01 (see also Table A1 and Table A2).
Table A1. Results of the additional univariate analysis of variance for judgmental bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the prime and on the gender of the judges.
Table A1. Results of the additional univariate analysis of variance for judgmental bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the prime and on the gender of the judges.
Variable Main effect(gender) Interaction(gender x prime)
F(1, 162) ηp2 F(1, 162) ηp2
Truth bias
Overall 2.40 .02 1.85 .01
Classification accuracy
Overall 0.37 .00 0.13 .00
True messages 3.48 .02 2.22 .01
Deceptive messages 0.38 .00 0.42 .00
Classification confidence
Overall 0.33 .00 0.20 .00
True messages 0.77 .01 0.00 .00
Deceptive messages 0.04 .00 0.75 .01
Table A2. Results of the additional univariate analysis of variance for judgmental bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the prime and on the set of the messages.
Table A2. Results of the additional univariate analysis of variance for judgmental bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the prime and on the set of the messages.
Variable df F (N = 163) p ηp2
Truth bias
Money priming 1 1.78 0.184 0.01
Messages-set 2 14.38 < .001 0.16
Money priming x messages-set 2 0.77 0.465 0.01
Classification accuracy
Money priming 1 0.6 0.441 0
Messages-set 2 21.87 < .001 0.23
Money priming x messages-set 2 1.87 0.158 0.02
Classification confidence
Money priming 1 0.13 0.72 0
Messages-set 2 0.08 0.926 0
Money priming x messages-set 2 1.81 0.167 0.02

References

  1. Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314(5802), 1154–1156. [CrossRef]
  2. Pfeffer, J., & DeVoe, S. E. (2009). Economic evaluation: The effect of money and economics on attitudes about volunteering. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(3), 500–508. [CrossRef]
  3. Roberts, J. A., & Roberts, C. R. (2012). Money matters: Does the symbolic presence of money affect charitable giving and attitudes among adolescents?. Young Consumers, 13(4), 329–336. [CrossRef]
  4. Wierzbicki, J., & Zawadzka, A. M. (2016). The effects of the activation of money and credit card vs. that of activation of spirituality – Which one prompts pro-social behaviours?. Current Psychology, 35(3), 344–353. [CrossRef]
  5. Winter, F., & Diekmann, A. (2020). 13 Does money change everything? Priming experiments in situations of strategic interaction. In Buskens, A., Corten, R., & Snijders, C. (Eds.), Advances in the sociology of trust and cooperation: Theory, experiments, and field studies (pp. 318–327). De Gruyter.
  6. Genschow, O., Schuler, J., Cracco, E., Brass, M., & Wänke, M. (2020). The effect of money priming on self-focus in the imitation-inhibition task. Experimental Psychology, 66(6), 377–444. [CrossRef]
  7. Teng, F., Chen, Z., Poon, K. T., Zhang, D., & Jiang, Y. (2016). Money and relationships: When and why thinking about money leads people to approach others. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 137, 58–70. [CrossRef]
  8. Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Power and the objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 111–127. [CrossRef]
  9. Zhou, X., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). The symbolic power of money: Reminders of money alter social distress and physical pain. Psychological Science, 20(6), 700–706. [CrossRef]
  10. Schuler, J., & Wänke, M. (2016). A fresh look on money priming: Feeling privileged or not makes a difference. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(4), 366–373. [CrossRef]
  11. Piff, P. K., Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., Cheng, B. H., & Keltner, D. (2010). Having less, giving more: The influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771–784. [CrossRef]
  12. Caruso, E. M., Vohs, K. D., Baxter, B., & Waytz, A. (2013). Mere exposure to money increases endorsement of free-market systems and social inequality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 301–306. [CrossRef]
  13. Kay, A. C., & Jost, J. T. (2003). Complementary justice: Effects of ‘‘poor but happy’’ and ‘‘poor but honest’’ stereotype exemplars on system justification and implicit activation of the justice motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 823–837. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.823.
  14. Rubin, Z., & Peplau, L. A. (1975). Who believes in a just world? Journal of Social Issues, 31(3), 65–89. [CrossRef]
  15. Ulatowska, J., & Cislak, A. (2022). Power and lie detection. PloS one, 17(6), e0269121. [CrossRef]
  16. Bond, C. F., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 214-234. [CrossRef]
  17. Hartwig, M., & Bond, C. F., Jr. (2011). Why do lie-catchers fail? A lens model meta-analysis of human lie judgments. Psychological bulletin, 137(4), 643–659. [CrossRef]
  18. Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities. Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley.
  19. Aamodt, M. G., & Custer, H. (2006). Who can best catch a liar? A meta-analysis of individual differences in detecting deception. Forensic Examiner, 15(1), 6–11.
  20. Reinhard, M.-A., & Sporer, S. L. (2008). Verbal and nonverbal behaviour as a basis for credibility attribution: The impact of task involvement and cognitive capacity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 477–488. [CrossRef]
  21. Kraut, R. E. (1978). Verbal and nonverbal cues in the perception of lying. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(4), 380–391. [CrossRef]
  22. Miller, G. R., & Stiff, J. B. (1993). Deceptive communication. Sage Publishing.
  23. Stiff, J. B., & Miller, G. R. (1986). ‘‘Come to think of it ...’’ Interrogative probes, deceptive communication, and deception detection. Human Communication Research, 12(3), 339–357. [CrossRef]
  24. Stiff, J. B., Miller, G. R., Sleight, C., Mongeau, P., Garlick, R., & Rogan, R. (1989). Explanations for visual cue primacy in judgments of honesty and deceit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(4), 555–564. [CrossRef]
  25. Vrij, A. (2000). Detecting lies and deceit. The psychology of lying and the implications for professional practice. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
  26. Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 1–59). Academic Press. [CrossRef]
  27. Zuckerman, M., & Driver, R. E. (1985). Telling lies: Verbal and nonverbal correlates of deception. In A. W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Multichannel integrations of nonverbal behavior (pp. 129–147). Erlbaum.
  28. Reinhard, M.-A. (2010). Need for cognition and the process of lie detection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 961–971. [CrossRef]
  29. Reinhard, M.-A., Sporer, S. L., Scharmach, M., & Marksteiner, T. (2011). Listening, not watching: situational familiarity and the ability to detect deception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(3), 467–484. [CrossRef]
  30. Chen, S., & Chaiken, S. (1999). The heuristic-systematic model in its broader context. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 73–96). Guilford Press.
  31. Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 41–72). Guilford Press.
  32. Levine, T. R., Daiku, Y., & Masip, J. (2021). The number of senders and total judgments matter more than sample size in deception-detection experiments. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(1), 191–204. [CrossRef]
  33. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior research methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. [CrossRef]
  34. Lodder, P., Ong, H. H., Grasman, R. P., & Wicherts, J. M. (2019). A comprehensive meta-analysis of money priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(4), 688–712. [CrossRef]
  35. Mok, A., & De Cremer, D. (2016). When money makes employees warm and bright: Thoughts of new money promote warmth and competence. Management and Organization Review, 12(3), 547–575. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/mor.2015.53.
  36. Stajkovic, A. D., Greenwald, J. M., & Stajkovic, K. S. (2022). The money priming debate revisited: A review, meta-analysis, and extension to organizations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 43(6), 1078–1102. [CrossRef]
  37. Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Social hierarchy: The self-reinforcing nature of power and status. Academy of Management annals, 2(1), 351–398. [CrossRef]
  38. Park, J. K., & Vohs, K. (2013). Self-affirmation has the power to offset the harmful effects of money reminders. ACR North American Advances. https://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/v41/acr_v41_15002.pdf.
  39. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290–292. [CrossRef]
  40. Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams Jr, R. B., Bahník, Š., Bernstein, M. J., ... & Nosek, B. A. (2014). Investigating variation in replicability. Social psychology, 45(3), 137–252. [CrossRef]
  41. Rohrer, D., Pashler, H., & Harris, C. R. (2015). Do subtle reminders of money change people’s political views?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(4), e73–e85. [CrossRef]
  42. Vázquez-Martínez, U. J., Morales-Mediano, J., & Leal-Rodríguez, A. L. (2021). The impact of the COVID-19 crisis on consumer purchasing motivation and behavior. European Research on Management and Business Economics, 27(3), 100166. [CrossRef]
  43. Gasiorowska, A., Chaplin, L. N., Zaleskiewicz, T., Wygrab, S., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Money cues increase agency and decrease prosociality among children: Early signs of market-mode behaviors. Psychological Science, 27(3), 331–344. [CrossRef]
  44. Cheng, L., Ma, Q., Qiu, W., & Pei, G. (2022). Decomposing the neural substrates of the supraliminal and subliminal buffering effects of money on negative emotions. Current Psychology, 1–14. [CrossRef]
  45. Ward, S. & Kim, J. (2021). How does money make life meaningful? Socioeconomic status, financial self-efficacy, and meaning in life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 18(6), 1–18. [CrossRef]
  46. Wojciechowski, J., Stolarski, M., & Matthews, G. (2014). Emotional intelligence and mismatching expressive and verbal messages: A contribution to detection of deception. PLoS One, 9(3), e92570. [CrossRef]
  47. Roulin, N., & Ternes, M. (2019). Is it time to kill the detection wizard? Emotional intelligence does not facilitate deception detection. Personality and Individual Differences, 137, 131–138. [CrossRef]
  48. Baker, A., ten Brinke, L., & Porter, S. (2013). Will get fooled again: Emotionally intelligent people are easily duped by high-stakes deceivers. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 18(2), 300–313. [CrossRef]
  49. Stewart, S. L., Wright, C., & Atherton, C. (2019). Deception detection and truth detection are dependent on different cognitive and emotional traits: An investigation of emotional intelligence, theory of mind, and attention. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(5), 794–807. [CrossRef]
  50. Kouchaki, M., Smith-Crowe, K., Brief, A. P., & Sousa, C. (2013). Seeing green: Mere exposure to money triggers a business decision frame and unethical outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121(1), 53–61. [CrossRef]
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and results of the one-way analyses of variance for the truth bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the priming manipulation (bank notes vs. paper sheets).
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and results of the one-way analyses of variance for the truth bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the priming manipulation (bank notes vs. paper sheets).
Variable Money prime Paper prime
M SD M SD
Truth bias 53.51% 10.56% 51.04% 12.39%
Overall classification accuracy 55.52% 11.05% 56.04% 10.44%
Classification accuracy for true messagesClassifications accuracy for deceptive messages 59.04% 13.09% 57.08% 13.20%
52.01% 17.20% 55.00% 18.73%
Overall classification confidenceClassification confidence for true messagesClassification confidence for deceptive messages 70.74% 9.43% 69.70% 14.97%
70.53% 10.32% 68.77% 15.57%
70.96% 10.53% 70.63% 15.39%
Table 2. Results of the mixed analysis of variance for the truth bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the priming manipulation (bank notes vs. paper sheets) and the message type (truth/lie).
Table 2. Results of the mixed analysis of variance for the truth bias, classification accuracy, and classification confidence depending on the priming manipulation (bank notes vs. paper sheets) and the message type (truth/lie).
Variable F(1, 162) p ηp2
Truth bias
Between-subjects effects
Money prime 1.88 .17 .01
Classification accuracy
Between-subjects effects
Money prime 0.10 .76 .00
Within-subjects effects
Message typeMoney prime x message type 6.40 .01 .04
1.88 .17 .01
Classification confidence
Between-subjects effects
Money prime 0.28 .60 .00
Within-subjects effectsMessage typeMoney prime x message type
3.03 .08 .02
1.19 .28 .01
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated