Preprint
Article

Intranasal Oxytocin and Pain Reduction: Testing a Social Cognitive Mediation Model

Altmetrics

Downloads

95

Views

32

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

10 November 2023

Posted:

13 November 2023

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Oxytocin is well known for its role in relationships and social cognition and has more recently been implicated in pain relief and pain perception. Connections between prosocial feelings and pain relief are also well documented, however, effects of exogenous oxytocin on social cognition and pain have not been explored. The current study tested whether intranasally-delivered oxytocin affects pain perception through prosocial behaviors. Additionally, moderation of the effects of oxytocin by life history or genetic polymorphisms are examined. Young adults (n=43; 65% female) were administered intranasal oxytocin (24 IU) or placebo in a crossover design on two visits separated by a one-week washout period. Pain was delivered via cold-pressor. Baseline measures for decision-making and social cognition were collected, as well as pain sensitivity and medication history. Saliva samples were collected for analysis of genetic markers, and urine samples were collected to assess oxytocin saturation. Following oxytocin administration, participants reported increased prosocial cognition and decision-making. Pain perception appeared to be adaptive, with procedural order and expectation affecting perception. Finally, behavioral trust and cooperation responses were significantly predicted by genetic markers. Oxytocin may increase a patient’s trust and cooperation and reduce pain sensitivity while having fewer physiological side effects than current pharmaceutical options.
Keywords: 
Subject: Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Introduction

Oxytocin & Pain

Oxytocin (OT) is well known for its involvement in feelings of prosocial behavior, partner selection, and for its role in childbirth [1,2,3,4,5,6]. The ability of exogenous OT to mediate emotion and impulsivity has also been explored, however, less is known about OT’s ability to mediate pain. While some evidence suggests that in both rodents and humans, OT can directly alleviate pain, primarily through anti-inflammatory properties [7,8,9,10], mediation of emotional and cognitive perception of pain may be a likely mode of action in humans [11]. Additionally, OT administration may lead to adaptive pain-related behaviors such as increased trust and cooperation, particularly in acute pain conditions. The bi-dimensional relationship is also supported, with evidence demonstrating that feelings of trust and safety decrease pain ratings [12]. Furthermore, attitudinal levels of trust in an individual – an inherent predisposition to trust – may predict pain tolerance/sensitivity or pain relief efficacy. OT’s ability to amplify a sense of well-being and attenuate stress and anxiety could be the driving force behind its analgesic effect [13]. Hence, one strong theory for OT's analgesic effect is its mediation between trust and pain, or in other words, OT attenuates pain by increasing trust. However, to date, this exploration has had mixed and inconsistent findings.
A recent meta-analysis examined the effects of intranasal oxytocin (OTIN) on self-perception of pain across 15 studies [14]. Results indicated that while some individual studies reported significant differences in pain perception, the overall findings yielded no significant statistical effects of OTIN compared to placebo regarding subject ratings of pain intensity or unpleasantness, pain threshold, or pain tolerance. These results challenge earlier assertions that OT directly affects pain perception in humans [15]. Alternatively, OT analgesic effects are idiosyncratic, tied to individual factors such as personality traits and life history, and to contextual factors such as situational factors and cognitive state of mind. The more complex story unfolding between OT and pain perception may be similar to the one linking oxytocin to social cognition [16].
In the remainder of this introduction, we present a review of the current research on the neurological mechanisms associating OT with pain, followed by a discussion on potential moderating factors, including life history, genetic markers, and environmental considerations. Lastly, the authors present embodied cognition as a conceptual framework from which the hypotheses on the indirect effects and context-modulated effects of OT on pain perception are formed.

Mechanisms

Kemp and Guastella [17] proposed a novel mechanism for explaining OT effects on pain based on neural systems regulating approach and avoidance motivation [17]. This is a significant theoretical advancement in the field [18]. The authors propose that the effects of OT on social behavior can be attributed to its influence on the neural systems underlying approach and avoidance motivation, suggesting that OT's impact extends beyond social interactions to various adaptive and maladaptive behaviors. They posit that OT affects the mesocorticolimbic circuitry linked to approach and the cortico-amygdala circuitry associated with avoidance. Additionally, they contend that OT enhances the salience of personally relevant and emotionally evocative stimuli, which may apply to both social and non-social cues. The authors propose an alternative conceptual framework, the General Approach-Avoidance Oxytocin (GAAO) model, which is gaining traction in the field (see review by [18]).
Neural measurements in some studies supported the notion that OT affects pain-related neuronal circuits, but a direct relationship between neural changes and self-perception of pain was not consistently observed [18]. In conclusion, while the analysis suggests a limited direct impact of intranasal OT on self-perception of pain, the diversity, and limitations of the studies warrant cautious interpretation, and further research is needed in human trials to explore the potential therapeutic effects of OTIN in specific painful conditions. Animal studies have consistently demonstrated that OT acts as a robust analgesic, but human studies have yielded mixed results with smaller effects [19]. This may be due to the mediating effects of the current environment during OTs receipt, and past experiences with endogenous OT. Hence, what has not been adequately explored is whether the effects of OT on human pain are mediated by the immediate social context and/or moderated by individual background.

Life history considerations

A dependency on context and life history may be a key explanatory factor of the varying social effects of OTIN. In part, this could be explained through conditioning. For instance, one of OT's natural releases occurs during skin-on-skin contact with others, an effect that occurs independent of type of touch or quality of the connection [20]. Thus, if a person is abused regularly in childhood, they might come to associate the release of OT with dread or disgust [21]. If true, the effects of OT would be interdependent on context and life history [16]. This lends an explanation to the effect of trust on pain, as it is quite imaginable that attitudinal levels of trust are related to life history, and suggests that life history affects the emotional valence of OT.
There is sufficient evidence to suggest an interconnection between OT and social-environmental factors. One prospective neurological investigation into the relationship between early life stress (ELS) and OT revealed that individuals who experienced emotional abuse in early life exhibit decreased functional connectivity between the right amygdala and pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (pgACC). This association remains even after accounting for age, IQ, and gray matter volume [22]. Notably, emotional abuse was the most significant predictor of this altered connectivity. Furthermore, OT's moderating effect on this circuit was diminished in individuals with higher ELS scores, emphasizing the importance of considering both early life experiences and functional dynamics in developing OT-based therapeutic interventions [22]. Their findings provide evidence for life histories' ability to blunt the effects of OT.
Several studies have found that previous relationships with caregivers and romantic partners can shape OT’s influence, be it positively or negatively [23]. Several studies have found that OTIN amplified the analgesic effect of holding a partner’s hand while experiencing pain [24]. Similarly, whether an individual is currently in a new romantic relationship or not can influence the impact of exogenous OT on feelings of attraction toward strangers [21]. In another example, the impact of OT on charitable giving was more substantial among individuals with positive experiences in their parental relationships [25]. In general, positive life history and personal familiarity tend to enhance OT effects on trust and cooperation [16].

Biological & physiological considerations

To further explore the effects of OT at the individual level, genetics must be considered. There are currently many supported single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) for physiological and psychological predispositions, including response to endogenous OT. Prosocial behaviors can be influenced by OT, and polymorphisms on the OT receptor gene have been linked to trust and social cooperation [26,27]. There is mounting evidence for biomarkers associated with OT uptake and receptivity [28,29,30], where an individual’s genotype can influence sensitivity to OT responsiveness and efficacy. In the current study, we assay SNPs related to pain sensitivity, OT sensitivity, impulsivity, and attitudinal trust to determine possible relationships between pain perception and OT efficacy.
Recent research has revealed how our physical state can affect our decision-making capabilities. Factors like hunger and sleep deprivation can impact impulsivity and risk/reward behaviors [31,32]. The sensation of pain can significantly influence decision-making [33,34], as well as serve as an adaptive alarm by sending signals from the body to alert us of potential health issues and motivate us to take corrective action [35,36]. Intranasal administration of OT has been shown to enhance various prosocial behaviors such as generosity, charitable giving, empathy, and trust [16]. It has also been shown to attenuate negative drug behaviors of substances like cocaine and methamphetamine [37].

A Conceptual Framework

Embodied cognition is the concept that various elements present in a person's surroundings can influence intricate human actions [38]. Studies have demonstrated that acute pain can lead to an increase in risk-taking behavior, trust, cooperation, and a likelihood to defer rewards [39,40], suggesting that our physical experiences can significantly impact our behavior. These connections between pain and decision-making can be understood as evolutionary adaptations. For example, in delayed discounting tasks, wherein a reward loses subjective value the farther out in time its receipt is anticipated, abstinence from specific drugs or a lower socioeconomic status may lead to an increased response, while deprivation from sleep, food, or water may have varying effects [41]. When it comes to managing pain, individuals tend to prioritize quick relief over delayed gratification, with chronic pain affecting the ability to see past ‘now’, including factors such as the severity of the pain, the impact it has on daily activities, and the availability of resources for long-term pain management [42]. Moreover, the level of risk associated with various pain relief methods can play a significant role in decision-making. Acute and chronic pain can drive individuals to increase risky behavior and poorer decision-making, regardless of future outcomes [33,43,44,45]. Trust and cooperation can both be considered risky and have been well-established in a variety of paradigms [46,47,48,49,50,51], however, see [52].
Thus, using OT as an analgesic may offer a unique combination of benefits under certain conditions. Correct application could potentially pave the way for a safer and more comprehensive approach to pain treatment. Traditional pharmaceutical analgesics primarily target pain reduction, but they have been found to diminish positive social behaviors that often accompany pain. These medications, such as acetaminophen, only address pain levels and tend to diminish the prosocial effects pain has on behavior, even simple over-the-counter analgesics [53]. Therefore, a novel therapeutic approach is to leverage the prosocial and behavioral aspects of pain. OT is well situated to fill this gap. The current study measured pain-related behaviors by examining decision-making tendencies and distinct social interactions. Specifically, we investigated whether OT moderates the effects of pain on individuals' sense of safety, trust, and willingness to cooperate. We hypothesized that acute pain would increase prosocial cognitions indicated by increased interpersonal trust, cooperation, and psychological safety scores, and that administration of OTIN will directly affect these measures. Additionally, we predict that acute pain levels will mediate the relationship between OT and decision-making, where a decreased perception of pain will be evidenced by decreased delayed discounting and loss aversion scores.

2. MATERIAL AND METHODS

Subject Sample & Procedure

Forty-three participants were recruited from the University of South Dakota (65% female, age M = 20, SD = 0.97). Pre-screening was conducted online, and individuals who met criteria were invited to participate in the laboratory portion of the study, consisting of a randomized, placebo-controlled crossover study over 2 sessions, separated by a 1-week washout period (see Figure 1 for a schematic of the study procedure). The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and all participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the experimental procedures approved by the University of South Dakota Institutional Review Board (protocol IRB-18-26, approval date: 21 September 2018). Subjects were compensated $25.00 following the first visit, and $65.00 following project completion. A sample size of forty-five was targeted based on similar previous studies [54] calculated in G*Power using an average effect size of 0.20, with a predicted power of 0.80.

Laboratory Testing

Participants were screened for exclusionary criteria to determine any contraindications to intranasal oxytocin administration, including use of blood pressure, antipsychotic or antidepressant medication, serious illness or infection, current pregnancy or recurrent nasal or sinus pain. In addition, exclusionary factors that could influence pain scores such as current use of analgesics or recent injury. Baselines for all decision-making and social cognition measures were collected in addition to key controls such as pain sensitivity and medication history.
At arrival, participants were given verbal instructions and a sterile collection cup (BD Vacutainer, 364956, Fisher Scientific, Hanover Park, IL) labeled with the participant’s specific code, time, and date to collect a urine sample. Samples were collected in a clean-catch container with a needle port, and kept on ice until processing. A vacutainer was then connected to the port and aliquots were collected from the sealed system and stored at -80oC until assayed for oxytocin. All female subjects were given a urine pregnancy test (HCG AccuMed; AccuMed Biotech LLC, Houston, TX; a positive test resulted in exclusion from further participation; 0 participants excluded). A saliva sample was collected for DNA extraction and analysis (Oragene-Discover; OGR-500, DNA Genotek, Ottowa, ON, Canada).

Intranasal Oxytocin Administration

Participants received 24 IU of IN (van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2015; Syntocinon®, NOVARTIS, Victoria Pharmacy, Zurich, Switzerland) or matched placebo (Laboswiss, Davos Platz, Switzerland) administered via nasal spray. Subjects receiving placebo at the first visit received OT at the second, for a crossover study design.

Measurements

Assessments began after a 30-minute rest period, participants were instructed to complete an online questionnaire on decision-making, cooperation, safety, and trust, followed by the cold pressor task (detailed below). During the cold pressor task, participants underwent 6 behavioral rating tests: Barratt Impulsivity Scale-11[55], Loss Aversion Scale, Delayed Discounting, Psychological Safety Scale [56], and the Interpersonal Trust Scale [57], as well as a trust game consisting of the Prisoner’s Dilemma and the Stag Hunt. The loss aversion scale was internally developed for the study following the methods and proportions of Kahneman & Tversky prospect theory [58]. measurement. In addition, the delayed discounting measure was calculated using the formula presented by Kirby & Marakovic (1995)[59].

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale – 11 (BIS-11)

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale created by Dr. Ernest Barratt is currently in is the eleventh edition. It assesses the personality trait of impulsivity across a multidimensional framework. This scale includes thirty items measuring three distinct constructs within impulsivity: attentional, motor, and nonplanning. Every item is scaled from one to four (Rarely/Never=1, Occasionally=2, Often=3, Almost, Always/Always=4). Twelve of the thirty items are reverse coded, which includes the entire subscale of self-control. Total scores on the BIS-11 are most typical, however, second-factor scores can be appropriate. The BIS-11 has a high internal reliability of 0.776 as measured by Cronbach’s alpha. It also demonstrates an acceptable test-retest reliability of 0.80 calculated using Pearson’s r [60].

Psychological Safety Scale

This measure assesses an individual’s perceived psychological safety within the context of a team. Psychological safety is operationalized as the degree of interpersonal risk one is comfortable with taking. Participants are asked to imagine themselves within a specific team or group and consider their responses within said context. The psychological safety scale contains seven items with a 1-5 Likert selection response. All seven items show strong inter-item correlation, and the scale has an overall high Cronbach’s alpha of 0.93 [56].

The Interpersonal Trust Scale (ITS)

ITS assesses trust between people across 40 declarative statements. Twenty-five items are considered overall Trust measures, and the remaining 15 items are labeled as “filler”, meaning they are only superficially related to trust. The inclusion of these items are to boost fidelity although they are not calculated into the final score. Confirmatory Factor analysis validates the dimensions of trust and interpersonal behaviors as proposed by the author. The interpersonal Trust Scale demonstrates strong reliability and validity [57]. Research on Common Dilemmas has found this scale to be a significant predictor of trusting behavior. High scores on the ITS are correlated with the increase in belief of promises and degree of cooperation [61].

Trust Game Dilemmas

The Prisoner’s Dilemma and the Stag Hunt were used to assess applied scenarios involving trust.
The Prisoner’s Dilemma was originally created by the mathematician Albert Tucker in 1950 as a means for depicting the cost and benefits of social competition [62]. The Stag Hunt was originally created by the philosopher Jean-Jacques Rousseau. Both vignettes were adapted to follow the same mathematical format but are still applied in two different contexts. In either, there are potentially rewards and consequences for trusting others. The Prisoner’s Dilemma is set in an interrogation environment and uses sentence duration as the motivator. The length of one’s sentence depends on a combination of their choice to either remain silent or talk, and the hypothetical other’s same choice. The Stag Hunt is set in the woods and uses food as the key incentive. The Stag Hunt’s outcome is based on the player’s choice and their assumption of what the other non-real player would do. In this scenario, a player can either trust a fellow hunter to wait for the larger reward of a stag, or attempt to take a rabbit prematurely which would provide them and them alone with food.

Parent Love-Withdrawal Scale

The Parent Love-Withdrawal Scale is a twenty-two-item questionnaire. It is split into two sections of repeating questions, one regarding the mother and one the father. Each item response ranges from 1-5 on a Likert-type scale. All of the items are negatively worded and inquire about the parent’s behavior and disciplinary approach given a variety of subject actions, such as disagreeing or disappointing. The PLWS was specific designed and intended for use as a moderator in oxytocin studies. A meta-analysis of eight studies found consistent evidence for the association between parent-child relations and OT levels [63]. The scale has a moderate to high Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87 [25].

Expectancy Bias Scale

The Expectancy Bias Scale is a brief measure developed by the researchers to account for trait levels of subject optimism prior to participation. It is comprised of six items with responses ranging from 1-5 of likelihood that a hypothetical event would occur within the next six months. Three items were reverse coded and described negative events occurring, such as, “receiving unexpected bad news”.

The Visual Analog Scale

The most common subjective instrumentation of the measurement of pain is the Visual Analog Scale (VAS). It is prolific because of its simplicity and time efficiency. There are several adaptations of it including requiring participants to mar an “X" somewhere on a 10mm line. The version used in this study scale asks for a patient’s subjective report of their pain intensity verbally on a one through ten scale. In addition, it is paired with a series of increasingly unhappy faces. The VAS has a high reliability with a Cronbach’s alpha of .93 [64].

Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)

The Beck Anxiety Inventory contains 21 items on a 0-3 response scale. The items are either subjective, somatic, or panic-related symptoms of anxiety. The BAI demonstrates sufficient test-retest reliability over time (r=.67) and a high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha =.94) [65]. The Beck Anxiety Inventory is considered the gold standard of anxiety measures within clinical psychology settings.

Delayed Discounting Measure (DDM)

The Delayed Discounting Measure uses the tradeoff scaling method across twelve items. Each item has two competing options. Option 1 is a monetary gain received in the present. Option 2 is a larger monetary gain received at a varying amount of days in the future. A tradeoff usually occurs from option 1 to option 2 when the participant is content with the size difference between the later reward relative to its receipt delay. This scale is meant to measure hyperbolic discounting specifically. The hyperbolic rate of discounting has already been calculated for each of the items. The hyperbolic discounting function is derived using the following formula: f(D)=1/(1+kt) [59]. This number describes how much an individual discounts the value of delayed rewards (when option 1 is chosen). The assigned quotient will be classified and used to mark the level of discounting the participant made at their tradeoff point. The classification for levels of discounting is broken into three categories: 4 lowest (low discounting), middle 4 (mild discounting), highest 4 (high discounting).

Loss Aversion Scale (LAS)

The Loss Aversion Scale measures the amount of money at which an individual is enticed enough to take a risk. The scale includes three options. Option 1 is a certainty. It yields an intermediate outcome, that is, there is no risk of loss nor chance of gain. Option 2 is a gamble yielding a gain with a probability of 50% and a loss (set at $10) with the same probability. The monetary gain is varied until indifference results and a tradeoff from certainty to risk occurs. The certain outcome is not varied and is, therefore, most naturally taken as the reference point. For a person who is averse to loss, the monetary gain must then be extra high to offset the loss [58]. The bigger the separation between the value of the gain and loss the more loss averse and individual. The score will be calculated by dividing the expected value ((Probability of Winning) x (Amount Won per Outcome) – (Probability of Losing) x (Amount Lost per Outcome)) of the gain by the expected value of the loss. The higher the score the more loss averse the individual.

Chronic Pain Grade Scale (CPGS)

The Chronic Pain Grade Scale is a short seven-item survey used to assess pain intensity and associated disability. The first three items measure pain intensity on a 1-10 scale where 0 is 'no pain' and 10 is 'pain as bad as it could be'. The last four items measure pain associated disability. The fourth items ask the participant to report the number of days the pain has kept them from engaging in their usual activities. The final three items use a shared 1-10 scale where 0 is 'no change' and 10 is 'extreme change' inquiring about specific activities. All items ask the participant to consider the past six months when answering [66]. All items are scored on an 11-point Likert scale, ranging from 0–10. The scores arecalculated for 3 subscales: the characteristic pain intensity score (items 1,2,3), is calculated as the mean intensity ratings for reported current, worst, and average pain. The disability score (item 5,6,7) is calculated as the mean rating for difficulty performing daily, social, and work activities. Finally, the disability points score, which ranges from 0–3, is derived from a combination of ranked categories of a number of disability days (item 4) and disability score. Subscale scores for pain intensity and disability are combined to calculate a chronic pain grade that classifies chronic pain patients into 5 hierarchical categories: grades 0 (no pain) to 5 (high disability-severely limiting) [66]. Reliability and validity checks have shown this metric to be sound as seen in a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.74, and a high two-week test-retest reliability score. Higher scores on the CPGS significantly predict greater chronic pain, the likelihood of unemployment, use of opioids, physician visits and depressed moods all suggesting sufficient construct validity [67].

Cold Pressor Task

The cold pressor task was designed by a team of human factors psychologists at the Hiemstra laboratory in the University of South Dakota following the standard design [68] to maintain a precise 40-degree Fahrenheit temperature. The pressor spanned across two rooms: a storage room which contained a large freezer (set to 40 degrees) and a pump that circulated the water, and in the next room, a small cooler in which the water flowed. A small mesh lining was inserted in the cooler to guarantee the same resting depth of each inserted hand. Water was constantly recirculated back into the freezer holding a consistent temperature. A small amount of chlorine (4/ppm) was added to ensure participant safety. The pump and freezer were separated from the participation room to reduce environmental noise and distraction. All subjects were instructed to submerge their hand to the start of the wrist, at which point a five-minute timer was started. They were allowed to remove their hand if needed. Subject pain levels were recorded every 30 seconds at designated timepoints between the pain-relevant questionnaires.

Urinary Oxytocin Extraction

Urine samples were transported frozen on dry ice to Assay Services at the Wisconsin National Primate Center (University of Wisconsin-Madison) for OT extraction and analysis. Aliquots were thawed, vortexed, and centrifuged for solid phase extraction (SPE, 100 mg, C-18, Oasis SepPak, WAT 023590, Waters Technologies Corp, Milford, MA) prior to OT analyses by enzyme-immunoassay (Oxytocin ELISA kit, ADI-901-153A, Enzo Life Sciences, Farmingdale, NY). Methods for SPE of OT have been published previously [69,70,71,72,73,74] briefly, columns were conditioned with 1 ml methanol followed by 1 ml distilled water using vacuum pressure. Cartridges were washed with 1 ml 10% Acetonitrile/0.1% Trifluroacetic acid to waste, then eluted with 1 ml of 80% acetonitrile, and evaporated in a SpeedVac at 45oC for 1 hour. Immediately after drying, samples were stored at 2-8oC in 300 µl ethanol until assayed.

Urinary Oxytocin and Creatinine Analysis

Pre and post OT administration extracted samples were brought to room temperature and were dried and resuspended in 250 µl of Assay Buffer. Duplicates of each sample, standard, and control were assayed following an overnight incubation. Oxytocin levels were detected using an automatic plate reader (SpectraMax, Molecular Devices, San Jose, CA) at an absorbance of 405 nm. Samples were compared to known standards and absorbance values were used to determine OT values. To account for variation in concentration of urine, creatinine was measured from each sample using a picric acid microtiter plate [69,75]. Oxytocin was expressed as pg OT per mg Creatinine for each sample. Pre and Post sample and percent change were calculated for each participant. Average time between pre and post sample was 1:10 ± 0.00 for visit 1 and 1:04 ± 0.00 for visit 2; the non-specific binding percent was 0.09% (recommended value 0.0%). The detection limit sensitivity of the assay was 15.0 pg/mL.

DNA Extraction

DNA from 43 samples were extracted using the QIAsymphony® instrument coupled with the QIAsymphony® DSP DNA midi kit version 1.0 according to the manufacturer’s instructions (QIAGEN Inc, Germantown, MD). The extracted DNA was eluted into ATE buffer composed of 10 mM Tris-Cl pH 8.3, 0.1 mM EDTA, and 0.04% NaN3 (sodium azide) with a final eluate volume of 60 μl.

SNP Genotyping

Extracted genomic DNA from each sample was normalized in DNA suspension buffer composed of 10 mM Tris and 0.1 mM EDTA to a working concentration of 20 ng/μl. For each SNP, normalized genomic DNA was added to a cocktail of TaqMan® Genotyping Master Mix and TaqMan® fluorescently labeled probes (Vic and Fam) in a ratio of 2 μl, 2.5 μl, and 1.5 μl respectively (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA). The DNA/PCR mix was loaded into a Bio-Rad 384 well plate (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA) to undergo PCR amplification. PCR was performed on the Applied Biosystems ViiA 7 Real-Time PCR System (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) with the following cycling parameters: 30 second pre-read stage at 60.0°C, 10 minute initial denaturing step at 95.0°C, PCR stage consisting of a 92.0°C denature step alternating with a 60.0°C annealing step for 40 cycles, followed by a 30 second post-read stage at 60.0°C. Genotype calls were made using TaqMan® Genotyper Software v1.3.1 by Life Technologies (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA).

Study Design

The treatment intervention was a double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over challenge study of OT versus placebo separated by a 1-week washout period. After the online pre-screening procedure, the intranasal challenge was given on two visits separated by a one-week drug-free period. Subjects who met protocol criteria were randomly allocated to receive either OT or placebo in a 1:1 ratio at the first visit. Subjects who received OT at the first visit received placebo at the second visit, and subjects that received placebo at the first visit received OT at the second visit. Subject assessments were done by an evaluator who was blinded to the content of the OT challenge. Immediately after OTIN or placebo administration, during a rest period of 30 minutes, subjects were seated at a computer and completed decision-making, cooperation, safety, and trust questionnaires. Upon completion of the questionnaires, once the OT had entered the subject’s system, participants underwent a cold pressor task for five minutes. Following the cold pressor a brief trust game was conducted, and the initial questionnaires repeated.

2. RESULTS

Data Exploration

All (n=43) subjects completed both rounds of the trial. The data was assessed using SPSS version # 27. No imputation was used for missing values and no transformations were completed following a review of the skewness and kurtosis of the primary variables. A table of the control, pain, and baseline measures descriptive and bivariate correlations can be seen in Table 1. A review of the table shows that parent history, expectations, and trait anxiety were all significantly correlated. Additionally, pain levels under the OT condition are significantly correlated to expectations, but pain levels under the placebo condition were not. Further exploration of bivariate correlations show that trust game choice had low but significant association with baseline control measures but demonstrated weak association with independent and dependent variables. No variables were significantly correlated, however, two controls were used as covariates due to their relevance in the literature; sex (p=.059) and anxiety (p=.067), in addition to procedural order.
It was additionally inquired at the end of each laboratory visit if the subjects believed that they had received OTIN or placebo. Between both visits, only 17 of 43 subjects (41%) correctly identified having received OT. Participant accuracy was consistent with previous studies that indicate little to no perceptual awareness associated with taking OTIN (i.e., no better than chance).

Main Effects

The first set of analyses analysis examined our hypotheses that OT would lesson pain and increase prosocial cognitions. The main effect of OT on pain was first examined. An ANCOVA was performed on pain while controlling for expectation bias (r= .378, p=.012) and procedural order (whether the participant received OT in the first visit or the second visit). The main effect was nonsignificant, although, two associations were found with the covariates. Results also show a significant interaction effect between procedural order and pain F(1,41) =5.817 , p = .03, Cohen’s d = .112.
Mean comparisons show that subjects who received OT on their first visit (M = 5.72) reported the highest pain levels of all four groups, while subjects who received OT in visit two (M = 4.76) reported the lowest. The most painful and least painful conditions were both on OT. In the first visit, OT increases pain levels compared to placebo, but in the second visit, OT decreases pain significantly compared to placebo, illustrated by a clear moderation effect (see Figure 2).
The other covariate, expectation bias, was a significant predictor of OT pain scores (r2=0.14, p=0.012), with lower expectations predicting lower pain under the OT condition, but not for placebo.
Pain scores by visit order of experimental condition.

OT & Social cognitions (H1-3)

An ANCOVA was conducted to test OT main effects on attitudinal trust with baseline measures and procedural order entered as covariates. There was a significant main effect of experimental condition on attitudinal trust scores F(1,39) =7.011 , p = .012, np2 = .152. A comparison of the means reveals that OT increased attitudinal trust (MPL=2.61 versus MOT=2.69). A significant interaction effect was found with trust measured at baseline F(1,39) =4.787 , p = .035, np2 = .112 suggesting a trait bias. That is, subjects with a higher baseline level of trust experienced a greater increase in trust following OT administration. A significant interaction effect was also found with procedural order F(1,39) =4.085 , p = .05, np2 = .095. This moderation resulted in similar effects as seen with pain, in which the highest levels of trust were reported while on OT in visit one (M=2.801), with the lowest levels in OT on visit two (2.571). Placebo levels of trust were practically unaffected (placebo visit 1 M=2.619, placebo visit 2 M=2.615).
The next ANCOVA was performed on cooperation using its baseline, procedural order, and the control variable parent-love withdrawal history (r= -.319, p=.037) as covariates. There was a significant main effect of experimental condition on willingness to cooperate F(1,40) =5.819 , p = .021, np2 = .130, indicating an increase in cooperation from placebo (MPL=59.7) to drug (MOT=60.0). Interaction effects between the DV and parent-love withdrawal were nonsignificant. Interaction with baseline approached significance F(1, 40) = 3.45, p = .053 (see Table 2). Higher levels at baseline were associated with an increased effect of OT on cooperation.

Analysis of covariance means difference test for cooperation by condition with controls (n=43).

The next ANCOVA was conducted to assess the difference in trust game choice between conditions. There was a significant main effect of experimental condition on behavioral trust F(1,40) =9.558 , p = .004, np2 = .197. Review of the group means show OT exposure increased the likelihood of trust choice selection, when controlling for procedural order. It should be noted that the trusting choice was more prevalent across all conditions. A significant interaction effect was not found between behavioral trust and sex or anxiety. However, post hoc analysis supports the presence of a simple effect by sex (see Table 3). A frequency comparison indicates that males were more like to switch from trusting to distrusting options under OTIN while females show nonsignificant variation between conditions.

Frequency of trust decisions made by gender and condition, n=43

The final social cognition ANCOVA was conducted on safety using the baseline measure, parent love withdrawal history (r= -.371, p=.014), and anxiety level (r= -.453, p=.002), and procedural order as covariates. There was a nonsignificant main effect of experimental condition on psychological safety F(1,40) =.061 , p = .806. No significant interaction effects were found.
Summary. Results largely support hypothesis two. Attitudinal trust, cooperation, and behavioral trust were directly and positively affected by administration of OT. Furthermore, baseline moderations support the trait nature of attitudinal trust and cooperation. Their interactions support the literature, demonstrating individual characteristics influence on OT’s effect. While pain did not show a main effect, there was a surprising moderation of procedural order that demonstrated OT’s analgesic capacity. Only the social cognition variable safety was unaffected. The identification of main effects was the first step toward establishing possible mediators.

Mediations (H4)

Hypothesis three examined social cognitive variables as possible mediators of the effect between OT and pain. Results of the ANCOVAs show evidence of OT’s effect on the social cognitive factors of attitudinal trust, behavioral trust and cooperation. OT may increase prosocial feelings, specifically cooperation and attitudinal trust, but there is insufficient evidence to claim a mediation effect. Increases in cooperation and trust were not associated with changes in pain levels as indicated by bivariate correlations. Multiple regressions show trust and cooperation scores were also not predictive of pain scores; the factors vary independently.
Furthermore, changes in trust game choice were not predictive of pain levels. OT did not affect safety and therefore cannot be considered. Overall, hypothesis three is not fully supported. The ANCOVAs demonstrate that OT can increase prosocial cognitions and reduce pain, but a link between these two effects could not be fully established. Although this does not rule out pain as a possible mediator because of design limitations regarding temporal order and predicted effects between social cognitive variables and pain. This issue is more fully addressed in the discussion.

Decision making & OT (H5)

Hypothesis five examined pain as a possible mediator between OT and decision-making, proposing that OT’s reduction in pain will influence decision outcomes. In order to indirectly assess pain as a mediator between OT and decision-making, main effects must be present. Two one-way repeated measures ANCOVA were conducted to determine if there is a statically significant difference between the placebo and OT conditions for the two decision-making variables, loss aversion and self-control. The first analysis examined mean differences in loss aversion while controlling for baseline and procedural order. A significant main effect was found F(1,37) =4.491, p = .041, Cohen’s d = .108. Main effect comparisons show loss aversion increases from placebo (M=.55) to drug condition (M=.860), i.e. participants under the influence of OT became more risk averse. No significant interaction effects were found.
The final decision-making ANCOVA compared OT effects on the composite variable self-control, an aggregate of the z-scores for the decision-making variables. Results show a main effect of condition on self-control F(1,39) =4.089, p = .05, np2 = .095. The group means are less interpretable due to the required transformation of the composite variable, however, the placebo condition scores demonstrate increased self-control compared to the drug condition. A review of scatterplots shows an interesting relationship between pain, OT, and self-control. Pain was predictive of self-control (r2= 149, p =.011) only in the OT condition (see Figure 3).
Pains scores across visit order of experimental condition across the decision-making composite score of self-control

Post Hoc Analysis

A series of between-subject’s analysis in the form of t-tests were performed. Means comparison for between gender and between intervention and placebo effects resulted in no significant differences across pain levels. T-tests were additionally run to assess the effects of OT on the primary research variables of trust, cooperation, and safety. Again, no significant differences were found.
Furthermore, the primary ANCOVAs were then rerun without controlling for procedural order. Behavioral trust and loss aversion showed a decreased effect size and significance, while attitudinal trust lost significance entirely. The baselines for cooperation and attitudinal trust also lost their significant interaction effects. The variable safety was unaffected. These findings provide evidence for the strong influence procedural order had on OT effects. Self-control was the only variable that lost significance when including procedural order from the analysis. A significant interaction effect was found with the self-control baseline suggesting a strong trait bias F(1,34) =7.418, p = .10. This suggests a direct effect of OT on decision-making since its effect was consistent between visits even when OT’s relationship with pain reverses. To increase the understanding of this relationship, the variables comprising self-control were also assessed. An ANCOVA was performed on delayed discounting and impulsivity using their respective baselines and procedural order as covariates. OT had a significant main effect on impulsivity F(1,40) =6.789, p = .013, np2 = .145. There was also a significant interaction with baseline F(1,39) =1.960, p = .170. Review of group means support the self-control finding showing elevated levels of impulsivity on OT. Delayed discounting did not have a significant main effect.

Exploratory genetics

Genetic markers were evaluated as possible predictors and covariates. To begin, a correlation matrix was reviewed for all baselines and controls. Results show no significant correlations between the six SNPs and any decision-making variables. No correlations were found between SNPs and pain levels. Significant correlations were found between the genetic markers and social cognitive variables. Behavioral trust scores are significantly associated with the Early Life Risk SLC6A4 rs25531 marker (r=.322 p=0.035). In addition, cooperation was significantly correlated with markers for pain sensitivity, impulsivity, and executive functioning.
Table 4. Genetic Marker Associations.
Table 4. Genetic Marker Associations.
Pearson Correlations
GMPain GMImpuls GMExFunc Cooperation
GMPain Pearson's r
p-value
GMImpuls Pearson's r -0.143
p-value 0.362
GMExFunc Pearson's r 0.681*** -0.064
p-value < .001 0.681
Cooperation Pearson's r -0.316* 0.314* -0.330*
p-value 0.039 0.040 0.031
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001, n = 43. Series of correlations between SNPs and cooperation
Next, correlations between markers and control variables were considered. One significant negative correlation was found between the Prosocial OTXR rs53576 (r=-0.308 p=0.047) and chronic pain. Regression analysis was conducted for each SNP with their theoretical indicators as measured in study questionnaires. No genetic markers significantly predictor their associated indicator.
For exploratory purposes, the hypothesis tests were rerun again, including genetic markers as covariates. SNPs were chosen based on their relevance supported in the literature. Two of the seven ANCOVAs were significantly affected. Cooperation had a marked increase in main effect size (np2 = 243) and had a new significant interaction with parent love withdrawal history when SNPs for prosocial disposition, early life risk sensitivity, and OT sensitivity are controlled F(1,35) =5.236, p = .028, np2 = .141. Lastly, the ANCOVA for loss aversion lost significance when additionally controlling for SNPs indicating OT sensitivity, impulsivity, and executive functioning. However, a new significant interaction occurred between loss aversion and the marker for impulsivity (SCL6A3 rs460000) controlled F(1,33) =4.416, p = .043.

2. DISCUSSION

The effect of OT can often appear paradoxical, as reflected in the literature. This study found similar conflicting findings, although as research continues, our understanding of these findings seems less perplexing. A primary notable finding was the reversal in pain perception between the first and second exposures, suggesting an adaptive response. During the first exposure, characterized by uncertainty and increased risk, OTIN resulted in heightened pain sensitivity, loss aversion, and delayed discounting, which could be evolutionarily advantageous. However, upon the second exposure, when uncertainty was reduced, OT increased pain tolerance, providing protective benefits once significant survival risk was ruled out. When considered alongside environmental and life history factors, the seemingly inconsistent influence of OT becomes more predictable. Our study revealed a significant effect of OT on pain perception, particularly in the context of repeated exposure to pain. This provides the first evidence for the need to examine the effects of OT over time and across repeated exposures with the same individual.
Moreover, the study demonstrated that acute pain increased state trust and cooperation, effects that may be hindered by conventional analgesics. Although the observed effects were moderate, the findings suggest that OT has the potential to sustain and strengthen prosocial attributes. Therefore, OT administration could enhance patient trust, cooperation with healthcare providers, and adherence to treatment regimens. A seemingly paradoxical relationship has also been identified, in which, trust decreases pain and pain increases trust. However, this may be resolved by considering the different types of trust, specifically, attitudinal/trait and state. The present study only examined the former. But, it may be that pain is lessoned by state trust, while pain increases attitudinal trust, as supported by the findings of this study. This trait/state distinction could account for the seemingly contradictory relationships between pain and social cognitions.
Lastly, exposure to OT increased risk aversion and impulsivity. This effect was consistent after controlling for procedural order. Examination of the latent variable of self-control also showed a decreased average in the experimental condition. Although, lower pain levels were associated with reduced decision-making biases overall and OT was associated with lower pain levels expected. However, there is evidence that OT also has a direct effect on decision-making beyond that of its effect on pain. A direct examination of the effect of OT on decision-making would lend insight into this relatively unexplored domain.

Limitations

For subject safety, the study employed the smallest effective dose of OT reported in the literature. However, future research should explore the effects of higher dosage levels. In addition, future research should be conducted to assess the possible range of effect sizes across individuals and contexts, and preferably with a larger sample size. While the primary analysis benefited from within-subjects testing, the post hoc exploration of between subject’s effects was likely underpowered. It remains unclear what the range of potential for OTs analgesic effect is in comparison to familiar analgesics, such as ibuprofen, for example. A RCT should be conducted to compare efficacy across a number of common pain alleviation treatments. Furthermore, it is crucial to validate life history variables and other risk assessment measures to identify patients who are less likely to benefit from integrative health approaches involving OT.
And while the mechanisms at play in the release and activation of OT provided a model of understanding for this research, these mechanisms were never directly tested with physiological measures. The OT research field would strongly benefit from more direct examinations of the neurological mechanisms and their interaction with OT to further clarify the neurocircuitry of the response. FMRI studies would help continue to tease apart the top-down and bottom-up modalities. Additionally, an appropriate next step would be to separate out the timing of the prosocial measures from the pain exposure. This allowed for the interaction to be examined, but also temporally cross-contaminated the outcomes.

Future Directions

To gain a clearer understanding of oxytocin's (OT) role in pain reduction, whether as an endogenous hormone or through therapeutic application, studies incorporating mediation and moderation analysis are essential. The extensive literature on the relationship between OT and prosocial behavior warns against making direct causal claims. For instance, exogenous OT has elicited paradoxical responses in some individuals, heightening antisocial behavior among those with aggressive traits or towards outgroup members [76,77,78,79,80]. Likewise, in certain scenarios, administering OT can intensify emotional and physical distancing based on individual familiarity and romantic relationship status [21,81]. Additionally, the dose-response effects of exogenous OT differ between males and females [78]. Incorporating pain, as observed in this study, introduces an added layer of complexity. Our research, which considered various situational and individual factors, revealed that OT manifested paradoxical effects on pain and prosocial behavior under specific conditions and among particular individuals. Models predicting the link between OT, pain perception, and management will undoubtedly become more refined as future research incorporates individual traits, situational contexts, gender, and life history as moderating elements. This will reduce the likelihood of unintended consequences in clinical settings.
An additional focus for upcoming research should be to leverage advancements in imaging technology to examine the neural pathways involved in OT and pain. This method could be particularly effective in clarifying the activation pathways to and from the amygdala, a central hub for both pain modulation and social cognition. The amygdala is a direct target for oxytocin through meso-cortical circuits and is also influenced by top-down projections (e.g., the fronto-amygdala pathway). Recent studies suggest that top-down and bottom-up OT projections activate distinct amygdala subregions [82]. An example of this is how OT can prompt either avoidance or approach behaviors based on specific activation pathways [83]. Furthermore, conditions like clinical depression may disrupt the fronto-amygdala circuitry, potentially diminishing OT's impact. Currently, these differential pathways haven't been explored in studies investigating exogenous effects on pain perception. However, delving into such areas could greatly enhance our comprehension of how OT influences pain perception in varying individuals and situations.

2. CONCLUSIONS

The most pronounced effect was seen in the distinct shift of OT’s effect between first and second exposure to pain. This reversal in pain perception appears to be adaptive, as seen in the decision-making variables. The first exposure involves uncertainty and, therefore, more risk, so increased pain, loss aversion, and delayed discounting would all be evolutionarily advantageous. However, upon the second exposure, when threat of uncertainty is relieved, the OT increases pain tolerance which can then be considered protective. This phenomenon could be thought of as the pain sensitivity-tolerance tradeoff. Acute pain was shown generally to increase state trust and cooperation as is consistent with the literature. However, this positive effect may be nullified by typical analgesics. While the discovered effects were moderate, the study’s findings suggest OT is able to maintain and even reinforce these prosocial attributes. Thus, OT could increase patient trust and cooperation with staff, as well as adherence to treatment regimens. An OT-based pharmaco-behavioral therapy could likely supplement traditional psychoeducation pain programs or cognitive-behavioral therapies and demonstrates potential as a titration aid for more problematic painkillers.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, X.T.W and P.L. and H.F.; Methodology, X.T.W., P.L., J.L.S., and H.F.; Validation, X.T.W., P.L., J.L.S., E.A.E., N.A.K., and H.F Formal Analysis, P.L., J.L.S., and H.F.; Investigation, P.L. and J.L.S.; Resources, X.T.W., E.A.E., and H.F.; Writing – Original Draft Preparation, P.L.; Writing – Review & Editing, P.L., J.L.S., E.A.E., N.A.K., and H.F.; Supervision, X.T.W. and H.F.; Project Administration, J.L.S. and H.F.; Funding Acquisition, P.L., X.T.W., and H.F. All authors contributed to the manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by a pilot grant from the Center for Brain and Behavior Research at the University of South Dakota.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available upon request to all interested researchers. The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors without undue reservation.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to recognize the research assistance of Logan Hale and the volunteers who participated in this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Campbell, A. Oxytocin and Human Social Behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review 2010, 14, 281-295. [CrossRef]
  2. MacDonald, K.; MacDonald, T.M. The Peptide That Binds. Harvard Review of Psychiatry 2010, 18, 1-21. [CrossRef]
  3. Uvnäs-Moberg, K.; Ekström-Bergström, A.; Berg, M.; Buckley, S.; Pajalic, Z.; Hadjigeorgiou, E.; Kotłowska, A.; Lengler, L.; Kielbratowska, B.; Leon-Larios, F.; et al. Maternal plasma levels of oxytocin during physiological childbirth - a systematic review with implications for uterine contractions and central actions of oxytocin. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2019, 19, 285-285. [CrossRef]
  4. Roels, R.; Rehman, U.S.; Carter, C.S.; Nazarloo, H.P.; Janssen, E. The link between oxytocin plasma levels and observed communication behaviors during sexual and nonsexual couple discussions: An exploratory study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2021, 129, 105265. [CrossRef]
  5. Walter, M.H.; Abele, H.; Plappert, C.F. The Role of Oxytocin and the Effect of Stress During Childbirth: Neurobiological Basics and Implications for Mother and Child. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 2021, 12, 742236-742236. [CrossRef]
  6. Carter, C.S. Sex, love and oxytocin: Two metaphors and a molecule. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2022, 143, 104948-104948. [CrossRef]
  7. Nersesyan, Y.; Demirkhanyan, L.; Cabezas-Bratesco, D.; Oakes, V.; Kusuda, R.; Dawson, T.; Sun, X.; Cao, C.; Cohen, A.M.; Chelluboina, B.; et al. Oxytocin Modulates Nociception as an Agonist of Pain-Sensing TRPV1. Cell Rep 2017, 21, 1681-1691. [CrossRef]
  8. Kingsbury, M.A.; Bilbo, S.D. The inflammatory event of birth: How oxytocin signaling may guide the development of the brain and gastrointestinal system. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 2019, 55, 100794. [CrossRef]
  9. Hilfiger, L.; Zhao, Q.; Kerspern, D.; Inquimbert, P.; Andry, V.; Goumon, Y.; Darbon, P.; Hibert, M.; Charlet, A. A Nonpeptide Oxytocin Receptor Agonist for a Durable Relief of Inflammatory Pain. Sci Rep 2020, 10, 3017-3017. [CrossRef]
  10. Nishimura, H.; Yoshimura, M.; Shimizu, M.; Sanada, K.; Sonoda, S.; Nishimura, K.; Baba, K.; Ikeda, N.; Motojima, Y.; Maruyama, T.; et al. Endogenous oxytocin exerts anti-nociceptive and anti-inflammatory effects in rats. Commun Biol 2022, 5, 907-907. [CrossRef]
  11. Boll, S.; Almeida de Minas, A.C.; Raftogianni, A.; Herpertz, S.C.; Grinevich, V. Oxytocin and Pain Perception: From Animal Models to Human Research. Neuroscience 2018, 387, 149-161. [CrossRef]
  12. Losin, E.A.R.; Anderson, S.R.; Wager, T.D. Feelings of Clinician-Patient Similarity and Trust Influence Pain: Evidence From Simulated Clinical Interactions. J Pain 2017, 18, 787-799. [CrossRef]
  13. Ito, E.; Shima, R.; Yoshioka, T. A novel role of oxytocin: Oxytocin-induced well-being in humans. Biophys Physicobiol 2019, 16, 132-139. [CrossRef]
  14. Lopes, S.; Osório, F.d.L. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on pain perception among human subjects: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Hormones and Behavior 2023, 147, 105282. [CrossRef]
  15. You, D.S.; Haney, R.; Albu, S.; Meagher, M.W. Generalized Pain Sensitization and Endogenous Oxytocin in Individuals With Symptoms of Migraine: A Cross-Sectional Study. Headache: The Journal of Head and Face Pain 2018, 58, 62-77. [CrossRef]
  16. Bartz, J.A.; Zaki, J.; Bolger, N.; Ochsner, K.N. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2011. [CrossRef]
  17. Kemp, A.H.; Guastella, A.J. The Role of Oxytocin in Human Affect. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2011, 20, 222-231. [CrossRef]
  18. Harari-Dahan, O.; Bernstein, A. A general approach-avoidance hypothesis of Oxytocin: Accounting for social and non-social effects of oxytocin. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2014, 47, 506-519. [CrossRef]
  19. Rash, J.A.; Aguirre-Camacho, A.; Campbell, T.S. Oxytocin and pain: a systematic review and synthesis of findings. The Clinical journal of pain 2014, 30, 453-462.
  20. Moberg, K.U.; Handlin, L.; Petersson, M. Neuroendocrine mechanisms involved in the physiological effects caused by skin-to-skin contact – With a particular focus on the oxytocinergic system. Infant Behavior and Development 2020, 61, 101482. [CrossRef]
  21. Freeman, H.; Scholl, J.L.; AnisAbdellatif, M.; Gnimpieba, E.; Forster, G.L.; Jacob, S. I only have eyes for you: Oxytocin administration supports romantic attachment formation through diminished interest in close others and strangers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2021, 134, 105415. [CrossRef]
  22. Fan, Y.; Herrera-Melendez, A.L.; Pestke, K.; Feeser, M.; Aust, S.; Otte, C.; Pruessner, J.C.; Böker, H.; Bajbouj, M.; Grimm, S. Early life stress modulates amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity: implications for oxytocin effects. Hum Brain Mapp 2014, 35, 5328-5339. [CrossRef]
  23. Yoon, S.; Kim, Y.-K. Possible oxytocin-related biomarkers in anxiety and mood disorders. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 2022, 116, 110531. [CrossRef]
  24. Kreuder, A.-K.; Wassermann, L.; Wollseifer, M.; Ditzen, B.; Eckstein, M.; Stoffel-Wagner, B.; Hennig, J.; Hurlemann, R.; Scheele, D. Oxytocin enhances the pain-relieving effects of social support in romantic couples. Hum Brain Mapp 2019, 40, 242-251. [CrossRef]
  25. van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Huffmeijer, R.; Alink, L.R.A.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Tops, M. The Impact of Oxytocin Administration on Charitable Donating is Moderated by Experiences of Parental Love-Withdrawal. Front Psychol 2011, 2, 258-258. [CrossRef]
  26. McQuaid, R.J.; McInnis, O.A.; Stead, J.D.; Matheson, K.; Anisman, H. A paradoxical association of an oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism: early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Frontiers in neuroscience 2013, 7, 128. [CrossRef]
  27. Feng, C.; Lori, A.; Waldman, I.D.; Binder, E.B.; Haroon, E.; Rilling, J.K. A common oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism modulates intranasal oxytocin effects on the neural response to social cooperation in humans. Genes, Brain and Behavior 2015, 14, 516-525. [CrossRef]
  28. Kosaka, H.; Okamoto, Y.; Munesue, T.; Yamasue, H.; Inohara, K.; Fujioka, T.; Anme, T.; Orisaka, M.; Ishitobi, M.; Jung, M. Oxytocin efficacy is modulated by dosage and oxytocin receptor genotype in young adults with high-functioning autism: a 24-week randomized clinical trial. Translational psychiatry 2016, 6, e872-e872. [CrossRef]
  29. Wu, N.; Li, Z.; Su, Y. The association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. Journal of affective disorders 2012, 138, 468-472. [CrossRef]
  30. Feldman, R.; Zagoory-Sharon, O.; Weisman, O.; Schneiderman, I.; Gordon, I.; Maoz, R.; Shalev, I.; Ebstein, R.P. Sensitive parenting is associated with plasma oxytocin and polymorphisms in the OXTR and CD38 genes. Biological psychiatry 2012, 72, 175-181. [CrossRef]
  31. Wang, C.; Gao, J.; Ma, Y.; Zhu, C.; Dong, X.W. Physical pain increases interpersonal trust in females. European Journal of Pain 2017, 22, 150-160. [CrossRef]
  32. Hamel, A.; Bastien, C.; Jacques, C.; Moreau, A.; Giroux, I. Sleep or Play Online Poker?: Gambling Behaviors and Tilt Symptoms While Sleep Deprived. Front Psychiatry 2021, 11, 600092-600092. [CrossRef]
  33. Apkarian, V.A.; Sosa, Y.; Krauss, B.R.; Thomas, S.P.; Fredrickson, B.E.; Levy, R.E.; Harden, N.R.; Chialvo, D.R. Chronic pain patients are impaired on an emotional decision-making task. Pain 2004, 108, 129-136. [CrossRef]
  34. Timm, A.; Schmidt-Wilcke, T.; Blenk, S.; Studer, B. Altered social decision making in patients with chronic pain. Psychol Med 2023, 53, 2466-2475. [CrossRef]
  35. Woolf, C.J.; Ma, Q. Nociceptors—Noxious Stimulus Detectors. Neuron 2007, 55, 353-364. [CrossRef]
  36. Pontén, M.; Fust, J.; D'Onofrio, P.; Dorp, R.v.; Sunnergård, L.; Ingre, M.; Axelsson, J.; Jensen, K. The pain alarm response - an example of how conscious awareness shapes pain perception. Sci Rep 2019, 9, 12478-12478. [CrossRef]
  37. Carson, D.S.; Guastella, A.J.; Taylor, E.R.; McGregor, I.S. A brief history of oxytocin and its role in modulating psychostimulant effects. Journal of psychopharmacology 2013, 27, 231-247. [CrossRef]
  38. Wilson, A.D.; Golonka, S. Embodied Cognition is Not What you Think it is. Front Psychol 2013, 4, 58-58. [CrossRef]
  39. Wang, C.; Gao, J.; Ma, Y.; Zhu, C.; Dong, X.W. Physical pain increases interpersonal trust in females. European Journal of Pain 2018, 22, 150-160. [CrossRef]
  40. Frith, C.D.; Singer, T. The role of social cognition in decision making. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 2008, 363, 3875-3886.
  41. Downey, H.; Haynes, J.M.; Johnson, H.M.; Odum, A.L. Deprivation Has Inconsistent Effects on Delay Discounting: A Review. Front Behav Neurosci 2022, 16, 787322-787322. [CrossRef]
  42. Wallace, L.S.; Wexler, R.K.; McDougle, L.; Miser, W.F.; Haddox, J.D. Voices that may not otherwise be heard: a qualitative exploration into the perspectives of primary care patients living with chronic pain. J Pain Res 2014, 7, 291-299. [CrossRef]
  43. Walteros, C.; Sánchez-Navarro, J.P.; Muñoz, M.A.; Martínez-Selva, J.M.; Chialvo, D.; Montoya, P. Altered associative learning and emotional decision making in fibromyalgia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 2011, 70, 294-301. [CrossRef]
  44. Berger, S.E.; Baria, A.T.; Baliki, M.N.; Mansour, A.; Herrmann, K.M.; Torbey, S.; Huang, L.; Parks, E.L.; Schnizter, T.J.; Apkarian, A.V. Risky monetary behavior in chronic back pain is associated with altered modular connectivity of the nucleus accumbens. BMC Research Notes 2014, 7. [CrossRef]
  45. Hess, L.E.; Haimovici, A.; Muñoz, M.A.; Montoya, P. Beyond pain: modeling decision-making deficits in chronic pain. Front Behav Neurosci 2014, 8, 263-263. [CrossRef]
  46. Axelrod, R. Effective Choice in the Prisoner's Dilemma. Journal of Conflict Resolution 1980, 24, 3-25. [CrossRef]
  47. Berg, J.; Dickhaut, J.; McCabe, K. Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History. Games and Economic Behavior 1995, 10, 122-142. [CrossRef]
  48. Gächter, S.; Herrmann, B.; Thöni, C. Trust, voluntary cooperation, and socio-economic background: survey and experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 2004, 55, 505-531. [CrossRef]
  49. Kosfeld, M.; Heinrichs, M.; Zak, P.J.; Fischbacher, U.; Fehr, E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 2005, 435, 673-676. [CrossRef]
  50. Gereke, J.; Schaub, M.; Baldassarri, D. Ethnic diversity, poverty and social trust in Germany: Evidence from a behavioral measure of trust. PLoS One 2018, 13, e0199834-e0199834. [CrossRef]
  51. Kleinert, T.; Schiller, B.; Fischbacher, U.; Grigutsch, L.-A.; Koranyi, N.; Rothermund, K.; Heinrichs, M. The Trust Game for Couples (TGC): A new standardized paradigm to assess trust in romantic relationships. PLoS One 2020, 15, e0230776-e0230776. [CrossRef]
  52. Eckel, C.C.; Wilson, R.K. Is trust a risky decision? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 2004, 55, 447-465. [CrossRef]
  53. Mischkowski, D.; Crocker, J.; Way, B.M. From painkiller to empathy killer: acetaminophen (paracetamol) reduces empathy for pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2016, 11, 1345-1353. [CrossRef]
  54. Zunhammer, M.; Geis, S.; Busch, V.; Greenlee, M.W.; Eichhammer, P. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on thermal pain in healthy men: a randomized functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychosomatic medicine 2015, 77, 156-166.
  55. Patton, J.H.; Stanford, M.S.; Barratt, E.S. Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology 1995, 51, 768-774. [CrossRef]
  56. Edmondson, A. Psychological Safety and Learning Behavior in Work Teams. Administrative Science Quarterly 1999, 44, 350-383. [CrossRef]
  57. Rotter, J.B. A new scale for the measurement of interpersonal trust1. Journal of Personality 1967, 35, 651-665. [CrossRef]
  58. Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica 1979, 47, 263. [CrossRef]
  59. Kirby, K.N.; Maraković, N.N. Modeling Myopic Decisions: Evidence for Hyperbolic Delay-Discounting within Subjects and Amounts. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 1995, 64, 22-30. [CrossRef]
  60. Stanford, M.S.; Mathias, C.W.; Dougherty, D.M.; Lake, S.L.; Anderson, N.E.; Patton, J.H. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: An update and review. Personality and Individual Differences 2009, 47, 385-395. [CrossRef]
  61. Schlenker, B.R.; Helm, B.; Tedeschi, J.T. The effects of personality and situational variables on behavioral trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1973, 25, 419-427. [CrossRef]
  62. Tucker, A.W. The Mathematics of Tucker: A Sampler. The Two-Year College Mathematics Journal 1983, 14, 228. [CrossRef]
  63. Galbally, M.; Lewis, A.J.; van Ijzendoorn, M.; Permezel, M. The Role of Oxytocin in Mother-Infant Relations: A Systematic Review of Human Studies. Harvard Review of Psychiatry 2011, 19, 1-14. [CrossRef]
  64. Miller, S.D.; Duncan, B.; Brown, J.; Sparks, J.; Claud, D. The outcome rating scale: A preliminary study of the reliability, validity, and feasibility of a brief visual analog measure. Journal of brief Therapy 2003, 2, 91-100.
  65. Fydrich, T.; Dowdall, D.; Chambless, D.L. Reliability and validity of the beck anxiety inventory. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 1992, 6, 55-61. [CrossRef]
  66. Von Korff, M.; Ormel, J.; Keefe, F.J.; Dworkin, S.F. Grading the severity of chronic pain. Pain 1992, 50, 133-149. [CrossRef]
  67. Dixon, D.; Pollard, B.; Johnston, M. What does the chronic pain grade questionnaire measure? Pain 2007, 130, 249-253. [CrossRef]
  68. Wolf, S.; Hardy, J.D. Studies on pain. Observations on pain due to local cooling and on factors involved in the “cold pressor” effect. The Journal of clinical investigation 1941, 20, 521-533.
  69. Crockford, C.; Wittig, R.M.; Langergraber, K.; Ziegler, T.E.; Zuberbühler, K.; Deschner, T. Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc Biol Sci 2013, 280, 20122765-20122765. [CrossRef]
  70. Finkenwirth, C.; van Schaik, C.; Ziegler, T.E.; Burkart, J.M. Strongly bonded family members in common marmosets show synchronized fluctuations in oxytocin. Physiol Behav 2015, 151, 246-251. [CrossRef]
  71. Reyes, T.L.; Galinsky, A.M.; Hoffmann, J.N.; You, H.M.; Ziegler, T.E.; McClintock, M.K. Social peptides: measuring urinary oxytocin and vasopressin in a home field study of older adults at risk for dehydration. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2014, 69 Suppl 2, S229-S237. [CrossRef]
  72. Seltzer, L.J.; Ziegler, T.E.; Pollak, S.D. Social vocalizations can release oxytocin in humans. Proc Biol Sci 2010, 277, 2661-2666. [CrossRef]
  73. Seltzer, L.J.; Ziegler, T.; Connolly, M.J.; Prososki, A.R.; Pollak, S.D. Stress-induced elevation of oxytocin in maltreated children: evolution, neurodevelopment, and social behavior. Child Dev 2014, 85, 501-512. [CrossRef]
  74. Ziegler, T.E. Measuring peripheral oxytocin and vasopressin in nonhuman primates. American Journal of Primatology 2018, 80. [CrossRef]
  75. Ziegler, T.E.; Scheffler, G.; Snowdon, C.T. The Relationship of Cortisol Levels to Social Environment and Reproductive Functioning in Female Cotton-Top Tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Hormones and Behavior 1995, 29, 407-424. [CrossRef]
  76. De Dreu, C.K.W.; Greer, L.L.; Van Kleef, G.A.; Shalvi, S.; Handgraaf, M.J.J. Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2011, 108, 1262-1266. [CrossRef]
  77. DeWall, C.N.; Gillath, O.; Pressman, S.D.; Black, L.L.; Bartz, J.A.; Moskovitz, J.; Stetler, D.A. When the Love Hormone Leads to Violence. Social Psychological and Personality Science 2014, 5, 691-697. [CrossRef]
  78. Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Domes, G.; Kirsch, P.; Heinrichs, M. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2011, 12, 524-538. [CrossRef]
  79. Yong, E. Dark side of the love hormone. New Scientist 2012, 213, 39-41. [CrossRef]
  80. Zhang, H.; Gross, J.; De Dreu, C.; Ma, Y. Oxytocin promotes coordinated out-group attack during intergroup conflict in humans. Elife 2019, 8, e40698. [CrossRef]
  81. Scheele, D.; Striepens, N.; Güntürkün, O.; Deutschländer, S.; Maier, W.; Kendrick, K.M.; Hurlemann, R. Oxytocin modulates social distance between males and females. J Neurosci 2012, 32, 16074-16079. [CrossRef]
  82. Eckstein, M.; Markett, S.; Kendrick, K.M.; Ditzen, B.; Liu, F.; Hurlemann, R.; Becker, B. Oxytocin differentially alters resting state functional connectivity between amygdala subregions and emotional control networks: Inverse correlation with depressive traits. NeuroImage 2017, 149, 458-467. [CrossRef]
  83. Steinman, M.Q.; Duque-Wilckens, N.; Trainor, B.C. Complementary Neural Circuits for Divergent Effects of Oxytocin: Social Approach Versus Social Anxiety. Biological psychiatry 2019, 85, 792-801. [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Study Plan Experimental concept design.
Figure 1. Study Plan Experimental concept design.
Preprints 90212 g001
Figure 2. Pain by Procedural Order.
Figure 2. Pain by Procedural Order.
Preprints 90212 g002
Figure 3. Pain Score by Self-Control under OT Condition.
Figure 3. Pain Score by Self-Control under OT Condition.
Preprints 90212 g003
Table 1. Descriptives.
Table 1. Descriptives.
Min Max Mean SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12.
Control Descriptives
1. = Impulsivity 1.54 3.02 2.17 .37
2. = Chronic Pain 1.00 5.29 2.45 1.09 .06
3. = Anxiety .00 44.00 15.07 11.99 .48** .15
4. = Expectations 1.50 5.00 3.64 .62 -.28** .22 -.31*
5. = Parent History 1.14 4.00 2.03 .68 .30** -.10 .48** -.35*
Baseline Descriptives
6. = Cooperation 44.62 90.11 61.71 10.87 -.16 .13 -.13 .16 -.19*
7. = Safety 15.00 29.00 23.04 3.67 -.32** .18* -.27** .17* -.17 -.01
8. = Trust 1.92 3.28 2.54 .35 -.19* -.03 -.06 -.003 -.08 .01 .14
9. = Delayed Discounting .00 .03 .02 .01 .08 .10 .09 .08 .05 .08 .02 -.08
10. = Loss Aversion .00 2.20 .77 .84 .07 -.08 .15 -.09 .09 -.03 -.11 .03 .11
Treatment
11. = Oxytocin 1.60 9.60 5.23 1.67 .22 .08 -.11 .38* -.24 -.12 .12 -.21 .10 .15
12. = Placebo 1.00 9.80 5.24 1.91 .358 .06 -.01 .21 -.21 .03 .23 .12 .03 -.06 .62**
*p > .05; **p > .01; N = 43. Descriptives statistics for the key variables. Correlations reported as Pearson’s r.
Table 2. Repeated Measures ANCOVA Cooperation.
Table 2. Repeated Measures ANCOVA Cooperation.
Within Subjects Effects
SS df MS F p
Cooperation 286.282 1 286.282 6.124 0.018
Cooperation ✻ Baseline 180.278 1 180.278 3.856 0.053
Cooperation ✻ Parent-Love History 161.369 1 161.369 3.452 0.071
Cooperation ✻ Procedural Order 18.525 1 18.525 .390 0.535
Residual 1869.896 40 46.747
Table 3. Trust Behavior by Sex.
Table 3. Trust Behavior by Sex.
Count Condition
OT PB
Males Trust 9 13
Don’t Trust 6 2
Females Trust 24 23
Don’t Trust 4 5
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated