Preprint
Article

Adult Ossabaw pigs prefer fermented sorghum tea over isocaloric sweetened water

Altmetrics

Downloads

97

Views

71

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

10 August 2023

Posted:

10 August 2023

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Three-choice preferences for fermented sorghum teas (FST) were examined among 11 adult Ossabaw pigs. The objectives for experiments 1 and 2 (respectively) were to determine preference for: 1) one of three types of FST (white vs. sumac vs. roasted sumac), and 2) isocaloric control (+control; made isocaloric with sucrose), blended FST (3Tea; equal parts of white, sumac, and roasted-sumac), or a negative control (-control; distilled water; avoidance). Pigs were first conditioned to navigate a three-choice preference maze. For experiment 1, no clear preference behaviors for tea-type were observed. Pigs consumed nearly all the teas, but pigs consumed the least amount of tea in the first session. Pigs spent the most time with their heads in the bowl when 3Tea was offered, followed by +control, and the least time was -control.  Regardless of tea-type, adult pigs show a strong preference for FST, even over +control, which was sweetened water. Adult pigs likely prefer the complexity of flavors, rather than the sweetness alone.  Future projects that explore the health benefits of this sorghum formula will not be impeded by avoidance among adult pigs.
Keywords: 
Subject: Biology and Life Sciences  -   Animal Science, Veterinary Science and Zoology

1. Introduction

Sorghum is a potentially more sustainable crop than other grains due to the plant’s resilience to dry, hot climates [1,2,3]. Therefore, new, palatable products need to be developed to improve both food availability and nutrition-security. Compared to other cereal grains, sorghum may provide more nutrition-security because of greater concentrations of phenolic compounds [4,5]. Phenolic compounds possess significant antioxidant abilities that modulate chronic inflammation [6], prevent cancerous cells proliferation [7], and may even improve satiety during weight loss programs involving tea-based products [8].
Nonetheless, sorghum strains with high concentrations of phenolic compounds (e.g., red or black sorghum) may contribute to increased bitterness in products because of the associated increased tannins [5,9,10]. Furthermore, when sorghum is heated, it decreases protein bioavailability in cooked sorghum, which may be caused by phenolic compounds binding to proteins [5,11]. These drawbacks may be remedied through the process of fermentation. [12,13,14]. For example, sorghum can be cultured to produce potentially beneficial by-products and increased protein bioavailability and the fermentation of the liquid may be palatable to adults [15,16,17].
For this current project, fermented sorghum-based tea was created because of the potential to have high concentrations of antioxidants and probiotics, thus providing a nutrition-secure beverage. Although sorghum-teas and fermented-teas originated in northeast China around 220 B.C [18,19], fermented teas are new to the other countries. The U.S. Kombucha is a recent example of a commercialized fermented tea, which is produced by first seeping black, green, or oolong tea leaves, then, adding sugar and a symbiotic culture of bacteria and yeast (SCOBY) [18,20]. These same methods could be applied to sorghum, but with Kombucha, the fermentation results in sour flavors and astringent mouthfeel [17,20]. A major influence of kombucha flavor is the organic acid content produced by the microorganisms within the SCOBY. Specifically, malic, gallic, and lactic acids contribute to added sour flavors and dry mouth feel, which also are associated with red wines even though these compounds contribute to the antioxidant properties of fermented beverages [20,21,22].
The novel flavors and mouth feel of fermented beverages are often appreciated by adult humans; The current authors will use adult pigs as a biomedical model to understand palatability and health benefits of fermented sorghum tea. However, previous research shows that pigs may reject sour or bitter flavors. Nevertheless, most preference research has been conducted on either young commercial pigs at weaning, or ad libitum fed sows that have inappetence after parturition [23,24,25,26]. There is a lack of research publications on flavor preference in adult pigs that must be limit fed. Therefore, the objectives of this study were: 1) Assess adult pig preference among three types of formulated sorghum teas, and 2) compare the preference or avoidance behaviors among choices of blended fermented sorghum tea, positive control isocaloric sugar water, and negative control water. The hypothesis was that adult pigs would prefer the sorghum tea with the least amount of gallic acid and the isocaloric control (sugar water) over a blend of fermented sorghum teas.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Animals and housing

This study followed the guidelines set forth by the Guide for the Care and Use of Agricultural Animals in Agriculture and Teaching (CorVus IACUC #4745). The animal portion of the project was conducted between May and June 2022. Twelve adult Ossabaw gilts and barrows (age 15.6 ± 0.62 SD months; 53.5 ± 0.62 kg body weight) were pair-housed by gender at indoor, climate-controlled facilities (21°C; CorVus Biomedical, LLC, Crawfordsville, Indiana). Artificial lighting was a 12-h cycle, with lights on from 0800 to 2000 h. One month prior to the start of the experiment, pig pairs were randomized into one of six pens that were stratified by gender (gilts and barrows). Pens provided 14.92 m2 of free space with plastic slotted flooring (Maxima Sow Slat Floor, Greenfield, WI USA). The gating for the pens were 94.0 cm tall and custom made with vertical standard stainless bars (Thorp Equipment, Thorp, WI USA). Two feeders (S3 Series Dry Sow Feeder, Crystal Spring Hog Equipment Agathe, Manitoba CA) were provided on each side of the pen with a drinker (ad libitum well water; QC Swine Water Cup System, Delphi, IN USA) next to one of feeders. Environmental enrichment consisted of two combined stainless-steel chains, with one link fastened near the drinker. Pigs were fed a total of 750 g feed per day to maintain lean body type (15% Crude Protein, 3% Crude Fat; 5% Fiber; DuMOR Hog Grower, Tractor Supply, Brentwood, TN USA). Pig pairs were randomly assigned to pen using the RAND function of Excel (Microsoft Excel, Version 10; Redmond, WA). Then, individual pigs within each pen were randomized for exposure order to the testing arena.

2.2. Formulation of solutions

Three varieties of sorghum (Nu Life, Scott City, KS) were used for this study: raw-white, raw-sumac, and roasted sumac strains. To create the sorghum tea (Figure 1), commercially available (for human consumption) sorghum was steeped in sterile water at room temperature for 16 hours. Then, the supernatant was recovered by filtering and brought to a boil before sugar was dissolved (Figure 1). Once boiled, 2 cups of supernatant (473 mL) were added to a jar with 1 cup of sugar (200 grams) and 8 cups (1892 mL) of deionized water. Afterwards, the solution was cooled to room temperature, and inoculated with a commercially available culture (Symbiotic Culture of Bacteria and Yeast, SCOBY; The Kombucha Shop, Madison, WI USA). Then, the solution fermented for 7 days at room temperature. Fermentation was then stalled by refrigeration. For this current experiment, the solution was refrigerated for 72 h before use. The pH of the fermented solutions were monitored and averaged 3.12 ±0.12 SD. An isocaloric (0.288 calories per mL), positive control (+control) was created with sugar (sucrose) and boiled distilled water. The negative control (-control) was distilled water only. Each batch of solution was sampled and frozen at -20 ⁰C for gallic acid analyses. Gallic analyses was determined using the Folin Ciocalteu Method as described previously [27,28,29,30].

2.3. Preference Testing

A commercially available, 3 compartment (i.e. pod) preference maze was used for the preference test (3-PodShape, by Hulbert and ShapeMaster, Odgen, IL USA; Figure 2; VideoS1).
The three compartments (pods) were made of plastic with non-slip floor mats. On the middle corner of each pod, a small bowl (0.59 L) was fastened over a 1.9 L bowl (stainless steel; Snap’y Fit, Midwest Homes for Pets, Muncie, IN USA). The smaller bowl was used to offer 100 mL of each solution; the large bowl was used to collect spillage. The maze was set up at the end of the facility, and two gates (described above) were used as the entry way (Figure 2). A standard pig board was used in the main isle to shut the subject in the maze (Figure 2). A camera (GeoVision Model GV-EBL4702-2F; Geovision, Irvine, CA USA) was placed at a 90⁰ angle over each pod and the entrance area for a total of 4 cameras over the testing arena. A customized NVR recorded each session continuously at 30 frames per second. In real time, one researcher recorded the time of entry and order of entry in each pod. After the session, residual solutions were collected from every bowl and volume refused was measured and recorded using a 25- or 100-mL graduated cylinder, then volume consumed was documented as a percentage.
Prior to experiments, pigs were acclimated and conditioned in pairs twice and then individually to properly navigate the maze and 100 mL of +control was available in each pod of the maze (Figure 2). Pigs then were conditioned to predict that there is at least one bowl with desired +control (100 mL). This method causes pigs to explore all 3 options at least once (Nofre et al., 2002) The location of +control was randomized during this conditioning phase (Figure 2). For the first experiment, each pod randomly contained one of three types of teas. Experiment 1 consisted of 4 identical sessions with the placement of tea-type randomized. Four sessions were required because the initial trial was the first time that pigs were ever exposed to fermented sorghum teas and therefore, novelty responses could be considered. Each session in experiment 1 consisted of a pod containing one of three varieties of sorghum tea on a rotating basis.
For experiment 2, three equal parts of the teas from experiment 1 were combined (3Tea; equal portions of white, roasted, sumac) and were tested against the positive and negative control (VideoS1). Due to the pigs already being familiar with the fermented tea and controls, only 3 identical sessions were conducted. For each pig and each session, the placement of solutions in the pods were randomized. For both experiments, containers were marked with generic numbers and stickers to improve blindness of the observers, although the tea was obviously a darker color than the control solutions. Control solutions were not dyed with food coloring because food coloring has flavor and pigs use their olfactory senses for preference over vision and food coloring may have additional flavors that pigs can detect.

2.4. Video Behavioral quantification

All videos were collected onto a single drive within the computer system. The videos from the 4 cameras were merged into one file and the Geovision codec was converted into a standard H264 into AVI format. The videos were then manually coded for behaviors using the specialized time stamping software (Observer 1 Noldus Observer Leesburg, VA, USA). An ethogram with mutually exclusive behaviors was created. Subsequently, a coding system was made within the software. The behaviors included the combination of spatial (Figure 2; entry, left middle and right; defined when the first two legs crossed the threshold) and structural definitions (non-oral behaviors, head in bowl, and non-nutritive oral behaviors, NNOB) which were previously described by (Hulbert et al., 2019). Three macros-based keyboards (X-Keys. Engineering, Williamston, MI USA) were programmed to correspond with the behavioral codes in the ethogram. Three trained ethologists coded the same eleven videos within the software. A Chronbach’s reliability test was performed for the 3 observers and 11 samples, and mean alphas were 0.94 ± 0.115 SD and 0.93 ± 0.061 SD for duration and frequency, respectively. After the agreement test was performed, each trained ethologist was randomly assigned up to 4 pigs to code every session. Ethologists were blinded to the arm treatments for the second conditioning phases, and the two treatment phases and only coded for generic spatial behaviors (e.g. left, middle, right).

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 (SAS, Cary, North Carolina/USA, SAS Institute Inc.). Data were first transposed in SAS from left, middle, right to correspond to the different choices (supplementary SAS code; S2TransposeCode). A linear mixed model for repeated measures was applied (REML). Included were the random variable of pigs and the fixed effects of choice, exposure session, and the interaction of choice and exposure and random effect pig were fitted. The covariance structure for both experiments was compound symmetry. The outcome variables included consumption, duration percentage, latency percentage, and rate/number per min. The data was analyzed for normality by the Shapiro-Wilkes test. Consumption data were also evaluated using the non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test. Least-Squares means for consumed volume, duration, latency, and rate LS means were assessed. Meanwhile, pairwise comparisons were carried out using the two-sided test. The Tukey-Kramer method was used to adjust for multiplicity. Every response variable has a p-value that was square root or logged transformed and are indicated within the table results. A treatment difference of P ≤ 0.05 was considered significant.

3. Results

3.1. General Results

This The fermented tea types had the following gallic acid concentrations (µg/mL): white, 34.57 ±0.47 SD; sumac, 140.41 ±0.41 SD, and roasted sumac, 133.35 ±0.81 SD. The blended tea (3Tea) had a gallic acid mean of 120.70 ±11.57 SD µg/mL. During the conditioning phase, one gilt passed away from unknown reasons (necropsy completed by veterinarian and cardiologist) and did not complete experiment 1 and 2.

3.2. Experiment 1

No Choice and Exp interaction were observed among any variables (p > 0.10; Table 1). There was a decrease in consumption in the first exposure when compared to later exposures (p = 0.005; Table 1). Regardless of tea type, pigs spent the most amount of time performing exploratory behaviors in exposures 1 and 2 (p = 0.029; Table 1). Consequently, gallic acid intake was the least for the white tea type, and sumac tea type was the greatest, followed by roasted sumac tea type (p <0.001; Table 1).
The rate at which the pigs performed non-oral behaviors was the lowest during the first exposure (P = 0.003; Table 1). Pigs performed non-oral behaviors the most (p = 0.050; Table 1; Figure 3) when they were exposed to sumac tea-type, and the least when they were exposed to the white tea type.
The pigs checked the bowl at the lowest rate per minute in the first and second exposure. There tended (p = 0.060; Table 1) to be a greater latency to express exploratory behaviors during the first exposure.

3.3. Experiment 2

Consumption volume was greatest for 3Tea compared to the positive and negative controls (p < 0.001; Table 2).
There was a choice by exposure interaction effect; pigs spent the most time in pods containing the 3Tea blend after the first exposure in experiment 2 (p < 0.003; Figure 4).
The total duration spent in the pod was greatest for 3Tea, and the least for -control (p < 0.001; Figure 5). Likewise, pigs spent the greatest duration with their heads over the bowl in the pods with 3Tea, which was greater than +control, and the least was -control (p < 0.001; Table 2; Figure 5). The pigs performed more NNOB in the pods with -control (p = 0.002; Table 2, Figure 5).

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to determine if the pigs possessed a preference for varying varieties of a fermented sorghum-based tea. This would aid in the development of a satiety product for human consumption. Swine are more biologically relevant subjects for flavor preference in humans than a rodent model. [31,32,33] This is largely due to the decreased phylogenetic distance between humans and swine as opposed to humans and rodents [34,35,36,37]. However, most research involving swine taste preference has been done in young pigs or lactating sows that are ad libitum fed [38,39,40,41,42].
Some swine researchers cite that sorghum is an inferior grain when compared to corn because growing pigs have reduced feed intake and average daily gain when fed a sorghum-based diet [43,44]. In swine production, the reputation of sorghum may be based off of fermented sorghum offered to pigs is dried distillers’ grains, a by-product that could potentially be fed to pigs. Producers are more likely to pick corn based dried distillers grain because of higher levels of metabolizable energy compared to sorghum-based distillers grain [45]. Nonetheless, researchers advise that sorghum be properly processed and not be derived from a by-product in order to increase bioavailability of nutrients [46,47,48] and the benefits of the antioxidant capacity may improve the immune system [49] which would be more appropriate for limit fed pigs [50]. Therefore, before the health benefits of fermented sorghum tea can be studied in adult pigs, the authors needed to determine if the sour and bitter flavors of fermented sorghum tea would be rejected. The hypotheses were that pigs would prefer the tea with the least gallic acid, and then prefer the isocaloric control over the blended fermented sorghum tea.
The isocaloric control was created to contain the same amount of sweetness as the fermented sorghum tea. The culture for this tea formula still requires sugar to activate the SCOBY and then they produce fructose and glucose left over as byproducts [51]. The formula also produces byproducts that add tartness. Acetic, gallic, glucuronic, and malic acids after fermentation result in the sourness that can cause rejection among subjects that have not been exposed to these flavors [23,52]. Due to the fermentation process, the fermented tea possesses some carbonation, although humans prefer carbonation [53,54,55]. The final layer commonly reported in the flavor breakdown of SCOBY fermented teas is that of bitterness and a slight astringency. This can be attributed higher than normal level of ethanol that is a naturally occurring byproduct of fermentation, but only 1.0% alcohol by volume of ethanol is legally allowed in commercial fermented teas [52,56]. However, the bitterness and slight astringency is largely attributed to the polyphenol content of the product [52,57].
For the current study, during the acclimation and conditioning phase the pigs spent a significant amount of time performing non-nutritive oral behaviors, which in the case of test areas, are interpreted as exploratory behaviors. Pigs first had to adjust to the novelty of the 3-choice maze test arena but reduced exploratory behaviors after each session. Then, they had to learn that at least one of three options contained the positive control (isocaloric water sweetened with sucrose), which is a method used pharmaceutical avoidance or preference research [58].
In the first experiment, three varieties of sorghum tea were evaluated. This first experiment was designed to include four sessions, because the first session would be the first time in the pigs’ life that they were exposed to a fermented product. As expected, pigs consumed less tea (regardless of type) in the first session compared to the three succeeding sessions. Age influences the amount of time pigs spend in an exploratory affective state [35,59]. Neophilia increases with age, therefore, in experiment 1, adult pigs displayed less exploratory behaviors and more appetitive behaviors compared to research with young pigs and preference or novelty testing [35,59]. For the first experiment, the only behavioral measure that was influenced by choice was the non-oral behavior duration measure. Non-oral behaviors could be interpreted as refractory or rather, satiety, behaviors [60]. In appetitive behavioral analyses, animals exhibit three affective states: first, appetitive (i.e. exploratory), then consumption, which is followed by a refractory period [61]. In a sense consumption of the teas and durations of head-in-bowl were great and similar among all tea types offered, the non-oral behaviors displayed near the white tea bowl indicate either: 1) a level of satiety, or; 2) a greater interest in the sumac-based teas. The authors suspect the latter because gallic acid and other polyphenols are associated with satiety in rodent and human trials [61,62], and the white fermented tea in the current experiment had significantly less gallic acid than the sumac teas. The non-oral behaviors associated with the roasted sumac tea were the same as the non-roasted sumac tea, even though gallic acid was reduced by the roasting process [63,64]. Nonetheless, there are not enough indicators of a preference for a type of tea for the current experiment, but rather adult pigs display high affinity for all three types of teas based on the fact that they consumed nearly all three teas after the first exposure.
The live observation measures (first choice and volume consumed) did not indicate a clear preference during the trials, therefore, in the second experiment, the authors blended the three teas from the first experiment to compare to the positive control (isocaloric water) and negative control (water). For overall consumption, pigs consumed very little of the negative control, while the isocaloric positive control and blended tea were nearly completely consumed. In addition, the greatest duration of exploratory behaviors were observed near the negative control. For preference research, a negative control is needed to determine if both the positive control and substrate of interest will be avoided [59,66] . The refusal volume and exploratory behaviors indicated that the pigs were dissatisfied with the negative control, and avoidance of the blended tea was not observed. Exploratory behaviors include NNOB, which in pigs are also referred to in the literature as oronasal or oral-nasal-facial behaviors because pigs also rub their face and root as a part of NNOB [66,67,68]. These behaviors are commonly studied in sows housed in gestation crates that produce a mentally unstimulating environment. However, the adult pigs in the current experiment were not housed in a mentally stagnant environment and the 3-Choice Maze test added enrichment to their daily activities.
In fact, there were clear indicators that the blended tea was preferred over the isocaloric control, even though they had the same level of calories from sugar. For example, the total amount of time spent in the pod with the blended tea was greater than the positive control and the total amount of time the head was in the bowl were greater the positive control, even though the blended tea had 50-fold increase of gallic acid. As mentioned previously, the acid by-products from fermentation increases the amount of sour, bitter, and astringent mouthfeel [52,57,69]. The results of the current study are in contrast to the assumption that pigs will reject these added flavors. This is due to the fact that the scope of inference in literature is focused on young pigs or neonatal pigs and their dams.
Albeit, for the current project, one must note that flavors that pigs have shown to avoid are in addition to the sweet flavors from the SCOBY products (i.e. fructose and glucose) [51]. Figuroa et al. (2019), compared preference-behaviors for umami concentrations (Monosodium glutamate MSG) and compared them to sucrose concentration in young commercial pigs (1 month old). This work demonstrated that although pigs preferred the highest concentration of umami (MSG), they preferred the medium concentrations of sucrose (4-8%; scaled from 0.5-30% solution). This preferred sucrose concentration is comparable to the current project. The positive control solution was 7.2% sucrose, which matched the Brix calculated sugar content of the fermented sorghum teas, thus matching Figuroa et al.’s sucrose findings. The authors of this manuscript can deduce that the added flavors from sorghum and fermentation are preferred with the backdrop of optimum sweetness. Classic pharmaceutical drug avoidance researchers identified bitter flavors without a sweet foundation. Young adult pigs (2-4 months) that were fasted from food and water for 8-16 hour had thresholds for acceptance of bitter compounds when compared to human thresholds [27]. However, more recent researchers identified that water deprivation confounds preference results [38]. In the current work, our pigs were not water deprived, but rather fed at maintenance. Researchers demonstrated in young pigs that they will prioritize palatability over biological value, which is commonly seen in humans as well [39,70]. For the current research, the polyphenols have biological value, but the added flavors did not appear to phase the adult pigs. In fact, they prefer the fermented sorghum tea over the isocaloric control, which may be an indication that nutrients may be valued in adult pigs.
There is little published research on adult pig preference on flavors [39,40]. Wang et al., (2014) examined if the supplementation of the feed of 52 Large White lactating sows would impact flavor preference in their piglets. The study was conducted to address insufficient feed intake of commercial sows during lactation. There were four treatments consisting of an astringent compound such as sodium butyrate, fruit-milk flavor and fruit-milk-anise flavor, as well as a control diet. Wang et al., (2014) determined that dams consumed for feed when they were treated with the fruit-milk-anise. Anise is a seasoning that leaves a bitter after taste [71], therefore researchers focused on preexposure to flavors via lactation so that piglets have less avoidance behaviors at weaning. The piglets from fruit-milk-anise fed sows had increased in feed intake at weaning, therefore, pre-exposure is key to acceptance of new flavors among young animals. In the current study, the adult pigs had a lack of neophobia for the additional flavors, even though the additional flavors were likely novel. This finding is likely due to the fact that the final product has the foundation of sweetness, but adult pigs that are limit fed are neophilic. Future work will determine if obese pigs have the same affinity to fermented sorghum tea as lean pigs.

5. Conclusions

The current study showed that adult pigs have a high affinity to fermented sorghum tea, regardless of tea type. The high concentrations of gallic acid did not deter the pig’s preference, but rather improved preference. Future work could determine if there is a threshold in sorghum varieties that have very high concentrations of gallic acid, such as black sorghum.
The current work now helps reduce the assumption that all pigs will reject novel, sour and bitter flavors. Furthermore, this research increases the widely accepted notion that pigs are a superior animal model to humans than murine and other rodent models. This research reinforces the legitimacy of the use of swine models for modeling preference in humans. The authors can conclude that the future work exploring the health benefits of fermented sorghum tea will not be impeded by a preference of the isocaloric control over the complex, high polyphenol fermented sorghum tea.

6. Implications

Sorghum is a highly prolific and sustainable crop that can be grown in many harsh climates, making it a more suitable crop in the face of climate change. Nonetheless, nutritional security and consumer-acceptance play a large role in sustainability; if the products produced from a resilient crop are rejected by consumers, then it cannot be considered a sustainable crop. Therefore, sorghum products must be formulated for acceptance, little waste, and improved health. In this preliminary study, fermented sorghum tea was preferred over an equally sweet positive control among adult pigs that were limit fed to maintain a healthy body weight. If future studies indicate that fermented sorghum tea has health benefits, then the evidence of sorghum as a sustainable crop will be strengthened.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at the website of this paper posted on Preprints.org. VideoS1.jpeg S2TransposeCode.docx

Author Contributions

L.E.H., F.M.A., M.A., M.S. and M.J.D. conceived and designed research experiment; C.E.N., M.J.G, E.M.B, D.M.H.I. and L.E.H performed experiments, quantified behavioral data, and assisted in drafting manuscript. F.M.A and L.A.K. formulated and produced the fermented tea and positive control. V.T. and K.W.L. analyzed the gallic acid. J.S, J.A.H reconstructed the entire barn for the three-pod maze system and new pen format. R.J., J.A.H., J.P.B., and J.S. provided technical assistance and novel animal husbandry requirements. H.W. and M.J.D. consulted with data management and statistics. C.E.N. composed the first draft of the manuscript and included this as a chapter in Master’s of Science Thesis at Kansas State University. All coauthors contributed to the drafting of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by USDA-ARS Center for Grain and Animal Health Research the non-assistance cooperative agreement #58-3020-0-026 with Kansas State University in Manhattan, KS. The pod shaped maze development and costs were supported by the contributions no. 21-002-J from the Kansas Agricultural Experiment Station (Manhattan, KS, USA) through Hatch projects 1013670, and 1013671 from the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture (Washington, DC, USA). .

Institutional Review Board Statement

All experimental procedures adhered to the guide or the Care and Use of Agricultural Animals in Research and Teaching. All procedures were reviewed and approved by CorVus biomedical ethics committee. (IACUC #4745).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Raw data are shared through the USDA-ARS Center for Grain and Animal Health Research publicly archived database.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank: Points North Surveillance technical assistance with camera systems; ShapeMaster for maze construction and development; Dr. Aramouni’s team for creating and transporting the teas to Indiana; CorVus Staff for the care of the animals; Dr. Trinetta’s lab for performing the flavor analysis and gallic acid concentrations on our samples.

Conflicts of Interest

No conflicts of interests, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.

References

  1. Chipanshi, A.C.; Chanda, R.; Totolo, O. Vulnerability Assessment of the Maize and Sorghum Crops to Climate Change in Botswana. Clim. Change 2003, 61, 339–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Chadalavada, K.; Kumari, B.D.R.; Kumar, T.S. Sorghum mitigates climate variability and change on crop yield and quality. Planta 2021, 253, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Warsame, A.A.; Sheik-Ali, I.A.; Jama, O.M.; Hassan, A.A.; Barre, G.M. Assessing the effects of climate change and political instability on sorghum production: Empirical evidence from Somalia. J. Clean. Prod. 2022, 360, 131893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Xiong, Y.; Zhang, P.; Warner, R.D.; Fang, Z. Sorghum grain: From genotype, nutrition, and phenolic profile to its health benefits and food applications. Compr. Rev. Food Sci. Food Saf. 2019, 18, 2025–2046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Xu, J.; Wang, W.; Zhao, Y. Phenolic Compounds in Whole Grain Sorghum and Their Health Benefits. Foods 2021, 10, 10–8. [Google Scholar]
  6. Punia, H.; Tokas, J.; Malik, A.; Satpal; Sangwan, S. Characterization of phenolic compounds and antioxidant activity in sorghum [Sorghum bicolor (L.) Moench] grains. Cereal Res. Commun. 2021, 49, 343–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Arora, I.; Sharma, M.; Tollefsbol, T.O. Combinatorial Epigenetics Impact of Polyphenols and Phytochemicals in Cancer Prevention and Therapy. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2019, 20.18, 4567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Li, Y.; Rahman, S.U.; Huang, Y.; Zhang, Y.; Ming, P.; Zhu, L.; Chu, X.; Li, J.; Feng, S.; Wang, X.; Wu, J. Green tea polyphenols decrease weight gain, ameliorate alteration of gut microbiota, and mitigate intestinal inflammation in canines with high-fat-diet-induced obesity, J. Nutr. Biochem. 2020, 78, 108–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Soares, S.; Kohl, S.; Thalmann, S.; Mateus, N.; Meyerhof, W.; De Freitas, V. Different phenolic compounds activate distinct human bitter taste receptors. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2013, 61.7, 1525–1533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Soares, S.; Brandão, E.; Guerreiro, C.; Soares, S.; Mateus, N.; de Freitas, V. Tannins in Food: Insights into the Molecular Perception of Astringency and Bitter Taste. Molecules 2020, 11, 2590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Duressa, D.; Weerasoriya, D.; Bean, S.R.; Tilley, M.; Tesso, T. Genetic basis of protein digestibility in grain sorghum. Crop Sci. 2018, 58, 2183–2199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kazanas, N.; Fields, M.L. Nutritional improvement of sorghum by fermentation. J. Food Sci. 1981, 46.3, 819–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Schons, P.F.; Battestin, V.; Macedo, G.A. Fermentation and enzyme treatments for sorghum. Braz. J. Microbiol. 2012, 43.1, 89–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rodríguez-España, M.; Figueroa-Hernández, C.Y.; de Dios Figueroa-Cárdenas, J.; Rayas-Duarte, P.; Hernández-Estrada, Z.J. Effects of germination and lactic acid fermentation on nutritional and rheological properties of sorghum: A graphical review. Curr. Res. Food Sci. 2022, 5, 807–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Elkhier MK, S.; Abd-ALRaheem, A. A. Effect of fermentation period on the chemical composition, in-vitro protein digestibility and tannin content in two sorghum cultivars (Dabar and Tabat) in Sudan. J. Appl. Biosci 2011, 39, 2602–2606. [Google Scholar]
  16. Sorour, M.A.; Mehanni, A.E.; Taha, E.M.; Rashwan, A.K. Changes of total phenolics, tannins, phytate and antioxidant activity of two sorghum cultivars as affected by processing. J. Food & Dairy Sci. 2017, 8, 267–274. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bwamu, K.E.; Nkirote, K.C.; Kahiu, N.E.; Edgar, K.D. Optimization of fermentation and malting process of sorghum beverage and effects on nutritional quality. Afr. J. Food Sci. 2022, 16, 252–260. [Google Scholar]
  18. Kapp, J.M.; Sumner, W. Kombucha: a systematic review of the empirical evidence of human health benefit. Ann Epidemiol. 2019, 30, 66–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sun, H.; Wang, H.; Zhang, P.; Ajlouni, S.; Fang, Z. Changes in phenolic content, antioxidant activity, and volatile compounds during processing of fermented sorghum grain tea. Cereal Chem. 97, 612–625. [CrossRef]
  20. Xu, W.; Tong, Y.; Tong, Q.; Liu, Y.; Wang, Z. Effects of different re-fermentation methods on the quality characteristics of kombucha beverages. J. Food Sci. Technol. 2023, 60.4, 1414–1424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Badhani, B.; Sharma, N.; Kakkar, R. Gallic acid: a versatile antioxidant with promising therapeutic and industrial applications. RSC Adv. 2015, 5, 27540–27557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Sterneder, S.; Stoeger, V.; Dugulin, C.A.; Liszt, K.I.; Di Pizio, A.; Korntheuer, K. ,... & Somoza, V. (2021). Astringent gallic acid in red wine regulates mechanisms of gastric acid secretion via activation of bitter taste sensing receptor TAS2R4. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2021, 69, 10550–10561. [Google Scholar]
  23. Kennedy, J.M.; Baldwin, B.A. Taste preferences in pigs for nutritive and non-nutritive sweet solutions. Anim. Behav. 1972, 20.4, 706–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Nelson Sandra, L.; Sanregret John, D. Response of Pigs to Bitter-tasting Compounds, Chem. Senses, 1997, 22, 129–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kittawornrat, A.; Zimmerman, J. Toward a better understanding of pig behavior and pig welfare. Anim. Health Res. Rev. 2011, 12.1, 25–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Guzmán-Pino, S.A.; Lazcano, C.; De Luca, V.; Figueroa, J.; Valenzuela, C.; Roura, E. Dietary Inclusion of Monosodium Glutamate in Gestating and Lactating Sows Modifies the Preference Thresholds and Sensory-Motivated Intake for Umami and Sweet Solutions in Post-Weaned Pigs. Animals 2019, 9.6, 336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Singleton, V.L.; Rossi, J.A. Colorimetry of total phenolics with phosphomolybdic-phosphotungstic acid reagents. Am. J. Enol. Vitic. 1965, 16, 144–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Jayaprakasha, G.K.; Singh, R.P.; Sakariah, K.K. Antioxidant activity of grape seed (Vitis vinifera) extracts on peroxidation models in vitro. Food chem. 2001, 73.3, 285–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Singh, R.P. , Chidambara Murthy, K.N. and Jayaprakasha, G.K. Studies on the antioxidant activity of pomegranate (Punica granatum) peel and seed extracts using in vitro models. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2002, 50, 81–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dhillon, B.; Awasthi, T.; Sodhi, N.S.; Sogi, D.S.; Jaiswal, S. A comparative study to investigate the effects of addition of milk and sugar on total polyphenol, flavonoid, catechin and tannin contents of green and black teas consumed in India. J. Food Meas. Charact. 2021, 15, 4652–4658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Galef, B.G., Jr.; Whiskin, E.E. Preference for novel flavors in adult Norway Rats (Rattus norvegicus). J. Comp. Psychol. 2003, 117, 96–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Brown, G.R.; Nemes, C. The exploratory behaviour of rats in the hole-board apparatus: is head-dipping a valid measure of neophilia? Behav. Process. 2008, 78, 442–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Caruso, M.J.; McClintock, M.K.; Cavigelli, S.A. Temperament moderates the influence of periadolescent social experience on behavior and adrenocortical activity in adult male rats. Horm. Behav. 2014, 66.3, 517–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Walters, E.M.; Prather, R.S. Advancing swine models for human health and diseases. Mo. Med. 2013, 110.3, 212. [Google Scholar]
  35. Fleming Stephen, A.; Dilger Ryan, N. Young pigs exhibit differential exploratory behavior during novelty preference tasks in response to age, sex, and delay, Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 321, 50–60. [Google Scholar]
  36. Roura, E.; Fu, M. Taste, nutrient sensing and feed intake in pigs (130 years of research: then, now and future), Anim. Feed Sci. Technol. 2017, 233, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Cirera, S.; Clop, A.; Jacobsen, M.J.; Guerin, M.; Lesnik, P.; Jørgensen, C.B.; Karlskov-Mortensen, P. A targeted genotyping approach enhances identification of variants in taste receptor and appetite/reward genes of potential functional importance for obesity-related porcine traits. Anim. Genet. 2018, 49, 110–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Clouard, C.; Val-Laillet, D. Impact of sensory feed additives on feed intake, feed preferences, and growth of female piglets during the early postweaning period. J. Anim. Sci. 2014, 92.5, 2133–2140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Guzmán-Pino, S. A. , Solà-Oriol, D. , Figueroa, J., & Pérez, J. F. Influence of the protein status of piglets on their ability to select and prefer protein sources, Physiol. Behav. 2014, 129, 43–49. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  40. Wang, J.; Yang, M.; Xu, S.; Lin, Y.; Che, L.; Fang, Z.; Wu, D. Comparative effects of sodium butyrate and flavors on feed intake of lactating sows and growth performance of piglets. Anim. Sci. J. 2014, 85.6, 683–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Zhu, L.; Wang, G.; Dong, B.; Peng, C.C.; Tian, Y.Y.; Gong, L.M. Effects of sweetener neotame on diet preference, performance and hematological and biochemical parameters of weaned piglets, Anim. Feed Sci. Technol. 2016, 214, 86–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Reyes-Camacho, D.; Pérez, J.F.; Vinyeta, E.; Aumiller, T.; Van der Klis, J.D.; Solà-Oriol, D. Prenatal Exposure to Innately Preferred D-Limonene and Trans-Anethole Does Not Overcome Innate Aversion to Eucalyptol. Affecting Growth Performance of Weanling Piglets. Animals 2021, 11, 2062. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ronda, V.; Aruna, C.; Visarada KB, R.S.; Bhat, B.V. Sorghum for animal feed. In Breeding Sorghum for diverse end uses, 1st ed.; Woodhead Publishing: Sawston, UK, 2019; pp. 229–238. [Google Scholar]
  44. Pan, L.; An, D.; Zhu, W.Y. Sorghum as a dietary substitute for corn reduces the activities of digestive enzymes and antioxidant enzymes in pigs. Anim. Feed Sci. Technol. 2021, 273, 114831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Corassa, A.; Stuani, J.L.; Ton, A.P.S.; Kiefer, C.; Sbardella, M.; Brito, C.O.; Amorim, A.B.; Gonçalves, D.B.C. Nutritional value of distillers dried grains with solubles from corn and sorghum and xylanase in diets for pigs. Rev. Bras. Zootec. 2019, 48, 20190012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Brambillasca, S.; Fernández–García, M.; Aguerre, M.; Repetto, J.L.; Cajarville, C. Characterization of the in vitro digestion of starch and fermentation kinetics of dry sorghum grains soaked or rehydrated and ensiled to be used in pig nutrition. J. Cereal Sci. 2019, 89, 102817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. McCuistion, K.C.; Selle, P.H.; Liu, S.Y.; Goodband, R.D. Sorghum as a feed grain for animal production. In Sorghum and Millets, 2nd ed.; Duodu K.G.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, NL, 2019; pp. 355–391. [Google Scholar]
  48. Thomas, L.L.; Espinosa, C.D.; Goodband, R.D.; Stein, H.H.; Tokach, M.D.; Dritz, S.S.; Woodworth, J.C.; DeRouchey, J.M. Nutritional evaluation of different varieties of sorghum and the effects on nursery pig growth performance. J. Anim. Sci. 2020, 98, skaa120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Fan, W.; Sun, X.; Cui, G.; Li, Q.; Xu, Y.; Wang, L.; Li, X.; Hu, B.; Chi, Z. A strategy of co-fermentation of distillers dried grains with solubles (DDGS) and lignocellulosic feedstocks as swine feed. Crit. Rev. Biotechnol. 2023, 43.2, 212–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. De Souza TC, R.; Árres IE, Á.; Gómez-Soto, J.G.; García, K.E.; Rodríguez, E.R.; Mariscal-Landín, G. Effect of tannins and kafirins on the nitrogen and energy balance and performance of pigs. Anim. Prod. Sci. 2023. [Google Scholar]
  51. Tran, T.; Billet, K.; Torres-Cobos, B.; Vichi, S.; Verdier, F.; Martin, A.; Alexandre, H.; Grandvalet, C.; Tourdot-Maréchal, R. Use of a minimal microbial consortium to determine the origin of kombucha flavor. Front. Microbiol. 2022, 13, 836617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Bishop, P.; Pitts, E.R.; Budner, D.; Thompson-Witrick, K. A. Kombucha: Biochemical and microbiological impacts on the chemical and flavor profile. Food Chem. Adv. 2022, 1, 100025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Redondo, N. , Gómez-Martínez, S., & Marcos, A. Sensory attributes of soft drinks and their influence on consumers' preferences. Food Funct. 2014, 5, 1686–1694. [Google Scholar]
  54. Barker, S.; Moss, R.; McSweeney, M.B. Carbonated emotions: Consumers’ sensory perception and emotional response to carbonated and still fruit juices. Food Res. Int. 2021, 147, 110534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Wendrick, N.A.; Sims, C.A.; MacIntosh, A.J. The Effect of Carbonation Level on the Acceptability and Purchase Intent of Muscadine and Fruit Wines. Beverages 2021, 7, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Jang, S.S. , McIntyre L., Chan M., Brown P.N., Finley J., Chen S.X. Ethanol Concentration of Kombucha Teas in British Columbia, Canada. J. Food Prot. 2021, 84, 1878–1883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Tran TM, K.; Kirkman, T.; Nguyen, M.; Van Vuong, Q. Effects of drying on physical properties, phenolic compounds and antioxidant capacity of Robusta wet coffee pulp (Coffea canephora). Heliyon 2020, 6. [Google Scholar]
  58. Nofre, C. , Glaser, D., Tinti, J.M. and Wanner, M. Gustatory responses of pigs to sixty compounds tasting sweet to humans. J. Anim. Physiol. Anim. Nutr. 2002, 86, 90–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Docking, C.M.; Van de Weerd, H.A.; Day JE, L.; Edwards, S. A. The influence of age on the use of potential enrichment objects and synchronisation of behaviour of pigs, Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 110, 244–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Jensen, P. (2017). The ethology of domestic animals: An Introductory Text, 3rd ed.; CABI: Boston, USA 2017; pp. 40–44. [Google Scholar]
  61. Urrutia, M.A.D.; Ramos, A.G.; Menegusso, R.B.; Lenz, R.D.; Ramos, M.G.; Tarone, A.G.; Cazarin, C.B.B.; Cottica, S.M.; da Silva, S.A.V.; Bernardi, D.M. Effects of supplementation with kombucha and green banana flour on Wistar rats fed with a cafeteria diet. Heliyon, 2021, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Chen, X.; Chen, K.; Cheng, H.; Liang, L. Soluble aggregates of myofibrillar proteins engineered by gallic acid: Colloidal structure and resistance to in vitro gastric digestion. J. Agric. Food Chem. 2022, 70.13, 4066–4075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hithamani, G.; Srinivasan, K. Effect of domestic processing on the polyphenol content and bioaccessibility in finger millet (Eleusine coracana) and pearl millet (Pennisetum glaucum). Food Chem. 2014, 164, 55–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. de Morais Cardoso, L.; Pinheiro, S.S.; de Carvalho, C.W.P.; Queiroz, V.A.V.; de Menezes, C.B.; Moreira, A.V.B.; de Barros, F.A.R.; Awika, J.M.; Martino, H.S.D.; Pinheiro-Sant'Ana, H.M. Phenolic compounds profile in sorghum processed by extrusion cooking and dry heat in a conventional oven. J. Cereal Sci. 2015, 65, 220–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lipsitch, M.; Tchetgen Tchetgen, E.; Cohen, T. Negative controls: a tool for detecting confounding and bias in observational studies. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 21.3, 383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Hulbert, L.E.; Bortoluzzi, E.M.; Luo, Y.; Mumm, J.M.; Coffin, M.J.; Becker, G.Y.; Vandevord, P.J.; McNeil, E.M.; Walilko, T.; Khaing, Z.Z.; Zai, L. Noninvasive, In-pen Approach Test for Laboratory-housed Pigs. JoVE, 2019, 148, 58597. [Google Scholar]
  67. Studnitz, M.; Jensen, M.B.; Pedersen, L.J. Why do pigs root and in what will they root?: A review on the exploratory behaviour of pigs in relation to environmental enrichment. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 107, 183–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Tatemoto, P.; Broom, D.M.; Zanella, A.J. Changes in Stereotypies: Effects over Time and over Generations. Animals 2022, 12, 2504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Wang, B.; Rutherfurd-Markwick, K.; Zhang, X.X.; Mutukumira, A.N. Isolation and characterisation of dominant acetic acid bacteria and yeast isolated from Kombucha samples at point of sale in New Zealand. Curr. Res. Nutr. Food Sci. 2022, 5, 835–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Figueroa, J.; Frías, D.; Solà-Oriol, D.; Tadich, T.; Franco-Rosselló, R; Nuñez, V.; Dwyer, D.M. Palatability in pigs, the pleasure of consumption. J. Anim. Sci. 2019, 97, 2165–2174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Charles, D.J.; Charles, D.J. 2013. Anise star. In Antioxidant Properties of Spices, Herbs and Other Sources, 1st ed; Springer: New York, USA, 2012; Volume 3, pp. 165–168. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Flow chart outlining the procedure for making fermented sorghum tea using symbiotic culture of bacteria and yeast (SCOBY).
Figure 1. Flow chart outlining the procedure for making fermented sorghum tea using symbiotic culture of bacteria and yeast (SCOBY).
Preprints 82068 g001
Figure 2. First Panel: 3PodShape Maze diagram- The isle was used as the chute and two standard gates were used to funnel the pig into the center entrance of the three pods (Entrance). After the subject entered the entrance, a pig board was used (dotted line) to close the maze. A bowl was placed in each pod and 100 mL of solution was offered. Second Panel- Flow chart representing each step of the preference study. 1The acclimation phase had 100 mL of isocaloric solution (sweet water) in every bowl and pigs that did not enter a pod were led into the area with a marshmallow or a feed pellet was dropped into the bowl. 2The phase did not include leading or extra marshmallows. 3The solutions were randomly assigned to pod so that each pig had a solution placed in at least three different pods.
Figure 2. First Panel: 3PodShape Maze diagram- The isle was used as the chute and two standard gates were used to funnel the pig into the center entrance of the three pods (Entrance). After the subject entered the entrance, a pig board was used (dotted line) to close the maze. A bowl was placed in each pod and 100 mL of solution was offered. Second Panel- Flow chart representing each step of the preference study. 1The acclimation phase had 100 mL of isocaloric solution (sweet water) in every bowl and pigs that did not enter a pod were led into the area with a marshmallow or a feed pellet was dropped into the bowl. 2The phase did not include leading or extra marshmallows. 3The solutions were randomly assigned to pod so that each pig had a solution placed in at least three different pods.
Preprints 82068 g002
Figure 3. Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, with the top error bar representing total duration SEM. The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). a,b,cLS Means differed (p < 0.05).
Figure 3. Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, with the top error bar representing total duration SEM. The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). a,b,cLS Means differed (p < 0.05).
Preprints 82068 g003
Figure 4. Line graph of sessions for total duration pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The error bars represent SEM. * p ≤ 0.05 negative control was less than isocaloric control and 3 Tea.
Figure 4. Line graph of sessions for total duration pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The error bars represent SEM. * p ≤ 0.05 negative control was less than isocaloric control and 3 Tea.
Preprints 82068 g004
Figure 5. Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). * p ≤ 0.05 LS Means for total duration LS means between 3Tea and Isocaloric; **p ≤ 0.01 for total LS means between –control and the other solutions. a,b,c p ≤ 0.05 LS Means within behavioral category.
Figure 5. Stack Bar Graph of pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were: an equal blend of fermented sorghum tea type (3Tea; white, sumac, and roasted sumac); isocaloric solution (-control; sucrose water), or distilled water (+control). The entire bar represents the total duration (% of observation) of time spent in pod with the corresponding solution, The bottom stacks represent the duration of head in bowl, the middle stacks represent the percent duration of exploratory behaviors (i.e. non-nutritive oral behaviors), and the top stacks represent the percent duration of all other behaviors (head still, non-oral, walking or standing). * p ≤ 0.05 LS Means for total duration LS means between 3Tea and Isocaloric; **p ≤ 0.01 for total LS means between –control and the other solutions. a,b,c p ≤ 0.05 LS Means within behavioral category.
Preprints 82068 g005
Table 1. Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from: white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted) sorghum. Pigs were exposed (Exp) four times after conditioning.
Table 1. Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with fermented sorghum tea type. The choices were fermented tea made from: white, sumac, or roasted sumac (roasted) sorghum. Pigs were exposed (Exp) four times after conditioning.
Choice Exposure p-Values
White Sumac Roasted SEM1 1 2 3 4 SEM1 Choice Exp Choice x Exp
Consumption2, mL 95.2 97.9 95.6 ±2.34 87.9a 99.9b 99.2b 97.9b ±2.64 0.628 0.005 0.653
 Gallic acid, mg 3.3a 13.7b 12.7c ±0.22 9.1a 10.3b 10.2b 10.1b ±0.25 <0.001 0.008 0.201
Duration, %
Total 28.2 31.8 30.0 ±1.25 30.1 29.8 30.5 29.7 ±1.48 0.208 0.988 0.922
Bowl3 21.9 24.2 23.9 ±1.69 23.7 21.0 24.6 24.0 ±1.80 0.264 0.213 0.923
Explore4,5 3.1 3.3 2.3 ±0.92 3.3a 4.7a 1.7b 1.8b ±1.01 0.617 0.029 0.958
Non-oral behaviors6 3.3a 4.3b,c 3.8c ±0.45 3.1d 4.0e 4.1e 3.9e ±0.48 0.050 0.097 0.949
Latency, %
Bowl3 24.4 24.7 26.8 ±3.60 30.9 19.3 24.3 26.7 ±4.22 0.890 0.327 0.672
Explore4 62.7 67.5 74.1 ±6.10 56.6d 68.7e 75.8e 71.1e ±6.58 0.192 0.060 0.970
Other6 23.5 23.8 27.5 ±7.85 30.8 19.5 23.4 26.1 ±8.10 0.696 0.338 0.626
Rate, no./min
Bowl3 0.68 0.70 0.68 ±0.068 0.57d,e 0.70e,f 0.72e,f 0.76f ±0.073 0.904 0.069 0.982
Explore4 0.40 0.43 0.32 ±0.079 0.47 0.36 0.36 0.35 ±0.084 0.302 0.360 0.907
Other6 0.97 1.15 1.08 ±0.120 0.81a 1.1b 1.2b 1.2b ±0.126 0.173 0.003 0.962
1Largest Standard Error Mean; 2Total volume consumed (100 mL maximum) data were non-parametric when Choice was considered. The Kruskal-Wallis Test for Consumption Choice was not significant (χ2 = 0.39; p = 0.82; df = 2) but was significant for gallic acid (χ2 = 97.5; p < 0.001; df = 2), 3Head over bowl, head moving, indicating drinking; 4p-values were derived from square root transformed data; 5All non-nutritive oral behaviors (NNOB); 6Walking, standing without any oral-related behavior (i.e. drinking, sniffing, licking, rooting, or rubbing using the mouth, snout, or face); a,b,c LS Means p ≤ 0.05; d,e,f LS Means p ≥ 0.05 ≤ 0.10.
Table 2. Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with choices of: distilled water (-control), isocaloric control (+control; water with sugar) or fermented sorghum blended tea (3Tea; equal parts of white, sumac, and roasted sumac). Pigs were exposed (Exp) to the 3-choice test three times.
Table 2. Pig (n = 11) preference behaviors in a 3-Choice maze with choices of: distilled water (-control), isocaloric control (+control; water with sugar) or fermented sorghum blended tea (3Tea; equal parts of white, sumac, and roasted sumac). Pigs were exposed (Exp) to the 3-choice test three times.
Choice Exposure p-values
-control +control 3Tea SEM1 1 2 3 SEM1 Choice Exp Choice x Exp
Consumption2, mL 18.0a 96.6b 99.0b ±2.21 74.1 68.9 70.6 ±2.97 <0.001 0.136 0.087
 Gallic Acid, mg 0.02a 0.20b 11.7c ±0.03 3.8a 3.8a 4.3b ±0.03 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001
Duration, %
Total 16.8a 34.5b 37.7c ±1.71 28.4 29.5 31.1 ±1.71 <0.001 0.597 0.033
Bowl3 5.6a 27.6b 31.9c ±1.87 20.3 21.8 23.0 ±1.92 <0.001 0.365 0.091
Explore4,5 7.1a 3.0b 1.4b ±1.45 4.4 3.5 3.5 ±1.50 0.002 0.866 0.261
Non-oral behaviors6 4.2 4.1 4.4 ±0.45 4.5 4.2 4.0 ±0.46 0.583 0.321 0.506
Latency, %
Bowl3 20.8 16.2 14.8 ±3.45 13.93 19.59 18.19 ±3.61 0.420 0.481 0.681
Explore4 60.1d 71.2e 76.0e ±7.55 69.0 65.7 72.6 ±7.74 0.086 0.608 0.714
Other6 16.2 15.2 13.8 ±3.11 12.2 15.6 17.4 ±3.26 0.849 0.480 0.958
Rate, no./min
Bowl3 20.75 16.18 14.77 ±3.459 0.42 0.48 0.68 ±3.615 0.420 0.481 0.681
Explore4 0.55d 0.38e 0.35e ±0.108 0.39 0.53 0.37 ±0.111 0.102 0.196 0.646
Other6 1.41 1.44 1.52 ±0.150 1.49 1.46 1.41 ±0.152 0.526 0.730 0.413
1Largest Standard Error Mean; 2Total volume consumed (100 mL maximum) data were non-parametric when Choice was considered. The Kruskal-Wallis Test for Choice was significant for consumption volume (χ2 = 0.71.6; P < 0.001; df = 2), and for gallic acid (χ2 = 88.9; p < 0.001; df = 2),4P-Values were derived from square root transformed data; 5All non-nutritive oral behaviors (NNOB); 6All non-oral behaviors walking or standing without any sniffing, drinking, chomping, licking, rooting, rubbing using the mouth, snout, or face); a,b,c LS Means ≤ 0.05; d,e LS Means ≥ 0.05 ≤ 0.10.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated