1. Introduction
Specific Language Disorder (SLI) or Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) is a developmental disorder characterized by a set of difficulties that affect language acquisition [
1]. This disorder manifests itself as an important limitation in the expression and/or comprehension of oral language [
2], which affects communicative practices in speech and language processing [
3,
4]. The most frequent deficits in SLI/DLD are related to the morphosyntactic level of language [
5,
6,
7,
8,
9]. These morphosyntactic difficulties are considered a clinical marker for the diagnosis of the disorder [
10]. Morphosyntactic problems of this kind have been observed in several languages [
8,
11], although most of the information has been obtained from English-speaking children with SLI/DLD [
12].
Syntactic complexity in Spanish-speaking children with SLI/DLD
Several studies on sentence complexity across different languages indicate that it poses a significant challenge for children with SLI/DLD [
13,
14,
15,
16,
17,
18,
19,
20]. Research on Spanish-speaking children with SLI/DLD has found that they exhibit difficulties in both sentence comprehension and production, particularly in the area of complex sentences [
13,
14,
21]. Moreover, production difficulties seem to be more pronounced as sentence complexity increases [
22]. These challenges are also more prevalent in contexts that require the use of more sophisticated linguistic resources, such as narrative discourse production [
23,
24].
Principio del formulario
Many studies on morphosyntactic complexity in children with SLI/DLD have focused on analyzing their language samples in terms of simple sentences and those related to coordination and subordination mechanisms. It has been found that compared to coordinated sentences, children with SLI/DLD have fewer problems with simple sentences [
22]. Furthermore, it has been suggested that these children tend to use simple sentences more frequently than complex ones [
25] and prefer using simple and coordinated sentences over complex sentences [
14,
17,
22,
26].
The analysis of syntactic complexity in children with SLI/DLD considers different perspectives on the concept of complexity. Some studies include coordinated sentences in their description of compound sentences, which they consider as constitutive of complexity. In this regard, it has been observed that children with SLI/DLD use significantly fewer compound sentences than their typically developing peers [
13]. Other studies focus only on subordination, which involves integrating one clause within another [
25,
27,
28]. From this perspective, it has been found that children with SLI/DLD have lower production of subordinate clauses than typically developing children [
14,
16,
21,
29,
30]. However, some studies have not found these differences to be statistically significant, at least in narrative language samples [
25,
28].
Longitudinal development and trajectory of syntactic complexity in children with typical development (DT) and with SLI/DLD
The use of subordinate clauses as a mechanism of syntactic complexity in children's linguistic development highlights their ability to produce sentences that are dependent on others [
31]. Typically developing children begin to use two or more verbs in a sentence around 2 years of age, with complex syntax emerging around 30 months; however, the structures and functions involved in complexity are not fully consolidated until after three years of age [
19,
28]. Although children with typical development show some level of complexity in their statements before the age of 4 [
32], the use of coordinated clauses predominates in preschool age [
19]. By 6 years of age, they handle syntactically more complex linguistic structures than in preschool years [
33], although simple sentences are still used more frequently than complex sentences. In the first years of schooling, from age 6 onwards, the use of subordination increases significantly [
34]. Finally, studies indicate that typically developing children use more complex clauses at 10 years than at 8 years [
35].
On the other hand, children with SLI/DLD preferably use simple sentences between 4 and 6 years of age [
17], although various indicators of complex syntax were also found at 5;9 years in a longitudinal case study [
20]. It has also been observed that these children use significantly fewer compound sentences in their narratives between the ages of 4 to 11 [
13]. Furthermore, Pavez et al. [
36] and Coloma et al. [
37] showed that, at 6 years of age, children with SLI/DLD maintained the same level of production of complex structures as a 4-year-old control group in narrative samples. Regarding conversation, it has been suggested that both, children with SLI/DLD and those with language delay, produce fewer complex structures than their typically developing peers [
16,
38]. In this regard, Hincapié-Henao et al. [
21] state that children with SLI/DLD have great difficulty in producing complex verbally formulated structures. Among these structures, constructions that reflect hypotactic relationships with time-related, final, and comparative value are also especially challenging [
22].
Describing the development of language longitudinally allows, on the one hand, to determine different growth patterns or trajectories that could define characteristics of -typical and atypical- language [
20,
39,
40] and, on the other, to investigate in greater depth the issue of the persistence of SLI/DLD difficulties over time [
41]. Law and Tomblin [
40] mention three hypotheses that explain the possible development patterns of language skills in children with SLI/DLD: 1) they coincide at the same starting point and diverge over time ("deterioration hypothesis"); 2) they develop at the same speed, but stop at a certain point, without further development ("plateau hypothesis"); and 3) they take off later, but their language development, although delayed, parallels that of typical development (“tracking hypothesis”). According to Law and Tomblin [
40], a reduced heterogeneity is observed in the growth characteristics of children with language disorders, a trajectory that would be similar to that of children with typical language development, at least in the school years, which would be consistent with the explanation of the tracking hypothesis. This finding coincides with the position that, although these children would be delayed, they would not be qualitatively different from children with TD [
42]. In this regard, it has also been suggested that children with TD and SLI/DLD would follow a similar path, although children with SLI/DLD obtain lower results [
41]. However, it is necessary to emphasize, at this point, that the competence and speed of development in children with SLI/DLD is lower than in children with typical development with respect to the emergence of syntactic complexity at the beginning of these trajectories [
27,
37], and that the specific characteristics of this starting point provides evidence that widespread vulnerabilities in complex syntax acquisition could typify SLI/DLD [
20].
Approaches for the description of syntactic complexity in children with TD and SLI/DLD
Various approaches have been utilized to assess complexity in children with typical development and SLI/DLD. These methods offer distinct viewpoints and outcomes, with some focusing on quantitative aspects and others on qualitative aspects. Quantitatively, syntactic complexity has been evaluated based on the average length of specific units, such as sentences, clauses, or utterances. Brown [
43] proposed the Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) measurement for analyzing children's language in early developmental stages, which is systematically related to age and accounts for the development of syntactic maturity [
44]. This index has been used in studies on various languages, including Spanish [
45], English [
46], and Portuguese [
47], in children with TD and SLI/DLD. The MLU has been linked to other measures of complexity, enabling researchers to observe that older children produce longer linguistic units containing more clauses [
31]. Combining the MLU with other indices of ungrammaticality has facilitated the identification of children with language difficulties [
48]. Additionally, measuring the MLU in bilingual children with TD (English-Spanish) has been predictive of their language skills in an English narrative retelling test, although the same result was not obtained in Spanish [
49].
An alternative approach to measuring syntactic complexity involves focusing on clauses and their relationships, as proposed by Hunt [
50,
51] (1965, 1970). According to this generativist perspective, a terminal unit (T-unit) is composed of a main clause and any attached or embedded clauses or non-clausal structures, representing both paratactic (juxtaposed and coordinated clauses) and hypotactic (subordinate clauses) relationships. This method allows for the quantitative growth of T-units to be visualized as a child's development becomes more complex, with indices increasing in parallel with age, schooling, and intellectual level [
51].
Describing language samples of children with SLI/DLD has frequently employed the quantification and structural analysis of simple, coordinated, and subordinate sentences as a means of characterizing language complexity [
13,
14,
21,
25,
27,
28,
29]. Within this tradition, sentence complexity is typically interpreted based on the presence or absence of different types of sentences, with complexity often referring to utterances that exceed the limits of a clause [
28].
Another approach to measuring language complexity involves a qualitative analysis of language samples, focusing not only on syntax, but also on thematic and discursive criteria. In this tradition, researchers have analyzed the distribution and organization of clauses based on interclausal relationships, which can be classified as isotactic, symmetrical and asymmetrical paratactic, hypotactic, and endotactic. For example, Alfaro, Crespo, and Alvarado [
26] studied these relationships in a narrative sample of children with SLI/DLD and typically developing children. They found that the SLI/DLD group produced more paratactic relationships than the TD group, although this difference was not statistically significant. This finding suggests that the SLI/DLD group produced less informative texts, indicating lower complexity.
From a systemic-functional perspective, the interpretation of the role of interdependence between clauses (parataxis and hypotaxis) is different. In this approach, clauses, which are considered as the central unit of grammatical meaning, are capable of expressing different types of meaning simultaneously and their function is explained by how they work together [
52]. The logical system of "taxis" (from the Greek, order, arrangement, category) captures the relationship of dependency and interdependence between adjacent clauses, which may be potentially part of different types of clause complexes [
53]. At the semantic level, clause complexes illustrate how a flow of events develops and becomes a text [
54]. According to this theoretical perspective, differences in the structure of oral or written texts reflect differences of a semantic nature [
52,
55].
This research will use the systemic-functional perspective to characterize the development of clause interdependence relationships in a corpus of Spanish-speaking children with SLI/DLD, compared to a group of children with typical development (TD), from a longitudinal perspective. To the best of our knowledge, this type of analysis has not been previously employed to examine this population. Thus, the findings of this study will contribute to providing information on syntactic complexity from a novel perspective. The following research questions will guide this investigation:
What is the most frequently used type of clause interdependence in both groups?
Do children with SLI/DLD exhibit similar patterns of development in different types of clause interdependence compared to children with TD, from a longitudinal perspective?
Are there significant differences in the use of clause interdependence relationships between children with SLI/DLD and TD in each grade, from a cross-sectional perspective?
Our findings indicate that both groups, throughout all grades of primary education, predominantly utilize parataxis as the primary relation between clauses. However, there is a notable decline in the use of paratactic relations and a substantial increase in the use of hypotactic relations from first to fourth grade. Although the developmental patterns are largely similar, the SLI/DLD group faces greater challenges in acquiring more intricate (hypotactic) relations by fourth grade, in contrast to the control group. This suggests that they exhibit less proficiency in employing these complex types of relations.
3. Results
Table 4 presents detailed descriptions of all measures for both groups.
Figure 1 complements the table by displaying the means and confidence intervals for each measure, along with the results of the Tukey post-hoc contrasts at each level, including the corresponding groupings of means.
In relation to the first question of the study, the results indicate that time-related parataxis is the most commonly used resource by both groups, albeit with some differences. Among children with typical development (TD), all three clause types differ significantly from each other in first and second grade, while by fourth grade, the differences between time-related parataxis and hypotaxis are no longer significant. Among children with specific language impairment/developmental language disorder (SLI/DLD), the three types of clauses are significantly different from each other in second and fourth grade, but not in first grade, where the production of hypotactic and paratactic relations does not differ significantly. Additional details on the mean differences and their grouping can be found in
Appendix A and
Appendix B.
Regarding the second question of the research, post-hoc contrasts revealed that the evolutionary pattern of time-related paratactic relationships is highly similar in both groups. In both cases, the use of these relationships decreases significantly from a high level in first grade to a lower level in fourth grade (with a slightly more marked decrease in the case of children with TD). For hypotactic relationships, identical mean clusters were observed for both groups. However, in contrast to the findings for time-related paratactic relationships, the percentage of production significantly increased between first and fourth grade. Finally, no substantial differences were observed between the groups in terms of paratactic relationships, with a low percentage of production in both groups and a slight difference in second grade.
Figure 2 displays the means of both groups across each school grade, providing a breakdown of the results according to the three types of interclausal relationships.
In relation to the third research question, the results of the Wilcoxon tests indicated that there were statistically significant differences between the SLI/DLD and TD groups in terms of time-related paratactic relationships in both second (W=165.5, p=0.01) and fourth grade (W=185.5, p=0.04). Significant differences in the use of paratactic relations were only observed in second grade (W=435.5, p <0.001), while significant differences in hypotactic relationships were only observed in fourth grade (W=386, p=0.04).
Overall, the results indicate that the relative importance of clause types is similar between the two groups. Specifically, paratactic relationships exhibit little change across grade levels, with mean percentages of usage remaining low (around 15% for most grades, and slightly above 20% for only one grade) in both groups. However, significant differences were observed between the groups and in the changes observed throughout the school grades in the percentages of production of time-related paratactic relationships and hypotactic relationships. In both groups, significant changes were observed in the percentages of production between first and fourth grade, indicating an increase in the production of more complex relationships and less use of simpler relationships. Nonetheless, children with SLI/DLD showed greater difficulties in mastering more complex relationships compared to TD children, as both types of clauses showed significant differences in fourth grade. Therefore, the results suggest that although the developmental patterns are highly similar, the performance of children with SLI/DLD does not reach the same level of sophistication as children with TD in fourth grade.
4. Discussion
In relation to the first question of this study, which concerns the type of clause interdependence relationship that occurs most frequently in each group, a clear predominance of paratactic relationships can be observed among clauses related in a time-related manner through the conjunction "and". This predominance begins to diminish as hypotactic relationships increase towards the fourth degree in both groups, although parataxis remains the type of relationship with the greatest presence in the examined corpus. The high presence of the conjunction "and" in children's speech can be attributed to its functionality as a predominant coordination link, which serves to express various types of relationships [
67]. This feature could account for its frequent use in the analyzed corpora, particularly in language samples of children with SLI/DLD. Alternatively, the greater frequency of simple structures coordinated by "and" could be attributed to a semantic mechanism used by children with SLI/DLD to compensate for their syntactic deficit, as suggested by van der Lely and Marshall [
68].
However, since both groups show the same pattern of use, it is important to question how much of this result is directly linked to the semantic content of the stories used. Alarcón and Auzá [
69] obtained opposite results to those observed in the present study, finding a greater use of subordination than coordination in a recounting task among first grade children with DT. They observed that the original story of their task favored the explicitation of multiple conditional and causal relationships, expressed through relationships of dominance and syntactic dependence, which could have had an impact on their results. Nevertheless, most studies with a structural focus coincide with the results of our study, in that coordination relationships and simple syntax are more commonly observed in narrative corpora, particularly in narrative samples of children with SLI/DLD [
13,
16,
17,
21,
38].
In relation to the second question of this study, it was possible to corroborate that children with SLI/DLD and TD follow a similar developmental pattern in the different types of clause interdependence relationships from first to fourth grade, although the results of the SLI/DLD group remain below those of the control group. This finding is significant, as there is insufficient information about how the grammar problems of these children evolve over time [
27]. It should be noted that one of the main characteristics of this disorder is the persistence over time of difficulty in acquiring and developing oral language [
70]. Although the distinction between disorder and delay is yet to have empirical evidence [
2], the persistence of performance below the levels reached by the control group in this study seems to indicate a language development disorder [
1], despite the progress observed in all aspects analyzed.
Through a longitudinal analysis of the data from both groups, two key aspects have been identified: 1) the similarity in the pattern of development between the SLI/DLD and control groups, and 2) the within-group advances that enable the detection of significant changes from one school grade to the next. In relation to the first aspect, previous research has noted that the development pattern of the SLI/DLD group is similar to that of the control group, although there is a gap in each of the measures analyzed. This finding is consistent with other studies that have reported lower competence and slower development in children with SLI/DLD compared to typically developing children [
37,
41,
71]. The "tracking hypothesis" proposed by Law, Tomblin, and Zhang [
40] posits that children with SLI/DLD may experience delayed language development compared to their typically developing peers, but that their language development follows a similar trajectory, at least during the school years. This finding aligns with the notion that, although children with SLI/DLD may be delayed in language acquisition, they are not qualitatively different from typically developing children [
42], which is also supported by Leonard [
8] who suggests that their acquisition process is part of normal language development.
Secondly, the longitudinal view allows us to observe significant changes from one school grade to the next, particularly in relation to time-related paratactic and hypotactic relationships. The results indicate that there is significantly less use of time-related parataxis and significantly more use of hypotaxis towards fourth grade within each group. However, a statistically significant difference is observed in the TD group's transition from 2nd to 4th grade, which is different from the behavior of the SLI/DLD group. These results are consistent with previous research, which suggests that children with SLI/DLD demonstrate an early emergence of some aspects of syntactic complexity, but their performance in terms of competence is lower compared to control groups, and their rate of development is different [
71]. These different patterns of change, which vary depending on the dimension being considered, have also been found in other longitudinal studies of children with SLI/DLD [
72].
On the other hand, the explosive increase in hypotactic relations in children with TD from second to fourth grade is related to their increasing capacity to produce complex and diversified sentences with age during childhood [
31,
35]. The schooling process also poses communicative challenges that have a significant impact on the development and consolidation of new hypotactic complex forms, such as final and concessive subordinate structures, which require mastery of the subjunctive [
19] (Serra et al., 2000).
In summary, this longitudinal intra-group perspective enables us to appreciate more subtle changes and differences, such as those that occur with SLI/DLD children in their transition from second to fourth grade.
On the other hand, when considering the possibility of significant differences between groups in each school grade, the most relevant results reveal two interesting findings: 1) all groups behave similarly in first grade according to the measures analyzed, and 2) by fourth grade, the SLI/DLD group tends to use significantly more simple interclausal relationships (such as time-related parataxis), while the DT group tends to use more complex interclausal relationships (such as hypotaxis). On the other hand, a specific pattern emerges in second grade. While the SLI/DLD group shows similar hypotactic relationships to the DT group, they differ in their use of simpler relationships. It has been suggested that by this age, typically developing children fully develop their grammatical skills [
19]. A longitudinal study with a SLI/DLD group found similar developmental patterns at age 7 [
40]. Between first and second grade, significant advances in grammatical skills are observed, particularly in the length and complexity of sentence production [
31,
73].
In conclusion, the SLI/DLD group exhibits a developmental pattern similar to that of the control group in all aspects analyzed across their transition from first, second, and fourth grades (longitudinal perspective), but a distinct one when comparing the groups with each other in each school grade. The most significant result from this latter perspective pertains to the SLI/DLD group's tendency, in fourth grade, to continue using simpler interclausal relationships, as compared to the DT group, which shows significantly more complexity in the way it interrelates clauses at the same school grade.
The problems that clausal complexity can cause in school-aged children can be related, from a functional perspective, to the academic demands that they must comprehend and produce when they enter school [
74]. The increasing complexity demands of the school curriculum at 6 and 7 years of age determine the use of increasingly complex grammatical structures [
69]. When comprehension problems arise in the classroom, it is common to underestimate the complexity of academic language used with children in this setting, and to associate these difficulties solely with the content of the classes rather than the way in which they are taught [
75]. In this sense, it is not possible to isolate language learning from all other aspects of learning [
76]. The complexity that characterizes the language that children are expected to use in school includes many structural levels at the word, sentence, and text levels [
77]. Therefore, children need to learn to process these extremely complex structures to function successfully in school.
In sum, the findings presented in this study regarding the development of clause complexity have important implications for educational practice, particularly considering that grammatical abilities play a crucial role as foundational tools for various challenging school activities faced by children with SLI/DLD. This longitudinal study provides valuable insights into the developmental trajectory of children with SLI/DLD compared to typically developing children in their utilization of more complex structures, specifically hypotactic relations. It is observed that children with SLI/DLD exhibit a similar developmental pattern to their TD peers in the use of these structures; however, they consistently perform significantly below the control group, particularly by fourth grade.
Based on these findings, it is crucial for grammatical interventions in educational settings to consider two key aspects. Firstly, during first grade, children with and without SLI/DLD exhibit similar usage of interclausal relations; however, this similarity does not guarantee subsequent adequate performance. Therefore, educational support at this stage should ensure an equal level of contents for all students, while also providing targeted preventive attention to children with SLI/DLD, even if their performance does not yet significantly differ from their TD classmates. This support could be implemented through the utilization of more implicit teaching techniques, as metalinguistic abilities are still underdeveloped at this age. Secondly, given the significant differences observed by fourth grade, it is essential to address the challenge faced in both educational and clinical practice: providing specialized support to address these difficulties before children with SLI/DLD reach this stage. These findings also raise questions about the effectiveness of current support offered to students with DLD during the initial years of schooling and, more importantly, challenge the prevailing notion of SLI/DLD as a temporary condition, which in some countries limits institutional support beyond the age of 9.