1. Introduction
Sorghum is a potentially more sustainable crop than other grains due to the plant’s resilience to dry, hot climates [
1,
2,
3]. Therefore, new, palatable products need to be developed to improve both food availability and nutrition-security. Compared to other cereal grains, sorghum may provide more nutrition-security because of greater concentrations of phenolic compounds [
4,
5]. Phenolic compounds possess significant antioxidant abilities that modulate chronic inflammation [
6], prevent cancerous cells proliferation [
7], and may even improve satiety during weight loss programs involving tea-based products [
8].
Nonetheless, sorghum strains with high concentrations of phenolic compounds (e.g., red or black sorghum) may contribute to increased bitterness in products because of the associated increased tannins [
5,
9,
10]. Furthermore, when sorghum is heated, it decreases protein bioavailability in cooked sorghum, which may be caused by phenolic compounds binding to proteins [
5,
11]. These drawbacks may be remedied through the process of fermentation. [
12,
13,
14]. For example, sorghum can be cultured to produce potentially beneficial by-products and increased protein bioavailability and the fermentation of the liquid may be palatable to adults [
15,
16,
17].
For this current project, fermented sorghum-based tea was created because of the potential to have high concentrations of antioxidants and probiotics, thus providing a nutrition-secure beverage. Although sorghum-teas and fermented-teas originated in northeast China around 220 B.C [
18,
19], fermented teas are new to the other countries. The U.S. Kombucha is a recent example of a commercialized fermented tea, which is produced by first seeping black, green, or oolong tea leaves, then, adding sugar and a symbiotic culture of bacteria and yeast (SCOBY) [
18,
20]. These same methods could be applied to sorghum, but with Kombucha, the fermentation results in sour flavors and astringent mouthfeel [
17,
20]. A major influence of kombucha flavor is the organic acid content produced by the microorganisms within the SCOBY. Specifically, malic, gallic, and lactic acids contribute to added sour flavors and dry mouth feel, which also are associated with red wines even though these compounds contribute to the antioxidant properties of fermented beverages [
20,
21,
22].
The novel flavors and mouth feel of fermented beverages are often appreciated by adult humans; The current authors will use adult pigs as a biomedical model to understand palatability and health benefits of fermented sorghum tea. However, previous research shows that pigs may reject sour or bitter flavors. Nevertheless, most preference research has been conducted on either young commercial pigs at weaning, or ad libitum fed sows that have inappetence after parturition [
23,
24,
25,
26]. There is a lack of research publications on flavor preference in adult pigs that must be limit fed. Therefore, the objectives of this study were: 1) Assess adult pig preference among three types of formulated sorghum teas, and 2) compare the preference or avoidance behaviors among choices of blended fermented sorghum tea, positive control isocaloric sugar water, and negative control water. The hypothesis was that adult pigs would prefer the sorghum tea with the least amount of gallic acid and the isocaloric control (sugar water) over a blend of fermented sorghum teas.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Animals and housing
This study followed the guidelines set forth by the Guide for the Care and Use of Agricultural Animals in Agriculture and Teaching (CorVus IACUC #4745). The animal portion of the project was conducted between May and June 2022. Twelve adult Ossabaw gilts and barrows (age 15.6 ± 0.62 SD months; 53.5 ± 0.62 kg body weight) were pair-housed by gender at indoor, climate-controlled facilities (21°C; CorVus Biomedical, LLC, Crawfordsville, Indiana). Artificial lighting was a 12-h cycle, with lights on from 0800 to 2000 h. One month prior to the start of the experiment, pig pairs were randomized into one of six pens that were stratified by gender (gilts and barrows). Pens provided 14.92 m2 of free space with plastic slotted flooring (Maxima Sow Slat Floor, Greenfield, WI USA). The gating for the pens were 94.0 cm tall and custom made with vertical standard stainless bars (Thorp Equipment, Thorp, WI USA). Two feeders (S3 Series Dry Sow Feeder, Crystal Spring Hog Equipment Agathe, Manitoba CA) were provided on each side of the pen with a drinker (ad libitum well water; QC Swine Water Cup System, Delphi, IN USA) next to one of feeders. Environmental enrichment consisted of two combined stainless-steel chains, with one link fastened near the drinker. Pigs were fed a total of 750 g feed per day to maintain lean body type (15% Crude Protein, 3% Crude Fat; 5% Fiber; DuMOR Hog Grower, Tractor Supply, Brentwood, TN USA). Pig pairs were randomly assigned to pen using the RAND function of Excel (Microsoft Excel, Version 10; Redmond, WA). Then, individual pigs within each pen were randomized for exposure order to the testing arena.
2.2. Formulation of solutions
Three varieties of sorghum (Nu Life, Scott City, KS) were used for this study: raw-white, raw-sumac, and roasted sumac strains. To create the sorghum tea (
Figure 1), commercially available (for human consumption) sorghum was steeped in sterile water at room temperature for 16 hours. Then, the supernatant was recovered by filtering and brought to a boil before sugar was dissolved (
Figure 1). Once boiled, 2 cups of supernatant (473 mL) were added to a jar with 1 cup of sugar (200 grams) and 8 cups (1892 mL) of deionized water. Afterwards, the solution was cooled to room temperature, and inoculated with a commercially available culture (Symbiotic Culture of Bacteria and Yeast, SCOBY; The Kombucha Shop, Madison, WI USA). Then, the solution fermented for 7 days at room temperature. Fermentation was then stalled by refrigeration. For this current experiment, the solution was refrigerated for 72 h before use. The pH of the fermented solutions were monitored and averaged 3.12 ±0.12 SD. An isocaloric (0.288 calories per mL), positive control (+control) was created with sugar (sucrose) and boiled distilled water. The negative control (-control) was distilled water only. Each batch of solution was sampled and frozen at -20 ⁰C for gallic acid analyses. Gallic analyses was determined using the Folin Ciocalteu Method as described previously [
27,
28,
29,
30].
2.3. Preference Testing
A commercially available, 3 compartment (i.e. pod) preference maze was used for the preference test (3-PodShape, by Hulbert and ShapeMaster, Odgen, IL USA;
Figure 2; VideoS1).
The three compartments (pods) were made of plastic with non-slip floor mats. On the middle corner of each pod, a small bowl (0.59 L) was fastened over a 1.9 L bowl (stainless steel; Snap’y Fit, Midwest Homes for Pets, Muncie, IN USA). The smaller bowl was used to offer 100 mL of each solution; the large bowl was used to collect spillage. The maze was set up at the end of the facility, and two gates (described above) were used as the entry way (
Figure 2). A standard pig board was used in the main isle to shut the subject in the maze (
Figure 2). A camera (GeoVision Model GV-EBL4702-2F; Geovision, Irvine, CA USA) was placed at a 90⁰ angle over each pod and the entrance area for a total of 4 cameras over the testing arena. A customized NVR recorded each session continuously at 30 frames per second. In real time, one researcher recorded the time of entry and order of entry in each pod. After the session, residual solutions were collected from every bowl and volume refused was measured and recorded using a 25- or 100-mL graduated cylinder, then volume consumed was documented as a percentage.
Prior to experiments, pigs were acclimated and conditioned in pairs twice and then individually to properly navigate the maze and 100 mL of +control was available in each pod of the maze (
Figure 2). Pigs then were conditioned to predict that there is at least one bowl with desired +control (100 mL). This method causes pigs to explore all 3 options at least once (Nofre et al., 2002) The location of +control was randomized during this conditioning phase (
Figure 2). For the first experiment, each pod randomly contained one of three types of teas. Experiment 1 consisted of 4 identical sessions with the placement of tea-type randomized. Four sessions were required because the initial trial was the first time that pigs were ever exposed to fermented sorghum teas and therefore, novelty responses could be considered. Each session in experiment 1 consisted of a pod containing one of three varieties of sorghum tea on a rotating basis.
For experiment 2, three equal parts of the teas from experiment 1 were combined (3Tea; equal portions of white, roasted, sumac) and were tested against the positive and negative control (VideoS1). Due to the pigs already being familiar with the fermented tea and controls, only 3 identical sessions were conducted. For each pig and each session, the placement of solutions in the pods were randomized. For both experiments, containers were marked with generic numbers and stickers to improve blindness of the observers, although the tea was obviously a darker color than the control solutions. Control solutions were not dyed with food coloring because food coloring has flavor and pigs use their olfactory senses for preference over vision and food coloring may have additional flavors that pigs can detect.
2.4. Video Behavioral quantification
All videos were collected onto a single drive within the computer system. The videos from the 4 cameras were merged into one file and the Geovision codec was converted into a standard H264 into AVI format. The videos were then manually coded for behaviors using the specialized time stamping software (Observer 1 Noldus Observer Leesburg, VA, USA). An ethogram with mutually exclusive behaviors was created. Subsequently, a coding system was made within the software. The behaviors included the combination of spatial (
Figure 2; entry, left middle and right; defined when the first two legs crossed the threshold) and structural definitions (non-oral behaviors, head in bowl, and non-nutritive oral behaviors, NNOB) which were previously described by (Hulbert et al., 2019). Three macros-based keyboards (X-Keys. Engineering, Williamston, MI USA) were programmed to correspond with the behavioral codes in the ethogram. Three trained ethologists coded the same eleven videos within the software. A Chronbach’s reliability test was performed for the 3 observers and 11 samples, and mean alphas were 0.94 ± 0.115 SD and 0.93 ± 0.061 SD for duration and frequency, respectively. After the agreement test was performed, each trained ethologist was randomly assigned up to 4 pigs to code every session. Ethologists were blinded to the arm treatments for the second conditioning phases, and the two treatment phases and only coded for generic spatial behaviors (e.g. left, middle, right).
2.5. Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 (SAS, Cary, North Carolina/USA, SAS Institute Inc.). Data were first transposed in SAS from left, middle, right to correspond to the different choices (supplementary SAS code; S2TransposeCode). A linear mixed model for repeated measures was applied (REML). Included were the random variable of pigs and the fixed effects of choice, exposure session, and the interaction of choice and exposure and random effect pig were fitted. The covariance structure for both experiments was compound symmetry. The outcome variables included consumption, duration percentage, latency percentage, and rate/number per min. The data was analyzed for normality by the Shapiro-Wilkes test. Consumption data were also evaluated using the non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test. Least-Squares means for consumed volume, duration, latency, and rate LS means were assessed. Meanwhile, pairwise comparisons were carried out using the two-sided test. The Tukey-Kramer method was used to adjust for multiplicity. Every response variable has a p-value that was square root or logged transformed and are indicated within the table results. A treatment difference of P ≤ 0.05 was considered significant.
4. Discussion
The aim of this study was to determine if the pigs possessed a preference for varying varieties of a fermented sorghum-based tea. This would aid in the development of a satiety product for human consumption. Swine are more biologically relevant subjects for flavor preference in humans than a rodent model. [
31,
32,
33] This is largely due to the decreased phylogenetic distance between humans and swine as opposed to humans and rodents [
34,
35,
36,
37]. However, most research involving swine taste preference has been done in young pigs or lactating sows that are ad libitum fed [
38,
39,
40,
41,
42].
Some swine researchers cite that sorghum is an inferior grain when compared to corn because growing pigs have reduced feed intake and average daily gain when fed a sorghum-based diet [
43,
44]. In swine production, the reputation of sorghum may be based off of fermented sorghum offered to pigs is dried distillers’ grains, a by-product that could potentially be fed to pigs. Producers are more likely to pick corn based dried distillers grain because of higher levels of metabolizable energy compared to sorghum-based distillers grain [
45]. Nonetheless, researchers advise that sorghum be properly processed and not be derived from a by-product in order to increase bioavailability of nutrients [
46,
47,
48] and the benefits of the antioxidant capacity may improve the immune system [
49] which would be more appropriate for limit fed pigs [
50]. Therefore, before the health benefits of fermented sorghum tea can be studied in adult pigs, the authors needed to determine if the sour and bitter flavors of fermented sorghum tea would be rejected. The hypotheses were that pigs would prefer the tea with the least gallic acid, and then prefer the isocaloric control over the blended fermented sorghum tea.
The isocaloric control was created to contain the same amount of sweetness as the fermented sorghum tea. The culture for this tea formula still requires sugar to activate the SCOBY and then they produce fructose and glucose left over as byproducts [
51]. The formula also produces byproducts that add tartness. Acetic, gallic, glucuronic, and malic acids after fermentation result in the sourness that can cause rejection among subjects that have not been exposed to these flavors [
23,
52]. Due to the fermentation process, the fermented tea possesses some carbonation, although humans prefer carbonation [
53,
54,
55]. The final layer commonly reported in the flavor breakdown of SCOBY fermented teas is that of bitterness and a slight astringency. This can be attributed higher than normal level of ethanol that is a naturally occurring byproduct of fermentation, but only 1.0% alcohol by volume of ethanol is legally allowed in commercial fermented teas [
52,
56]. However, the bitterness and slight astringency is largely attributed to the polyphenol content of the product [
52,
57].
For the current study, during the acclimation and conditioning phase the pigs spent a significant amount of time performing non-nutritive oral behaviors, which in the case of test areas, are interpreted as exploratory behaviors. Pigs first had to adjust to the novelty of the 3-choice maze test arena but reduced exploratory behaviors after each session. Then, they had to learn that at least one of three options contained the positive control (isocaloric water sweetened with sucrose), which is a method used pharmaceutical avoidance or preference research [
58].
In the first experiment, three varieties of sorghum tea were evaluated. This first experiment was designed to include four sessions, because the first session would be the first time in the pigs’ life that they were exposed to a fermented product. As expected, pigs consumed less tea (regardless of type) in the first session compared to the three succeeding sessions. Age influences the amount of time pigs spend in an exploratory affective state [
35,
59]. Neophilia increases with age, therefore, in experiment 1, adult pigs displayed less exploratory behaviors and more appetitive behaviors compared to research with young pigs and preference or novelty testing [
35,
59]. For the first experiment, the only behavioral measure that was influenced by choice was the non-oral behavior duration measure. Non-oral behaviors could be interpreted as refractory or rather, satiety, behaviors [
60]. In appetitive behavioral analyses, animals exhibit three affective states: first, appetitive (i.e. exploratory), then consumption, which is followed by a refractory period [
61]. In a sense consumption of the teas and durations of head-in-bowl were great and similar among all tea types offered, the non-oral behaviors displayed near the white tea bowl indicate either: 1) a level of satiety, or; 2) a greater interest in the sumac-based teas. The authors suspect the latter because gallic acid and other polyphenols are associated with satiety in rodent and human trials [
61,
62], and the white fermented tea in the current experiment had significantly less gallic acid than the sumac teas. The non-oral behaviors associated with the roasted sumac tea were the same as the non-roasted sumac tea, even though gallic acid was reduced by the roasting process [
63,
64]. Nonetheless, there are not enough indicators of a preference for a type of tea for the current experiment, but rather adult pigs display high affinity for all three types of teas based on the fact that they consumed nearly all three teas after the first exposure.
The live observation measures (first choice and volume consumed) did not indicate a clear preference during the trials, therefore, in the second experiment, the authors blended the three teas from the first experiment to compare to the positive control (isocaloric water) and negative control (water). For overall consumption, pigs consumed very little of the negative control, while the isocaloric positive control and blended tea were nearly completely consumed. In addition, the greatest duration of exploratory behaviors were observed near the negative control. For preference research, a negative control is needed to determine if both the positive control and substrate of interest will be avoided [
59,
66] . The refusal volume and exploratory behaviors indicated that the pigs were dissatisfied with the negative control, and avoidance of the blended tea was not observed. Exploratory behaviors include NNOB, which in pigs are also referred to in the literature as oronasal or oral-nasal-facial behaviors because pigs also rub their face and root as a part of NNOB [
66,
67,
68]. These behaviors are commonly studied in sows housed in gestation crates that produce a mentally unstimulating environment. However, the adult pigs in the current experiment were not housed in a mentally stagnant environment and the 3-Choice Maze test added enrichment to their daily activities.
In fact, there were clear indicators that the blended tea was preferred over the isocaloric control, even though they had the same level of calories from sugar. For example, the total amount of time spent in the pod with the blended tea was greater than the positive control and the total amount of time the head was in the bowl were greater the positive control, even though the blended tea had 50-fold increase of gallic acid. As mentioned previously, the acid by-products from fermentation increases the amount of sour, bitter, and astringent mouthfeel [
52,
57,
69]. The results of the current study are in contrast to the assumption that pigs will reject these added flavors. This is due to the fact that the scope of inference in literature is focused on young pigs or neonatal pigs and their dams.
Albeit, for the current project, one must note that flavors that pigs have shown to avoid are in addition to the sweet flavors from the SCOBY products (i.e. fructose and glucose) [
51]. Figuroa et al. (2019), compared preference-behaviors for umami concentrations (Monosodium glutamate MSG) and compared them to sucrose concentration in young commercial pigs (1 month old). This work demonstrated that although pigs preferred the highest concentration of umami (MSG), they preferred the medium concentrations of sucrose (4-8%; scaled from 0.5-30% solution). This preferred sucrose concentration is comparable to the current project. The positive control solution was 7.2% sucrose, which matched the Brix calculated sugar content of the fermented sorghum teas, thus matching Figuroa et al.’s sucrose findings. The authors of this manuscript can deduce that the added flavors from sorghum and fermentation are preferred with the backdrop of optimum sweetness. Classic pharmaceutical drug avoidance researchers identified bitter flavors without a sweet foundation. Young adult pigs (2-4 months) that were fasted from food and water for 8-16 hour had thresholds for acceptance of bitter compounds when compared to human thresholds [
27]. However, more recent researchers identified that water deprivation confounds preference results [
38]. In the current work, our pigs were not water deprived, but rather fed at maintenance. Researchers demonstrated in young pigs that they will prioritize palatability over biological value, which is commonly seen in humans as well [
39,
70]. For the current research, the polyphenols have biological value, but the added flavors did not appear to phase the adult pigs. In fact, they prefer the fermented sorghum tea over the isocaloric control, which may be an indication that nutrients may be valued in adult pigs.
There is little published research on adult pig preference on flavors [
39,
40]. Wang et al., (2014) examined if the supplementation of the feed of 52 Large White lactating sows would impact flavor preference in their piglets. The study was conducted to address insufficient feed intake of commercial sows during lactation. There were four treatments consisting of an astringent compound such as sodium butyrate, fruit-milk flavor and fruit-milk-anise flavor, as well as a control diet. Wang et al., (2014) determined that dams consumed for feed when they were treated with the fruit-milk-anise. Anise is a seasoning that leaves a bitter after taste [
71], therefore researchers focused on preexposure to flavors via lactation so that piglets have less avoidance behaviors at weaning. The piglets from fruit-milk-anise fed sows had increased in feed intake at weaning, therefore, pre-exposure is key to acceptance of new flavors among young animals. In the current study, the adult pigs had a lack of neophobia for the additional flavors, even though the additional flavors were likely novel. This finding is likely due to the fact that the final product has the foundation of sweetness, but adult pigs that are limit fed are neophilic. Future work will determine if obese pigs have the same affinity to fermented sorghum tea as lean pigs.
Figure 1.
Flow chart outlining the procedure for making fermented sorghum tea using symbiotic culture of bacteria and yeast (SCOBY).
Figure 1.
Flow chart outlining the procedure for making fermented sorghum tea using symbiotic culture of bacteria and yeast (SCOBY).
Figure 2.
First Panel: 3PodShape Maze diagram- The isle was used as the chute and two standard gates were used to funnel the pig into the center entrance of the three pods (Entrance). After the subject entered the entrance, a pig board was used (dotted line) to close the maze. A bowl was placed in each pod and 100 mL of solution was offered. Second Panel- Flow chart representing each step of the preference study. 1The acclimation phase had 100 mL of isocaloric solution (sweet water) in every bowl and pigs that did not enter a pod were led into the area with a marshmallow or a feed pellet was dropped into the bowl. 2The phase did not include leading or extra marshmallows. 3The solutions were randomly assigned to pod so that each pig had a solution placed in at least three different pods.
Figure 2.
First Panel: 3PodShape Maze diagram- The isle was used as the chute and two standard gates were used to funnel the pig into the center entrance of the three pods (Entrance). After the subject entered the entrance, a pig board was used (dotted line) to close the maze. A bowl was placed in each pod and 100 mL of solution was offered. Second Panel- Flow chart representing each step of the preference study. 1The acclimation phase had 100 mL of isocaloric solution (sweet water) in every bowl and pigs that did not enter a pod were led into the area with a marshmallow or a feed pellet was dropped into the bowl. 2The phase did not include leading or extra marshmallows. 3The solutions were randomly assigned to pod so that each pig had a solution placed in at least three different pods.
Figure 3.
Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, with the top error bar representing total duration SEM. The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). a,b,cLS Means differed (p < 0.05).
Figure 3.
Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, with the top error bar representing total duration SEM. The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). a,b,cLS Means differed (p < 0.05).
Figure 4.
Line graph of sessions for total duration pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The error bars represent SEM. * p ≤ 0.05 negative control was less than isocaloric control and 3 Tea.
Figure 4.
Line graph of sessions for total duration pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The error bars represent SEM. * p ≤ 0.05 negative control was less than isocaloric control and 3 Tea.
Figure 5.
Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). * p ≤ 0.05 LS Means for total duration LS means between 3Tea and Isocaloric; **p ≤ 0.01 for total LS means between –control and the other solutions. a,b,c p ≤ 0.05 LS Means within behavioral category.
Figure 5.
Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). * p ≤ 0.05 LS Means for total duration LS means between 3Tea and Isocaloric; **p ≤ 0.01 for total LS means between –control and the other solutions. a,b,c p ≤ 0.05 LS Means within behavioral category.
Table 1.
Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from: white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted) sorghum. Pigs were exposed (Exp) four times after conditioning.
Table 1.
Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from: white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted) sorghum. Pigs were exposed (Exp) four times after conditioning.
|
Choice |
Exposure |
p-Values |
|
White |
Sumac |
Roasted |
SEM1
|
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
SEM1
|
Choice |
Exp |
Choice x Exp |
Consumption2, mL |
95.2 |
97.9 |
95.6 |
±2.34 |
87.9a
|
99.9b
|
99.2b
|
97.9b
|
±2.64 |
0.628 |
0.005 |
0.653 |
Gallic acid, mg |
3.3a
|
13.7b
|
12.7c
|
±0.22 |
9.1a
|
10.3b
|
10.2b
|
10.1b
|
±0.25 |
<0.001 |
0.008 |
0.201 |
Duration, % |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Total |
28.2 |
31.8 |
30.0 |
±1.25 |
30.1 |
29.8 |
30.5 |
29.7 |
±1.48 |
0.208 |
0.988 |
0.922 |
Bowl3
|
21.9 |
24.2 |
23.9 |
±1.69 |
23.7 |
21.0 |
24.6 |
24.0 |
±1.80 |
0.264 |
0.213 |
0.923 |
Explore4,5
|
3.1 |
3.3 |
2.3 |
±0.92 |
3.3a
|
4.7a
|
1.7b
|
1.8b
|
±1.01 |
0.617 |
0.029 |
0.958 |
Non-oral behaviors6
|
3.3a
|
4.3b,c
|
3.8c
|
±0.45 |
3.1d
|
4.0e
|
4.1e
|
3.9e
|
±0.48 |
0.050 |
0.097 |
0.949 |
Latency, % |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Bowl3
|
24.4 |
24.7 |
26.8 |
±3.60 |
30.9 |
19.3 |
24.3 |
26.7 |
±4.22 |
0.890 |
0.327 |
0.672 |
Explore4
|
62.7 |
67.5 |
74.1 |
±6.10 |
56.6d
|
68.7e
|
75.8e
|
71.1e
|
±6.58 |
0.192 |
0.060 |
0.970 |
Other6
|
23.5 |
23.8 |
27.5 |
±7.85 |
30.8 |
19.5 |
23.4 |
26.1 |
±8.10 |
0.696 |
0.338 |
0.626 |
Rate, no./min |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Bowl3
|
0.68 |
0.70 |
0.68 |
±0.068 |
0.57d,e
|
0.70e,f
|
0.72e,f
|
0.76f
|
±0.073 |
0.904 |
0.069 |
0.982 |
Explore4
|
0.40 |
0.43 |
0.32 |
±0.079 |
0.47 |
0.36 |
0.36 |
0.35 |
±0.084 |
0.302 |
0.360 |
0.907 |
Other6
|
0.97 |
1.15 |
1.08 |
±0.120 |
0.81a
|
1.1b
|
1.2b
|
1.2b
|
±0.126 |
0.173 |
0.003 |
0.962 |
1Largest Standard Error Mean; 2Total volume consumed (100 mL maximum) data were non-parametric when Choice was considered. The Kruskal-Wallis Test for Consumption Choice was not significant (χ2 = 0.39; p = 0.82; df = 2) but was significant for gallic acid (χ2 = 97.5; p < 0.001; df = 2), 3Head over bowl, head moving, indicating drinking; 4p-values were derived from square root transformed data; 5All non-nutritive oral behaviors (NNOB); 6Walking, standing without any oral-related behavior (i.e. drinking, sniffing, licking, rooting, or rubbing using the mouth, snout, or face); a,b,c LS Means p ≤ 0.05; d,e,f LS Means p ≥ 0.05 ≤ 0.10. |
Table 2.
Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with choices of: distilled water (-control), isocaloric control (+control; water with sugar) or fermented sorghum blended tea (3Tea; equal parts of white, sumac, and roasted sumac). Pigs were exposed (Exp) to the 3-choice test three times.
Table 2.
Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with choices of: distilled water (-control), isocaloric control (+control; water with sugar) or fermented sorghum blended tea (3Tea; equal parts of white, sumac, and roasted sumac). Pigs were exposed (Exp) to the 3-choice test three times.
|
Choice |
Exposure |
p-values |
|
-control |
+control |
3Tea |
SEM1
|
1 |
2 |
3 |
SEM1
|
Choice |
Exp |
Choice x Exp |
Consumption2, mL |
18.0a
|
96.6b
|
99.0b
|
±2.21 |
74.1 |
68.9 |
70.6 |
±2.97 |
<0.001 |
0.136 |
0.087 |
Gallic Acid, mg |
0.02a |
0.20b
|
11.7c
|
±0.03 |
3.8a
|
3.8a
|
4.3b
|
±0.03 |
<0.001 |
<0.001 |
<0.001 |
Duration, % |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Total |
16.8a
|
34.5b
|
37.7c
|
±1.71 |
28.4 |
29.5 |
31.1 |
±1.71 |
<0.001 |
0.597 |
0.033 |
Bowl3
|
5.6a
|
27.6b
|
31.9c
|
±1.87 |
20.3 |
21.8 |
23.0 |
±1.92 |
<0.001 |
0.365 |
0.091 |
Explore4,5
|
7.1a
|
3.0b
|
1.4b
|
±1.45 |
4.4 |
3.5 |
3.5 |
±1.50 |
0.002 |
0.866 |
0.261 |
Non-oral behaviors6
|
4.2 |
4.1 |
4.4 |
±0.45 |
4.5 |
4.2 |
4.0 |
±0.46 |
0.583 |
0.321 |
0.506 |
Latency, % |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Bowl3
|
20.8 |
16.2 |
14.8 |
±3.45 |
13.93 |
19.59 |
18.19 |
±3.61 |
0.420 |
0.481 |
0.681 |
Explore4
|
60.1d
|
71.2e
|
76.0e
|
±7.55 |
69.0 |
65.7 |
72.6 |
±7.74 |
0.086 |
0.608 |
0.714 |
Other6
|
16.2 |
15.2 |
13.8 |
±3.11 |
12.2 |
15.6 |
17.4 |
±3.26 |
0.849 |
0.480 |
0.958 |
Rate, no./min |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Bowl3
|
20.75 |
16.18 |
14.77 |
±3.459 |
0.42 |
0.48 |
0.68 |
±3.615 |
0.420 |
0.481 |
0.681 |
Explore4
|
0.55d
|
0.38e
|
0.35e
|
±0.108 |
0.39 |
0.53 |
0.37 |
±0.111 |
0.102 |
0.196 |
0.646 |
Other6
|
1.41 |
1.44 |
1.52 |
±0.150 |
1.49 |
1.46 |
1.41 |
±0.152 |
0.526 |
0.730 |
0.413 |
1Largest Standard Error Mean; 2Total volume consumed (100 mL maximum) data were non-parametric when Choice was considered. The Kruskal-Wallis Test for Choice was significant for consumption volume (χ2 = 0.71.6; P < 0.001; df = 2), and for gallic acid (χ2 = 88.9; p < 0.001; df = 2),4P-Values were derived from square root transformed data; 5All non-nutritive oral behaviors (NNOB); 6All non-oral behaviors walking or standing without any sniffing, drinking, chomping, licking, rooting, rubbing using the mouth, snout, or face); a,b,c LS Means ≤ 0.05; d,e LS Means ≥ 0.05 ≤ 0.10. |