1. Introduction
Learning involves the interaction of cognitive, emotional and physiological elements. In this context, emotions are considered a very important part of the knowledge acquisition process. The Greek philosopher Plato (427-347 b.C.) already indicated that "all learning has an emotional basis". Nevertheless, there are many definitions for emotions, and no definition is considered unanimous. In addition, stress is not only a negative aspect, it is also necessary in learning, being a positive aspect when it is under control [
1,
2,
3,
4].
According to Scherer et al [
5], emotions occur when a person evaluates an event or situation as important. For instance, the most recent stress situation (COVID-19 pandemic) has caused an emotional disturbance in the general population and in the field of education in particular. A consequence of negative emotions is stigmatization, by which students are mentally affected by continuous fear experiences. This occurs because emotions are accompanied by sentimental, cognitive, behavioral and physiological changes [
6,
7,
8].
In 2020, the World Health Organization (WHO) declared pandemic status to COVID-19 infection due to the alarming levels of virus spreading into the population [
9]. The fears generated by exposure to COVID-19 resulted in anxiety and states of stress, in all people and particularly in education actors (teachers and students). Although, direct measures were taken to safeguard the well-being of both teachers and students, the pandemic situation altered their emotional state, leading to burnout and decreased learning ability [
10,
11,
12,
13]. Other pandemics, such as in the influenza pandemic, high levels of stress, anxiety and low mood were observed in this case in healthcare workers [
14]. These situations undoubtedly lead to increased fatigue and decreased intellectual performance. In the field of education, the pandemic situation has forced students to be at home, not attending universities and high schools, or alternatively attending in small groups. Meanwhile online classes have certain advantages, some students, particularly those studying health sciences, require more resources making it difficult to receive suitable classes [
16,
17]. Otherwise said, students who usually follow their studies on a face-to-face basis are not very familiar with the on-line experience undergoing a systemic shock.
2. Objective
The main objective of this narrative review is to analyze the influence of stress and emotions (both components interconnected) in the learning process. The analysis will be focused in university students in several key points that are interrelated and at the same time, have a direct influence in the learning process. COVID-19 pandemic will be taken as a recent example of learning disturbance due to stress and emotion. The learning process represents a stress by itself, but at the same time is influenced by emotions. Finally, we wanted to know how emotions derived from stress response have influence in intelligence, and in particular in talented individuals. Particular aspects such anxiety, fear and depression will be analyzed in different subsections in this narrative review, trying to understand in more detail their influence in the learning process.
In addition, we present information from a physiological point of view as a key element for understanding the role of emotions in the systemic response. This information could open interest for future pharmacological research. In this context, emotions help to promote learning by stimulating the activity of neural networks and reinforcing synapses (neuronal connections). At the same time, the central nervous system (CNS) is plastic, allowing to change neuronal connections in response to external and internal factors. In the learning process, emotions open the door to all these changes. Emotions follow complex reaction patterns, involving experiential, behavioral and physiological elements such as anger or fear in stress moments. These patterns are subjectively experienced as strong feelings usually directed toward a specific object and typically accompanied by physiological and behavioral changes in the body. Therefore, emotions play a fundamental role in learning facilitating the consolidation of memories. Those experiences that are associated with a high emotional charge last more easily in the memory. All these aspects were presented in more detail in the following sections.
3. Methods
To conduct this narrative review, a comprehensive literature search was conducted using the databases PubMed, Ovid MEDLINE and EMBASE and the following search terms “EMOTIONS”, LEARNING” “EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE, "COVID-19", and "STRESS". Then, we combined some terms such as “LEARNING AND EMOTIONS” and “LEARNING AND STRESS” to obtain more specific information. First, we have carefully read and selected the summaries of the articles. It should be noted that many of them presented very similar message. Therefore, we selected those that communicate the data in a simple way. Then, we read in more detail and analyzed those important aspects that could contribute to the development of the review.
4. COVID-19, an example of learning disturbances due to emotions and stress
Emotions are manifested as a response to manage against environment changes trying to maintain subject’s well-being [
18]. The COVID-19 pandemic is the closest example that undoubtedly has generated fear and anxiety (negative emotions) in many segments of the population [
12]. In the university context, Othman et al [
19] reported that students were susceptible to developpe stress and depression. This could clearly be amplified by the isolating situation that the pandemic generated. In this context, stress and anxiety difficulted learning and increased dropout. Therefore, students must balance between individual stressors and education demands [
19,
20,
21]. In general, stress tends to be seen as a negative condition at the beginning, although it is key for adaptation at long term [
22,
23]. This occurs because stress response is mediated by the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary axis (SAM) (a neuroendocrine stress-response system), in particular by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, culminating in the release of adrenal hormones such as cortisol. However, at long term, hormones released are involved in the regulation of the immune system and inflammatory activities, contributing to such adaptation [
2,
23].
On the other hand, teaching during a global pandemic was challenging [
24,
25,
26,
27]. However, before the COVID-19 outbreak, the mental health of young adults was already a global concern. Early reports during the COVID-19 pandemic showed that students did not prefer e-learning
vs face-to-face teaching [
28]. However, in the last moments of pandemic, the majority of studies reported positive perceptions of e-learning [
29]. The change in this tendency occurred because learning-centered approaches were used to facilitate access to contents. Despite all advantages, e-learning was not perceived positively in all academic contexts, particularly in health science education. In this line, the main drawback of this e-learning was the impossibility of live practices necessary to acquire future professional competences [
30,
31]. Moreover, additional study-related worries have been described in the vast majority of both graduate and undergraduate students [
32]. Most of the studied participants were worried about concentration capacity, academic progress, future plans and academic performance. However, the study of Copeland et al. [
33] conducted in USA found that students enrolled in a campus wellness were more confident and optimistic that their home quarantined counterparts. In this line, a study carried out at our own University (Valladolid), Odriozola-González et al. [
34] studied the emotional impact of the COVID-19 pandemic situation. They found that around 25-30% of students surveyed reported moderate to extreme severe anxiety, depression and stress scores as a result from the fear of e-learning during quarantine COVID-19 pandemic. Finally, a systematic review and meta-analysis compared the mental health of nonquarantined and quarantined people. Isolation in quarantine was found to be an independent risk factor for adverse mental health outcomes, increasing 2.8-fold the risk of depression 2.0-fold anxiety and 2.7-fold stress response disorders [
35]. Another study performed on 932 participants showed that people in isolation were at greater risk for anxiety and depression [
36].
5. Stress and the learning process
Evidence strongly assess that stress has a key influence in the learning process. First of all, stress affects memory recall. It is well known to most students that the acute stress undergone when facing an evaluation test can make difficult to recall information that might be available in less stressful circumstances [
37]. Nevertheless, the impact of stress on the process of encoding information into memory is controversial. Some authors defend that stress impairs memory functions [
38], meanwhile others show that stress enhances memory encoding [
39]. However, it is difficult to determine the factors that are responsible for the reported discrepancies. It seems that stress could be considered as a positive component when it is under control and a negative factor when the circumstances are clearly adverse (no possibility of control) (1).
Stress negatively affects the physical and mental health of students and hinders academic performance [
40,
41]. During the first years at the university, students experience high levels of stress and anxiety, largely due to the new situation and the competition between students themselves [
42,
43]. Learning stressors are also associated with personal stressors, such as family demands, work, or sports, that compete with the demands of education. Undoubtedly, the pressure felt by the student exacerbates their state of stress and anxiety [
43].
Stress acts on different neural pathways and brain locations critical for memory. It has rapid effects by producing an increase in dopaminergic and noradrenergic activity in the prefrontal cortex [
44,
45]. It acts as well through the hypothalamus-amygdala axis (HPA) to regulate the secretion of stress hormones adrenaline and noradrenaline [
46,
47,
48,
49,
50]. These hormones stimulate the vagus nerve and ultimately influence the hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex, among other regions [
47,
48,
49,
50,
51,
52].
6. Emotions and the learning process
Emotions are interpretations of the internal and external environment. The perceived information is used subsequently for action [
2,
23]. In addition, emotions have a motivational function that predisposes to repeat behaviors linked to positive feelings. In this line, emotion regulation is multidimensional, and includes: (a) awareness, understanding and acceptance of emotions; (b) the ability to control impulsive behaviors during distress; (c) the ability to use appropriate situational strategies to modulate the intensity and duration of emotions; and (d) the willingness to experience negative affective states in order to engage in meaningful life activities [
8]. Loss of control, stress, anxiety and threat perception have been found to be involved in the reaction to emotions, resulting in increased anxiety and feelings of helplessness [
8].
Many different criteria have been used to classify emotions. Ekman [
53] indicates that basic/innate emotions have a differentiated facial expression that is universal and easy to identify. However, acquired emotions seem to follow a more complex interpretation pattern depending on whether they are positive, negative or neutral, pleasant and unpleasant, problematic, individual or collective [
53,
54]. Altogether, an adequate classification for emotions could be: a) primary or innate, also called basic, pure or elementary; b) secondary or acquired, also known as social. In general, the primary would include fear, surprise, anger, rage, disgust, sadness, and happiness among others. The secondary includes guilt, shame, contentment, jealousy, acceptance, resignation, and pleasure, among others.
However, in the learning process, classification using the terms “positive”, “negative” and related terms seem to be more appropriate. Therefore, negative emotions affect well-being and provoke a desire to avoid or evade. These include anger, aversion, fear, anxiety, sadness, shame, among others. Positive emotions are considered healthy because they positively affect the well-being of the individual. They favor the way people think, reason and act, including joy, humor, love, happiness, among others. To complete this classification, ambiguous emotions that do not fit with the previous criteria should be considered, including hope, surprise and compassion. Finally, toxic emotions with a very negative component are considered as well in this classification, being envy as the most representative [
55]. Altogether, COVID-19 was the most clear and recent example indicating how emotions influence the learning process.
In addition, it is also necessary to differentiate between emotions and emotional state. The emotion is characterized by an alteration of the physiological response that predisposes to an organized systemic response [
23,
56,
57]. However, emotional state refers to mood, which is of longer duration and lower intensity. Emotions are of short duration and are triggered by a specific stimulus [
58]. Coming back to the example of COVID-19 pandemic, the first evidence is that the emotional impact on each individual is very particular. For some, their emotional wellbeing decreased as they did not know how to cope with the uncertainty, anxiety, boredom and even sadness that staying at home implied. For others, the same situation was a challenge turning these emotions into positive ones [
59,
60].
Therefore, it is well known that positive emotions support learning by increasing intellectual, physical and social resources, favoring creative development and improving coping strategies [
61,
62]. These emotions end up modifying the perception of learning and, therefore, the success achieved after the effort. Success should be considered as a non-measurable entity, as it is not an academic grade, a job, a salary achieved, or a qualification from the environment. Success is mainly an emotional and individual state, which is not defined from outside references but from personal introspection. In this context, the concept of success fits with the definition stated by John Wooden in 1997 [
63] "Success is a peace of mind that is direct result of self-satisfaction in knowing you did your best to become the best that you are capable of becoming".
6.1. Physiology of emotions
Kandel et al. [
64] reported that the emotional state is composed of an overt element characterized by physical sensations and another characterized by a concrete feeling (conscious process). Both emotional state and feelings are regulated in different anatomical structures. Concrete feelings are regulated by the cerebral cortex, partly the cingulate and the orbitofrontal cortex. Emotional states are regulated by a joint action with nervous, endocrine and skeletal-motor responses. The amygdala is the intercommunication structure of somatic expression of emotions (hypothalamus and brainstem nuclei), and is the system of interpretation of concrete feelings, especially fear (cingulate, parahippocampal and frontal cortex) [
65]. This occurs because the brain builds a permanent mental image during the emotional state with a mental map that characterizes the state of the body (viscera, musculoskeletal system) [
64]. When the brain detects emotional stimuli, it sends specific commands to the CNS, NES (neuroendocrine system), ANS (autonomic nervous system) and the musculoskeletal system. This results in different reactions to emotions. The involuntary physiological response seems to be the first reaction to emotion. The psychological response comes after, including the way in which information is processed and finally, the behavioral response that generates a change in mood. Based on the systems involved in emotions, as well as the development of processes and functions, five components are identified [
5]:
- (a)
Physiological, which fulfils the function of regulation of organ systems, depending on the CNS, ANS and NES [
2,
66,
67]. These systems regulate physiological and emotional responses influencing the unconscious and instinctive behavior (important in survival). Many of these innate and primitive behaviors are altered by the brain cortex [
68]. For instance, human unconscious behaviors such as confidence, hope, joy, guilt and despair, are influenced by conscious moral, social and cultural codes [
66,
67].
- (b)
The cognitive component is linked to information processing and has the function of evaluation.
- (c)
The motivational component is linked to the CNS which prepares and directs actions. In emotional states there are more active CNS locations [
68].
- (d)
Motor expression, which fulfils a communicative function by informing the behavioral reaction and intention.
- (e)
The subjective aspect serves to monitor the internal state of the organism and its interaction with the environment.
The fundamental function of the amygdala (subcortical structure in the internal temporal lobe) is the processing and storage organ of emotional reactions. It produces short-term adaptation, which allows to increase rapid, unconscious responses that, although not very precise, are effective. The central nucleus coordinates efferent information that gives rise to both autonomic (sympathetic and parasympathetic), endocrine and behavioral emotional responses [
57,
64,
69]. The amygdala facilitates the formation of stimulus associations and helps to establish the emotional meaning of different situations [
70].
Emotional information follows two pathways (one fast and one slow) to the amygdala. A third, equally important pathway (hippocampus-amygdala) underpins contextual conditioning. Through the thalamus-amygdala connections, an affective process with simple sensory features is elaborated, and through the thalamocortical connections, the complex process without affective components is produced. Through the cortico-amygdaline connections the emotional component contributed to the complex information elaborated in the cortex [
66,
71].
The final process results in adaptive changes and reorganization of CNS due to its neuroplasticity. These changes occur both by learning and by adaptation to internal or external situations. In this process, activities such as neurogenesis might be considered.
6.2. Non-physiological components of emotions
Emotions have different components: subjective, behavioral and emotional [
62,
72,
73,
74,
75]. In this sense, and from the point of view of stress, the influence of personal traits, self-perception and previous experiences must be considered, as they depend to a large extent on the type of stressor [
76]. This explains why coping requires behavioral and cognitive effort to manage stressful situations. In this context, some strategies are considered to focus on the problem (person-situation relationship) and others on the emotion or emotional disturbances resulting from the stressful situation [
77,
78].
6.3. Functions of emotions
Most authors focus on the cognitive and intellectual functions of the brain, including thinking, learning and decision-making. However, the brain is not only a center of reasoning, but also an agent of motivation and expression of emotions. Under normal conditions, a balance between the emotional and rational mind is stablished. Emotions shape rational operations and the rational mind adjusts and sometimes modulates emotional decisions [
57].
The left hemisphere of the brain, the rational part of the brain, is where structured functions, i.e. language. The right hemisphere, on the other hand, is the emotional hemisphere, which governs subjective feelings, i.e. the ability to appreciate art and music. The emotional part processes much faster than the rational mind, because this last-one establishes the cause-effect relationship, being supported by objective evidence, and it can lead to re-evaluation of the situation, giving the possibility to change a previous decision [
68,
69,
70,
71].
The part of the brain that manages the emotional mind is the limbic system that is related to the regulation of emotions and behavior, i.e. memory, attention, emotions, personality and behavior [
2,
76]. Also, the amygdala is the key brain structure for emotion management [
67,
70], providing connections that produce emotional reactions and enable the inhibition of behavior. Therefore, amygdala main function is to integrate emotions by inducing the physiological response and the preparation of the behavioral response. In addition, the amygdala is responsible for coordinating the areas of somatic expression of emotion and the cerebral cortex, responsible for conscious feeling. This means that it plays a key role in the assessment of the emotional meaning of experiences [
77,
78].
The four important neurotransmitter pathways in an emotional-motivational sense are those mediated by dopamine, serotonin, noradrenaline and endorphin. The release of dopamine translates motivation into action. Upon stress exposure, dopamine mediates by selecting the optimal response for coping with stressful situations [
76]. Different hormones and neurotransmitters complement this reward emotional response such as testosterone and oxytocin respectively [
76]. Finally, the neurotransmitter serotonin, known as the "mood hormone", is related to well-being and helping to manage stress. In addition, serotonin inhibits anger and regulates temperature, mood, appetite, sleep, falling in love, among others [
76]. The result is that emotions have three clear functions: adaptive, social and motivational [
79,
80] (
Table 1).
All emotions, pleasant or unpleasant, are necessary and useful, as they allow to quickly assess situations, to inform if something is important for well-being. They are very important for increasing learning capacity and memory, all of which will allow them to play an important role in decision-making [
81].
Coming back to the example of COVID-19, all functions of emotions were altered at the current time of pandemic. However, not all people were equally vulnerable to these events. Johnson et al. [
82] in a survey study, analyzed the impact of COVID-19 on the Argentinean population and observed a high level of uncertainty and fear in the population in relation to COVID-19. Similar data were obtained in India, where they expressed a high degree of concern and uncertainty about COVID-19 and a higher percentage indicated the need for psychological support to reduce the impact on their mental health [
83]. Alcalá et al. [
84] have observed that most students felt fear, anxiety, stress and uncertainty related to COVID-19 infection. Other authors [
85] have obtained similar results in studies on the return to clinical learning after the COVID-19 outbreak. Depending on the vulnerability of the subject, and once the acute phase is over, it can be observed a functional or adaptive coping response, or a dysfunctional, maladaptive or counterproductive coping response [
86]. Stress is one of the risks of adaptive dysfunctions. In this sense, Buitrago et al. [
87] listed possible reactions in situations of intense stress such as the COVID-19 pandemic, affecting mental health. Altogether, emotions precede feelings [
88].
Regarding the mechanisms involved in stress, understanding the biological basis on executive functions may provide more insight into the mechanisms underlying the effects of stress on cognition [
89,
90]. One of the main functions of emotions is to facilitate the appearance of appropriate behaviors facing stress [
81]. The expression of emotions allows to predict the behavior associated with them. In this line, performance of cognitive tasks prior to an executive task can modulate performance [
91,
92]. Nevertheless, disturbances coming from amygdala such as fear, can affect learning and memory storage [
93,
94,
95]. Therefore, knowing how to modulate emotions in stressful situations could promote effective learning. Emotions can act on the psychological processes responsible for focusing attention or solving problems [
96]. This emotional self-regulation, understood as the ability to motivate oneself, would be one of the dimensions encompassed by the so-called emotional intelligence [
97].
6.4. Emotional intelligence
Many authors have asserted that emotional intelligence (EI) contributes to an individual's ability to adapt socially, work more effectively in teams, perform better, and cope more effectively with stress and other forms of environmental pressure [
98,
99]. Therefore, EI functions as an essential predictor of students' learning and cognitive health [
100,
101]. For instance, students and faculty have managed the learning process during the pandemic through their emotional intelligence and cognitive engagement in blended learning environments [
102]. Higher education institutions have managed the problem of campus closure by switching from face-to-face to online classes [
103].
EI involves the perception, processing, regulation and management of emotions. From a technical point of view applied to learning, EI is defined as a capacity-based skill that enables training in specific competences that can be directly applied to a specialized field. Otherwise said, EI can be used to address specific aspects of the teacher-student relationship [
103]. EI includes everything that is not covered by academic intelligence, such as impulse control, self-motivation and social relationships, among others [
54,
104]. Therefore, EI links the thinking part of the brain (neocortex) with the emotional part and the limbic system [
105,
106].
The application of EI has been proposed in clinical, social, educational and organizational settings [
107]. Various studies [
106,
107,
108,
109] have explored the relationship between individual differences in EI and academic outcomes. According to their results, it appears that the construct of trait EI may serve as a moderator of the relationship between intelligence and school performance.
The abilities that constitute the EI construct are multiple. However, at present two major models of EI can be found: a) ability model, which studies abilities that deal with affective information [
106,
109]; and b) mixed models that link emotional and cognitive abilities with personality [
105]. From the ability model, EI is expressed as a set of abilities to perceive, access, understand and regulate emotions to promote emotional and intellectual growth [
109]. In this context, self-awareness and self-motivation (dimensions of EI) have a direct, positive and significant impact on study habits [
110]. However, other dimensions of EI (emotion regulation and social skills) have less influence. The structure of emotional awareness is based on cognitive schemes that are different among individuals and are strictly dependent on past experiences expressed through language. Emotional awareness undergoes a structural transformation following a hierarchical development defined by five levels (in progressive order): physical sensations, action tendencies, individual emotions, mixtures of emotion and mixtures of emotional experience. Therefore, organization of emotional experiences is based on the varying complexity of emotional representations [
111,
112].
Self-awareness is reinforced by self-care behaviors such as exercise and journal writing. In general, students are an intelligent population and therefore have good study habits in any condition. Nevertheless, students with higher levels of EI have a high level of cognitive engagement in many conditions. In this sense, it has been communicated that individuals who show higher levels of EI are likely to be able to identify emotional states in themselves and others. They can use the information to better control the environment according to the situation. In this sense, EI teaches the individual to move from behaviors that seek self-gratification to ones in which gratification is received by understanding emotional needs in himself and others [
113].
6.5. The impact on talented individuals
The influence of stress and emotions has to be mentioned on those individuals with special learning qualities. These individuals organize information more accurately and efficiently than others [
114,
115]. Talented subjects are unique individuals who are endowed with special attributes or characteristics that make them develop, learn and perform skills more easily than others in similar environments [
116]. In the fields of art, sport and science, talented individuals acquire a large number of complex patterns to store new knowledge about what actions or processes should be performed for structured learning [
117]. Altogether, due to these qualities, talented individuals can manage very efficiently stress and emotions in the learning process.
7. Discussion
Stressful situations, such as the COVID-19 pandemic situation, can bring out negative emotions, resulting in disturbances in the learning process, generating anxiety and frustration in students. However, positive emotions have an amplifying character, helping to perceive and interpret everything around properly, helping to understand, learn, manage and improve life in all aspects. Therefore, the activation of coping strategies creates key mechanisms for dealing positively with problems and new challenges. Altogether, this response generates positive emotions that are instrumental for the self-regulation of learning.
Therefore, emotions can condition learning and for this reason, it is important to identify and manage emotions. Regarding the information presented in this review, we propose for future research to test some strategies to mitigate the negative impact of stress and emotions in the learning process. Techniques of meditation and mindfulness could be implemented to reduce stress and improve mental health. These techniques could help in cognitive restructuring to identify, evaluate and change wrong thinking. In addition, relaxation techniques could be useful for physical recovery. Muscular and respiratory systems are the main targets for relaxation. We propose to test in a future research a protocol in which students have to identify the moment of the day when stress appears and perform routinely relaxation techniques. Reduction of anxiety should be verified in this proposal. Finally, decipher in more detail strategies followed by talented individuals could be an interesting area for future research. In this line, an increase of attention in the study will help to manage academic situations of stress.
Regarding limitations, as a narrative review some articles related to the topic could be missed, although extensive literature has been presented. Since sex can influence feelings of anxiety and frustration, articles regarding this particular aspect were scarce and this aspect was not afforded in the review. Finally, more physiological research is necessary to correlate anxiety feelings with activation patterns of the CNS. In this context, recent research has revealed new insights into the role of the insular cortex in emotional processing and the interaction between the insula and the amygdala [
118]. Changes in these patterns after activation of coping strategies could be an interesting area for future research and pharmacological interventions.
8. Conclusions
Stress and emotions have a main influence in the learning process. COVID-19 pandemic was the more recent reference of social stress and people isolated in quarantine were at greater risk for anxiety and depression. Students were one the population segments affected in learning process. In this context, stress tends to be seen as a negative condition in the learning process because impairs memory functions. However, other evidences indicate that stress enhances memory encoding. Therefore, the role of stress in encoding information needs further investigation. In addition, learning stressors are associated with other stressors, such as family, work or sports demands. On the other hand, emotions can influence the learning process. Positive emotions are healthy and can favor optimal learning. However negative or toxic (very negative) emotions can impair significantly health and the learning process. Emotional states are regulated by a combined function of nervous, endocrine and skeletal-muscle systems, being amygdala the intercommunication structure for somatic expression of emotions. In conclusion, modulation of emotions in stress could promote effective learning through focusing attention or solving problems. This emotional self-regulation is known as emotional intelligence and it is used to address specific aspects not covered by academic intelligence. Talented individuals are a reference for future research because they can manage very efficiently stress and emotions in the learning process.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, A.C.; methodology, A.C, A.C-G. and F.D; validation, A.C, A.C-G, D.C.N, and F.D.; formal analysis, A.C, A.C-G, D.C.N, and F.D.; investigation, A.C, and, F.D.; resources, A.C-G; data curation, A.C, A.C-G. and F.D; writing—original draft preparation, A.C, and F.D..; writing—review and editing, A.C, E. R. and F.D.; visualization, A.C, A.C-G, D.C.N, and F.D.; supervision, A.C, A.C-G, D.C.N, and F.D.; funding acquisition, A.C-G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.”.
Funding
Caja Rural de Soria.
Acknowledgments
Caja Rural de Soria.
Conflicts of Interest
No conflicts.
References
- Borod, J.C. The neuropsychology of emotion; Oxford University Press: New York, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Córdova, A. Fisiología Dinámica; Masson-Elsevier: Madrid, Spain, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman, P.; Davidson, R.J. The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Scherer, K.R. What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Soc Sci Inf 2005, 44, 695–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scherer, K.R.; Schorr, A.; Johnstone, T. Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory, Methods, Research; Oxford University Press: New York, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Lange, J.; Dalege, J.; Borsboom, D.; van Kleef, G.A.; Fischer, A.H. Toward an integrative psychometric model of emotions. Perspect Psychol Sci 2020, 15, 444–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pulido, F.; Herrera, F. La influencia de las emociones sobre el rendimiento académico. Ciencias Psicológicas 2017, 11, 29–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gratz, K.L.; Roemer, L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2004, 26, 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Organización de Naciones Unidas. Cronología de la respuesta de la OMS a la COVID-19. 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/es/news/item/29-06-2020-covidtimeline.
- Heiss, R.; Gell, S.; Röthlingshöfer, E.; Zoller, C. How threat perceptions relate to learning and conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19: Evidence from a panel study. Pers Individ Differ 2021, 175, 110672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Mulukom, V.; Pummerer, L.J.; Alper, S.; Bai, H.; Cˇavojová, V.; Farias, J.; Kay, C.S.; Lazarevic, L.B.; Lobato, E.J.C.; Marinthe, G.; et al. Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: A systematic review. Soc Sci Med 2022, 301, 114912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Šrol, J.; Ballová Mikušková, E.; Cˇavojová, V. When we are worried, what are we thinking? Anxiety, lack of control, and conspiracy beliefs amidst the COVID-19 pandemic. Appl Cogn Psychol 2021, 35, 720–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jutzi, C.A.; Willardt, R.; Schmid, P.C.; Jonas, E. Between conspiracy beliefs, ingroup bias, and system justification: How people use defense strategies to cope with the threat of COVID-19. Front Psychol 2020, 11, 578586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blake, H.; Bermingham, F.; Johnson, G.; Tabner, A. Mitigating the Psychological Impact of COVID-19 on Healthcare Workers: A Digital Learning Package. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020, 17, 2997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goulia, P.; Mantas, C.; Dimitroula, D.; Mantis, D.; Hyphantis, T. General hospital staff worries, perceived sufficiency of information and associated psychological distress during the A/H1N1 influenza pandemic. BMC Infect Dis 2010, 10, 322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patel, R.S.; Bachu, R.; Adikey, A.; Malik, M.; Shah, M. Factors Related to Physician Burnout and Its Consequences: A Review. Behav Sci (Basel) 2018, 8, 98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fawaz, M.; Samaha, A. E-learning: Depression, anxiety, and stress symptomatology among Lebanese university students during COVID-19 quarantine. Nurs Forum 2021, 56, 52–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barsade, S.G.; Coutifaris, C.G.; Pillemer, J. Emotional contagion in organizational life. Res Organ Behav 2018, 38, 137–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Othman, N.; Ahmad, F.; El Morr, C.; Ritvo, P. Perceived impact of contextual determinants on depression, anxiety and stress: a survey with university students. Int J Mental Health Systems 2019, 13, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chernomas, W.M.; Shapiro, C. Stress, depression, and anxiety among undergraduate nursing students. Int J Nurs Educ Scholarsh 2013, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crary, P. Beliefs, behaviors, and health of undergraduate nursing students. Holist Nurs Pract 2013, 27, 74–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turner, K.; McCarthy, V.L. Stress and anxiety among nursing students: A review of intervention strategies in literature between 2009 and 2015. Nurse Educ Pract 2017, 22, 21–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Camargo, B. Estrés, Síndrome General de Adaptación o Reacción General de Alarma. Rev Méd Científica 2004, 17, 78–86. [Google Scholar]
- Observatoire National de la Vie Etudiante Repères sur la santé des étudiants. Published 2018. Available online: http://www.ove-national.education.fr/publication/reperes-sur-la-sante-des-etudiants/.
- Kannarkat, J.T.; Smith, N.N.; McLeod-Bryant, S.A. Mobilization of telepsychiatry in response to COVID-19—moving toward 21st century access to care. Adm Policy Ment Health 2020, 47, 489–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, M.R.; Agho, K.E.; Stevens, G.J.; Raphael, B. Factors influencing psychological distress during a disease epidemic: data from Australia’s first outbreak of equine influenza. BMC Public Health 2008, 8, 347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, C.; Pan, R.; Wan, X.; Tan, Y.; Xu, L.; Ho, C.S.; Ho, R.C. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020, 17, 1729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pires, C. Perceptions of Pharmacy Students on the E-Learning Strategies Adopted during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Systematic Review. Pharmacy (Basel) 2022, 10, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Naciri, A.; Radid, M.; Kharbach, A.; Chemsi, G. E-learning in health professions education during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review. J Educ Eval Health Prof 2021, 18, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Abbasi, S.; Ayoob, T.; Malik, A.; Memon, S.I. Perceptions of students regarding E-learning during Covid-19 at a private medical college. Pak J Med Sci 2020, 36, S57–S61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Camargo, C.P.; Tempski, P.Z.; Busnardo, F.F.; Martins, M.A.; Gemperli, R. Online learning and COVID-19: a meta-synthesis analysis. Clinics (Sao Paulo) 2020, 75, e2286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wang, X.; Hegde, S.; Son, C.; Keller, B.; Smith, A.; Sasangohar, F. Investigating Mental Health of US College Students During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Cross-Sectional Survey Study. J Med Internet Res 2020, 17, 22–e22817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Copeland, W.E.; McGinnis, E.; Bai, Y.; Adams, Z.; Nardone, H.; Devadanam, V.; Rettew, J.; Hudziak, J.J. Impact of COVID-19 Pandemic on College Student Mental Health and Wellness. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2021, 60, 134–141.e2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Odriozola-González, P.; Planchuelo-Gómez, Á.; Irurtia, M.J.; de Luis-García, R. Psychological effects of the COVID-19 outbreak and lockdown among students and workers of a Spanish university. Psychiatry Res 2020, 290, 113108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henssler, J.; Stock, F.; van Bohemen, J.; Walter, H.; Heinz, A.; Brandt, L. Mental health effects of infection containment strategies: Quarantine and isolation—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2021, 271, 223–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, L.; Jacob, L.; Yakkundi, A.; McDermott, D.; Armstrong, N.C.; Barnett, Y.; López-Sánchez, G.F.; Martin, S.; Butler, L.; Tully, M.A. Correlates of symptoms of anxiety and depression and mental wellbeing associated with COVID-19: A cross-sectional study of UK-based respondents. Psychiatry Res 2020, 291, 113138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shields, G.S.; Sazma, M.A.; McCullough, A.M.; Yonelinas, A.P. The effects of acute stress on episodic memory: A meta-analysis and integrative review. Psychol Bull 2017, 143, 636–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maheu, F.S.; Collicutt, P.; Kornik, R.; Moszkowski, R.; Lupien, S.J. The perfect time to be stressed: a differential modulation of human memory by stress applied in the morning or in the afternoon. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2005, 29, 1281–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smeets, T.; Giesbrecht, T.; Jelicic, M.; Merckelbach, H. Context-dependent enhancement of declarative memory performance following acute psychosocial stress. Biol Psychol 2007, 76, 116–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reeve, K.L.; Shumaker, C.J.; Yearwood, E.L.; Crowell, N.A.; Riley, J.B. Perceived stress and social support in undergraduate nursing students' educational experiences. Nurse Educ Today 2013, 33, 419–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Goff, AM. Stressors, academic performance, and learned resourcefulness in baccalaureate nursing students. Int J Nurs Educ Scholarsh 2011, 8, Article–1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chernomas, W.M.; Shapiro, C. Stress, depression, and anxiety among undergraduate nursing students. Int J Nurs Educ Scholarsh 2013, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner, K.; McCarthy, V.L. Stress and anxiety among nursing students: A review of intervention strategies in literature between 2009 and 2015. Nurse Educ Pract 2017, 22, 21–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arnsten, A.F. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009, 10, 410–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shansky, R.M.; Lipps, J. Stress-induced cognitive dysfunction: Hormone-neurotransmitter interactions in the prefrontal cortex. Front Hum Neurosci 2013, 7, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, A.P.; Kennedy, P.J.; Cryan, J.F.; Dinan, T.G.; Clarke, G. Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: focus on the Trier Social Stress Test. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014, 38, 94–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Joëls, M.; Fernandez, G.; Roozendaal, B. Stress and emotional memory: a matter of timing. Trends Cogn Sci 2011, 15, 280–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thoma, M.V.; Kirschbaum, C.; Wolf, J.M.; Rohleder, N. Acute stress responses in salivary alpha-amylase predict increases of plasma norepinephrine. Biol Psychol 2012, 91, 342–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- de Quervain, D.J.; Aerni, A.; Roozendaal, B. Preventive effect of beta-adrenoceptor blockade on glucocorticoid-induced memory retrieval deficits. Am J Psychiatry 2007, 164, 967–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roozendaal, B.; Okuda, S.; de Quervain, D.J.; McGaugh, J.L. Glucocorticoids interact with emotion-induced noradrenergic activation in influencing different memory functions. Neuroscience 2006, 138, 901–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schwabe, L.; Joëls, M.; Roozendaal, B.; Wolf, O.T.; Oitzl, M.S. Stress effects on memory: an update and integration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2012, 36, 1740–1749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Patel, P.D.; Katz, M.; Karssen, A.M.; Lyons, D.M. Stress-induced changes in corticosteroid receptor expression in primate hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008, 33, 360–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ekman, P. ¿Qué dice ese gesto? RBA: Barcelona, Spain, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Goleman, D. La inteligencia emocional; Kairos: Barcelona, Spain, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman, P. Are there basic emotions? Psychol Rev 1992, 99, 550–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martínez-Sarmiento, L.F.; González, M.L.G. Utilización de la plataforma virtual Moodle para el desarrollo del aprendizaje autorregulado en estudiantes universitarios. Educar 2019, 55, 479–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adolphs, R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2002, 12, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bisquerra, R. Educación emocional y competencias básicas para la vida. Rev Invest Educ 2003, 21, 7–43. [Google Scholar]
- Lane, A.M.; Terry, P.C. The Nature of Mood: Development of a Conceptual Model with a Focus on Depression. J App Sport Psychol 2000, 12, 16–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaeta, M.L.; Gaeta, L.; Rodriguez, M.S. The Impact of COVID-19 Home Confinement on Mexican University Students: Emotions, Coping Strategies, and Self-Regulated Learning. Front Psychol 2021, 12, 642823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bisquerra, R. Psicopedagogía de las emociones; Síntesis: Madrid, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson, B.L. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am Psychol 2001, 56, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wooden, J.; Jamison, S. A lifetime of observations and reflections on and off the court; 1st Ed, Contemporary Books: New York, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Kandel, E.R.; Schwartz, J.H.; Jessell, T.M. Principles of neural science; McGraw-Hill: New York, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Damasio, A. The Strange Order of Things; Penguin Books: New York, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman, L.; Lewis, M.; Haviland-Jones, JM. Handbook of emotions; Guilford Press: New York, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh, J.L. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu Rev Neurosci 2004, 27, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rosenzweig, M.R.; Leiman, A.L.; Marc-Breedlove, S. Psicobiología: Neurociencia Conductual, Cognitiva y Clínica; Ariel: Barcelona, Spain, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Rains, D.G. Principios de neuropsicología humana; McGraw-Hill: México DF, México, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, M.; Chiba, A.A. The amygdala and emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1996, 6, 221–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ledoux, J.E. Cognitive-Emotional Interactions in the Brain. Cognition & Emotion 1989, 3, 267–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kleinginna, P.R.; Kleinginna, A.M. A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motiv Emot 1981, 5, 345–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trull, T.J.; Phares, E.J. Psicología Clínica. Conceptos, métodos y aspectos prácticos de la profesión; Thompson: Madrid, Spain, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein, S. Coaching as a Treatment for ADHD. J Atten Disord 2005, 9, 379–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ashby, F.G.; Isen, A.M.; Turken, A.U. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol Rev 1999, 106, 529–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cacioppo, J.T.; Tassinary, L.G.; Bernston, GG. Handbook of Psychophysiology; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Torras, M.; Portell, I.; Morgado, I. La amígdala: implicaciones funcionales [The amigdaloid body: functional implications]. Rev Neurol 2001, 33, 471–476. [Google Scholar]
- Sánchez-Navarro, J.P.; Román, F. Amígdala, corteza prefrontal y especialización hemisférica en la experiencia y expresión emocional. Ann Psychol 2004, 20, 223–240. [Google Scholar]
- Reeves, A. Emotional intelligence: recognizing and regulating emotions. AAOHN J 2005, 53, 172–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reeve, J.M. Motivación y emoción; McGraw-Hill/Interamericana: México DF, México, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Chóliz, M. Psicología de la emoción: El proceso emocional; Universidad de Valencia: Valencia, Spain, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M.C.; Saletti-Cuesta, L.; Tumas, N. Emociones, preocupaciones y reflexiones frente a la pandemia del COVID-19 en Argentina. Ciênc saúde coletiva 2020, 25, 2447–2456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roy, D.; Tripathy, S.; Kar, S.K.; Sharma, N.; Verma, S.K.; Kaushal, V. Study of knowledge, attitude, anxiety & perceived mental healthcare need in Indian population during COVID-19 pandemic. Asian J Psychiatr 2020, 51, 102083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alcalá-Albert, G.J.; García-Carpintero, E.; Gómez-Moreno, C.; González-Morón, C.; Sanz-Melero, A.; Robledillo-Mesa, A.S.; Vélez-Vélez, E. Back to Clinical Training during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Perspective of Nursing Students. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022, 19, 14242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Masha’al, D.; Shahrour, G.; Aldalaykeh, M. Anxiety and coping strategies among nursing students returning to university during the COVID-19 pandemic. Heliyon 2022, 8, e08734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eckenrode, J. Impact of chronic and acute stressors on daily reports of mood. J Personality Social Psychol 1984, 46, 907–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buitrago, F.; Ciurana, R.; Fernández-Alonso, M.; Tizón, J.L. Pandemia de la COVID-15 y salud mental: reflexiones iniciales desde la atención primaria de salud española. Aten Primaria 2021, 53, 89–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Damasio, A. En busca de Spinoza. Neurobiología de la emoción y los sentimientos; Crítica: Barcelona, Spain, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Shields, G.S.; Sazma, M.A.; Yonelinas, A.P. The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: A meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neurosc Biobehav Rev 2016, 68, 651–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwabe, L.; Tegenthoff, M.; Höffken, O.; Wolf, O.T. Simultaneous Glucocorticoid and Noradrenergic Activity Disrupts the Neural Basis of Goal-Directed Action in the Human Brain. J Neurosci 2012, 32, 10146–10155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmeichel, B.J. Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. J Exp Psychol Gen 2007, 136, 241–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hagger, M.S.; Wood, C.; Stiff, C.; Chatzisarantis, N.L. Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2010, 136, 495–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maren, S. Long-term potentiation in the amygdala: a mechanism for emotional le1rning and memory. Trends Neurosci 1999, 22, 561–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rogan, M.T.; Stäubli, U.V.; LeDoux, J.E. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature 1997, 390, 604–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laredo, S.A.; Steinman, M.Q.; Robles, C.F.; Ferrer, E.; Ragen, B.J.; Trainor, B.C. Effects of defeat stress on behavioral flexibility in males and females: modulation by the mu-opioid receptor. Eur J Neurosci 2015, 41, 434–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cole, P.M.; Martin, S.E.; Dennis, T.A. Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Develop 2004, 75, 317–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salovey, P.; Mayer, J.D. Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition & Personality 1990, 9, 185–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lopes, P.N.; Brackett, M.A.; Nezlek, J.B.; Schütz, A.; Sellin, I.; Salovey, P. Emotional intelligence and social interaction. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2004, 30, 1018–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mikolajczak, M.; Menil, C.; Luminet, O. Explaining the protective effect of trait emotional intelligence regarding occupational stress: Exploration of emotional labour processes. J Res Personality 2007, 41, 1107–1117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jan, S.U.; Anwar, M.A. Emotional Intelligence, Library Use and Academic achievement of University Students. J Australian Library Information Ass 2019, 68, 38–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vega-Hernández, M.C.; Patino-Alonso, M.C.; Cabello, R.; Galindo-Villardón, M.P.; Fernández-Berrocal, P. Perceived emotional intelligence and learning strategies in Spanish university students: A new perspective from a canonical non-symmetrical correspondence analysis. Front Psychol 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Xue, E.; Li, J.; Li, T.; Shang, W. China’s education response to COVID-19: A perspective of policy analysis. Educ Philosophy Theory 2021, 53, 881–893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphrey, N.; Curran, A.; Morris, E.; Farrell, P.; Woods, K. Emotional Intelligence and Education: A critical review. Educl Psychol 2007, 27, 235–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron, R.M. The Bar-On model of emotional-social intelligence (ESI). Psicothema 2006, 18 (Extra 1), 13–25. [Google Scholar]
- Petrides, K.V.; Furnham, A. The Role of Trait Emotional Intelligence in a Gender-Specific Model of Organizational Variables. J App Social Psychol 2006, 36, 552–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, J.D.; Salovey, P.; Caruso, D. Models of Emotional Intelligence. In Handbook of Intelligence; Sternberg RJ, Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 396–420. [Google Scholar]
- Agnoli, S.; Mancini, G.; Pozzoli, T.; Baldaro, B.; Russo, P.M.; Surcinelli, P. The interaction between emotional intelligence and cognitive ability in predicting scholastic performance in school-aged children. Personality Individual Differences 2012, 53, 660–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mavroveli, S.; Petrides, K.V.; Sangareau, Y.; Furnham, A. Exploring the relationships between trait emotional intelligence and objective socio-emotional outcomes in childhood. Br J Educ Psychol 2009, 79, 259–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hansenne, M.; Legrand, J. Creativity, emotional intelligence, and school performance in children. Int J Educ Res 2014, 53, 264–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iqbal, J.; Asghar, M.Z.; Ashraf, M.A.; Yi, X. The Impacts of Emotional Intelligence on Students' Study Habits in Blended Learning Environments: The Mediating Role of Cognitive Engagement during COVID-19. Behav Sci (Basel) 2022, 12, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lane, R.D.; Quinlan, D.M.; Schwartz, G.E.; Walker, P.A.; Zeitlin, S.B. The Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale: a cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. J Pers Assess 1990, 55, 124–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lane, R.D.; Schwartz, G.E. Levels of emotional awareness: a cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry 1987, 144, 133–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, D.R. Emotional intelligence as a crucial component to medical education. Int J Med Educ 2015, 6, 179–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simon, H.A.; Chase, W.G. Skill in chess. American Scientist 1973, 61, 394–403. [Google Scholar]
- Newell, A.; Rosembloom, P.S. Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In Cognitive skills and their acquisition; Anderson, J., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1981; pp. 1–55. [Google Scholar]
- Drobnic, F.; Figueroa, J. Talented, expertise, or both? Apunts Medicina de l'Esport 2007, 42, 186–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortega-de San Luis, C.; Ryan, T.J. Understanding the physical basis of memory: Molecular mechanisms of the engram. J Biol Chem 2022, 298, 101866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Šimić, G.; Tkalčić, M.; Vukić, V.; Mulc, D.; Španić, E.; Šagud, M.; Olucha-Bordonau, F.E.; Vukšić, M.R.; Hof, P. Understanding Emotions: Origins and Roles of the Amygdala. Biomolecules 2021, 11, 823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1.
Emotional aspects. Relationship between functions, components and organ systems.
Table 1.
Emotional aspects. Relationship between functions, components and organ systems.
Emotional function |
Emotional component |
Physiological function (organism systems) |
Event evaluation |
Cognitive component |
Information processing (CNS) |
System regulation |
Neurophysiological component |
Support (CNS, ANS, NES) |
Action preparation and direction |
Motivational component (action tendencies) |
Executive (CNS) |
Communication of reaction and behavioral intention |
Motor expression component (facial and vocal expression) |
Action (SNS) |
Monitoring of internal state and organism-environment interaction |
Subjective feeling component (emotional experience) |
Monitoring (CNS) |
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).