Preprint
Review

Language Skills in Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactiv-Ity Disorder and Specific Language Impairment: A Systematic Review

This version is not peer-reviewed.

Submitted:

13 November 2023

Posted:

13 November 2023

You are already at the latest version

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Abstract
(1) Background: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) are neurodevelopmental disorders that affect various areas of chil-dren’s development, such as language. There is an emerging idea that ADHD is characterised by the presence of language difficulties, which could be explained by the high comorbidity between ADHD and DLD. The purpose of this study is to analyse the empirical evidence of language com-petence in children aged 6–17 years old, diagnosed with ADHD and/or DLD. (2) Method: Fifteen studies with experimental designs were identified from Scopus, PsycINFO and WoS databases, reporting on language skills in children diagnosed with ADHD and/or DLD. Studies relating ex-ecutive functioning to language skills in this target population were also included. (3) Results: The literature is heterogeneous and different components of language are also examined. Even though the results are contradictory they convincingly demonstrate that there are overlapping symptoms between ADHD and DLD, such as language skills and executive functions. (4) Conclusions: the differences in the samples limit the generalisability of the results. Therefore, this review highlights the importance of considering language skills when designing individualised interventions for the population with ADHD and DLD, both in comorbidity and in isolation.
Keywords: 
Subject: 
Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Introduction

Although idiopathic difficulties in language development without overt intellectual disability have been recognized since the early 19th century [1], different terminology has been used and the term developmental language disorder (DLD) has come into use in recent years.
DLD affects approximately 7% of the population and its prevalence varies between studies and countries [2]. DLD has a high prevalence: it is 7 times more common than autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and 46 times more common than childhood hearing loss, yet DLD has been understudied [2,3,4].
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and DLD are two of the most common neurodevelopmental and comorbid disorders [5,6]. Children with comorbid DLD and ADHD may present with a range of symptoms that may overlap and affect different areas, such as language and executive function.
Symptoms of comorbid DLD and ADHD vary in severity and can affect academic and social functioning, increasing interest in understanding these conditions in order to provide appropriate support and intervention.

1.1. Language Skills

Language skills enable an individual to use language at a level of accuracy that conveys meaning in production and comprehension. These skills include both productive and receptive language skills, such as speaking, listening, reading and writing, and their effective use in a variety of practical contexts [7]. The five basic areas of language (phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics) are essential for understanding and using language effectively. They work together to form a dynamic, integrative whole that enables individuals to communicate and comprehend spoken and written language [7,8].
Language skills are essential for communication and are crucial for navigating personal and professional life. Limited language skills affect a range of areas, including behaviours, learning, future employment, social aspects and well-being [9].
Language development plays a crucial role in children's overall cognitive and social development. However, some children may face challenges in acquiring language skills, which can impact their academic performance and social interactions. Two common conditions that can affect language skills in children are ADHD and DLD [10].

1.2. Designations for Idiopathic Language Impairment

Idiopathic language impairment has been given a variety of names. Specific Language Impairment (SLI) was widely used in research studies to refer to the idiopathic subset of children with language disorders [11]. However, the term SLI was too restrictive, implying that the child had a relatively pure language problem with no other impairments. For this reason, the term SLI was rejected by the CATALISE panel in 2017. Instead of SLI, an alternative term, Developmental Language Disorder (DLD), was proposed by the international CATALISE consortium and endorsed for use when language impairment is not associated with a known biomedical aetiology [12].
The CATALISE consortium also agreed that (a) the presence of risk factors (neurobiological or environmental) does not preclude a diagnosis of DLD, (b) DLD may co-occur with other neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., ADHD), and (c) DLD does not require a discrepancy between verbal and non-verbal abilities [12].
Prior to the CATALISE consortium, the American Psychiatric Association (APA) published the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition [13], which established new guidelines for the diagnosis of language disorders. Instead of the term DLD, the DSM-5 manual includes the term 'language disorder' within neurodevelopmental disorders to define persistent difficulties in using language.
The difference in terminology is a source of instability in establishing criteria for idiopathic language disorders. Following the CATALISE consortium, the term DLD will be used in this study.
DLD, which affects approximately 7.5% of the world's population, is a very heterogeneous condition that can affect language production and/or comprehension with varying degrees of severity in different language domains (e.g., semantic, morphosyntactic, pragmatic) [14,15], which may explain the paucity of evidence regarding the accuracy and precision with which DLD is diagnosed in children [16].

1.3. ADHD

According to the Fifth Edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), ADHD is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders, affecting approximately 5-7% of the school population [13].
It is characterized by a persistent pattern of inattention, hyperactivity and/or impulsivity that interferes with functioning or development. Symptoms of ADHD can vary, and individuals may have a predominance of inattention, hyperactivity-impulsivity, or a combination of the two. These can cause several difficulties such as emotional, behavioural, psychiatric and cognitive problems, and its severity will depend on the time of detection [13].
Although language difficulties are not included as a core diagnostic criterion, children diagnosed with ADHD have large deficits in their language functioning across expressive, receptive and pragmatic language modalities, leading to general language problems [17,18].

1.4. Comorbidity

According to the DSM-5, both ADHD and Language Disorder (LD) are considered neurodevelopmental disorders. These disorders can cause various difficulties such as emotional, behavioural, psychiatric and cognitive problems, and their severity depends on the time of detection [13].
In addition to being two of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders, some empirical reports suggest that ADHD and DLD may also be common co-occurring conditions [5,6]. There is a complex relationship between DLD and ADHD, with overlapping and distinct features [4].
Scientific evidence supports the link between learning difficulties and executive functioning in neurodevelopmental disorders [19,20,21]. Children with comorbid DLD and ADHD may exhibit difficulties in executive functions, including working memory, cognitive flexibility, attention and inhibitory control [17,22]. In this regard, some studies have found that the co-occurrence of these conditions has further implications for children's educational trajectories [23,24]. In fact, children with DLD may have attention deficits that are not significant enough to warrant a diagnosis of ADHD, but still affect their learning in the classroom [25].
Besides executive deficits, language difficulties are another overlapping symptom in comorbid DLD and ADHD. DLD is a well-defined condition characterized by impairments in receptive and expressive language, as evidenced by difficulty following multi-step instructions, difficulty with coherent conversation and expression, and impairments in reading and writing [26,27]
Overall, language problems are also observed in children with ADHD, although it is unclear which component of language is most affected. Some studies show difficulties at the morphosyntactic level [17], while others show major deficits in language functioning in the expressive, receptive and pragmatic modalities of language [18,28].
Understanding the symptoms of comorbid DLD and ADHD, it is important to identify and address these difficulties to ensure appropriate diagnosis and treatment for affected children.

1.5. The Current Review

Research shows that children diagnosed with ADHD have language impairments [6,17,29]. In fact, ADHD and DLD are two of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders and are co-occurring disorders [5,6], which increases the interest in comparing them.
Given the high prevalence rates of both ADHD and DLD [30], it is important to understand how these conditions affect children’s language skills. This systematic review aims to examine the existing literature on language skills in children with ADHD and/or DLD. By synthesising findings from different studies, we aim to identify the specific language difficulties experienced by these children and to explore potential overlaps or differences between the two conditions.
Understanding the language profiles of children with ADHD and/or DLD can have significant implications for diagnosis, intervention, and support. By identifying the specific language challenges these children face, clinicians and educators can tailor interventions to address their unique needs. Additionally, this review will contribute to the existing literature by highlighting gaps in knowledge and suggesting avenues for future research.
In summary, this systematic review aims to provide a comprehensive overview of language skills in children with ADHD and/or DLD. By summarising the available evidence, we hope to improve our understanding of the language difficulties these children experience and inform effective strategies for assessment and intervention.

2. Materials and Methods

The current systematic review was performed using the PRISMA guidelines (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) [31]. The PRISMA guidelines provide a 27-item checklist which allows reviewers to report the relevant information and make an accurate analysis of all the studies found.

2.1. Search Strategy

The literature search was performed using three databases (Web of Science (WoS), PsycINFO and Scopus) in August 2023. The main objective was to analyse the empirical evidence of language competence in children and adolescents diagnosed with ADHD and/or DLD/SLI. The following key words were used: (“Specific Language Impairment” OR “Developmental Language Disorder” OR “DLD” or “SLI”) AND (“ADHD” OR “attention deficit hyperactivity disorder” OR “attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder” OR “attention deficit disorder”) AND (“language skills” OR “language” OR “language competence”) AND (“comorbid*” OR “co-occurring” OR “coexisting”).

2.3. Selection Criteria

The articles produced by the search were selected according to various inclusion/exclusion criteria, as follows.
Articles were included if they met the following criteria:
a) Empirical studies written in English or Spanish.
b) Included samples aged between 6 and 18 years.
c) Empirical studies that included children and adolescents diagnosed with ADHD and/or DLD/SLI.
d) Published in the last ten years (2013-2023).
e) Studies focusing on language skills and/or executive functioning in individuals diagnosed with ADHD and/or DLD/SLI.
On the other hand, articles were excluded if they met the following criteria:
a) Conference papers, reviews, doctoral theses, meta-analyses, case reports or book chapters.
b) Studies with a sample age range other than 6 to 18 years.
c) Articles on neurodevelopmental disorders other than DLD/SLI and/or ADHD, such as cognitive delay, deafness, autism spectrum disorders, neurological deficits or dyslexia.
d) Studies focusing on biological variables, biomarkers, genetic variables, mental health, behavioural problems, clinical trials; studies focusing on parental reports.

2.4. Study Selection

A total of 260 studies were found in the databases used (219 from Scopus, 23 from WoS and 18 from PsycINFO). Duplicates (25 results) and results from book chapters, doctoral theses or conference papers (14 results) were removed. In total, 195 studies were excluded by title (n = 173) or by abstract (n = 22). Finally, 15 studies were selected as meeting the inclusion criteria. The whole process is illustrated in the following flowchart (Figure 1) in accordance with the PRISMA declaration [31].

3. Results

We first describe the general characteristics of the 15 selected studies (Table 1). Two-fifths (40%, n=6) of the studies investigated the language skills of English speakers. The 15 studies were published in 12 different journals and, as Table 1 shows, the most studies were published in 2021.
Table 1 summarizes the demographic characteristics of the selected studies. The 15 studies report results from nine countries with sample sizes varying from 370 to 5 participants (Table 1). We selected studies with participants aged between 6 and 17 years, but in this case most studies (n =9) focused on childhood (7 - 12 years). Only 40% (n = 6) of the selected studies reported participants’ gender.
Just under half (46%, n = 7) of the articles studied populations with both a diagnosis of DLD and a diagnosis of ADHD. Only two of the fifteen studies included participants with comorbid DLD and ADHD, despite DLD and ADHD having high rates of comorbidity [30].
Ten of the selected articles compared the performance of different groups (ADHD group, DLD group, typically developing (TD) group) in different language tasks and the remaining 5 articles included measures of executive function in addition to language skills. There was a great deal of variability in the study of language skills. Some studies focused on narrative skills, while others focused on morphology, syntax, ... (Table 2).
There was variation in the use of instruments, but the selected studies were consistent in using instruments such as the Children's Communicative Checklist, Second Edition (CCC-2), the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-4) or the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III). In terms of executive function, the most frequent items measured were working memory and attention (Table 2).
The study of language skills involves the assessment of different language domains. In our selection of articles, we found studies that focused on examining language skills in a population diagnosed with DLD [31,35,37,39]. In these articles there was a great diversity as they focused on different language domains, notably syntax, semantics, and pragmatics. For example, Kagnovich and colleagues [37] measured general linguistic aptitude and working memory and the found that children with DLD had difficulties integrating visual information into long-term a-phonemic representations. However, it is important to note that none of the selected studies dealt with the study of phonology in isolation.
Brinton and colleagues [32] analysed the way in which five children with DLD related internal states when they spontaneously told a story from pictures and when they were asked directly about these states. Descriptions of the internal states of the story characters' emotions increased in response to prompts, but the children often produced emotion words that did not accurately reflect the content of the story.
It is well known that ADHD children have difficulties with language skills. In Vassiliu's study [41], the performance of the ADHD group demonstrated structural language difficulties, even though the ADHD group performed significantly higher than the DLD group. In the case of pragmatics, the ADHD group scored lower than any other group, but no statistical significance was found.
Similar results were found in Staikova's study [40], which investigated pragmatic language functioning in children with ADHD through different formal tests and questionnaires for parents. They found that children with ADHD had less developed skills in several aspects of pragmatic language compared to their typically developing peers, and that these problems were evident over and above general language functioning.
Zenaro and colleagues [42] compared the oral narrative skills of children with ADHD and children with TD. As in the studies mentioned above, they found that the ADHD group had lower levels of coherence than the TD group, with lower scores on the structural elements of theme/topic and outcome.
Narrative skills refer to the ability to tell a story or narrate an event in a coherent, organized way. The literature suggests that children with DLD may have difficulties with narrative skills due to their impaired language functioning, whereas children with ADHD may have better grammar, general core language skills, receptive and expressive language skills, but may still struggle with executive functions [10]. Narrative tasks engage not only language skills but also executive functions [43]. Stanford and Delage [6,17] examined syntactic and morphosyntactic aspects while measuring executive-function variables in French-speaking children. The EF and morphosyntactic profiles of children with ADHD and DLD are different [17]. Deficits in morphosyntax are not characteristic of ADHD, but performance in the ADHD group may mimic morphosyntactic impairment. This may explain the difference between the groups in the use of visual cues: children with DLD were more sensitive than children with ADHD to visual cues that may have required more attentional resources. In contrast, the DLD group were less sensitive to linguistic cues, as these cues required syntactic processing [6].
To determine which of the language areas is most affected in each of these disorders, and to understand the overlap of symptoms between the two, they need to be studied together. Our selection included six studies involving joint study of ADHD and DLD populations and a control group [6,17,29,32,36,41], while only two studies included samples with ADHD and DLD as comorbidity [22,34].
The results are contradictory. While Paredes-Cartes and Moreno-García [29] found significant differences in semantic and pragmatic language skills between the DLD and ADHD groups, indicating that children with DLD had more difficulties at the semantic level, the ADHD group had more difficulties at the pragmatic level. Helland and colleagues [36] aimed to investigate the linguistic profiles of children with DLD. Their results indicate that the DLD group was significantly more impaired than the ADHD group in the language and syntax scales. In the pragmatics scale, both groups exhibited equivalent difficulties, and in the semantic scale, they demonstrated the same level of impairment.
Conflicting results have also been found in articles examining populations with comorbid ADHD and DLD, particularly in relation to the symptom of hyperactivity and language skills. Redmond and colleagues [22] focused on morphosyntactic aspects in children with comorbid DLD and ADHD, typically developing children, and children with DLD. They found no significant differences between the DLD + ADHD and DLD groups. They also found that the severity of ADHD symptoms and sentence recall performance were moderately positively correlated. In contrast, El Sady and colleagues [34] concluded that hyperactivity was the most important factor affecting language in ADHD, as they found that children with DLD and comorbid ADHD had poorer language reception than children with DLD without ADHD.
Delgado and colleagues [33] compared the development of metapragmatic awareness in children with DLD, ADHD and a control group. They found that an age effect was evident for the ADHD group, but significant differences were found between the DLD and control groups, while children diagnosed with predominantly inattentive ADD were found to perform similarly to the control group.
Children with language impairment (LI) often perform below the level of typically developing peers in measures of both processing speed and working memory, and pro-cessing factors may contribute to the understanding of language disorders [44]. Ralli and colleagues [39] measured several EF variables such as verbal fluency and working memory, as well as measures of expressive vocabulary and sentence completion. Their results indicated that children with DLD were outperformed by their TD peers on measures of WM and verbal fluency (phonological and semantic).
Hyperactivity is also an important factor which affects language in ADHD [34]. However, O’Neil and colleagues [38] reported that there was no significant association between the severity of pre-schoolers' hyperactivity/impulsivity and their language skills at 4-6 years old.
The scientific research suggests that processing speed is related to different aspects of language ability, including syntactic development, language impairment, social and language delays, and vocabulary/word learning. [44,45].
Gooch and colleagues [35] conducted a longitudinal study in a British population to investigate the relationship between speed of processing (SOP), language and inattention/hyperactivity. The results showed that the DLD group had increased symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity commonly associated with ADHD. Symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity moderate the effect of SOP on language, but SOP does not predict later language progress.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the specific language difficulties experienced by children with DLD and/or ADHD and to explore potential overlaps or differences between the two conditions.
First, the review showed that the symptoms of these two disorders overlap, including language difficulties and deficits in EF.
There is a great deal of variability in language assessment research. Conflicting results are to be expected due to the complexity of conducting assessments in different language domains and because it is important to be aware of the variability of the tests used in our selection of studies.
Some studies focused on receptive language, others on expressive language. Most of the studies included in this review were concerned with examining grammar, semantics, and pragmatics, as these are the most impaired language areas in DLD and ADHD. However, the results are contradictory. Some research indicated that children with DLD have more morphological and semantic difficulties than children with ADHD [6,17,29]. While other research also identified morphological and semantic difficulties in children with ADHD [41].
The studies selected for the review also showed varied results in terms of the performance of children with ADHD in language tasks: studies in children with ADHD focusing on language use indicated difficulties in language use compared to the control group [40] although the differences between the groups were not always significant [29]. On the other hand, positive correlations were sometimes found between ADHD symptoms and sentence recall tasks [22].
The study of language skills is a complex field involving several different factors. For example, the assessment of pragmatic language is challenging. Firstly, the construct includes verbal and non-verbal skills, and secondly, as the definition of pragmatics is linked to social context, the assessment of pragmatic skills through standardised tests may not reflect a subject’s true ability to use language in natural settings [40]. In relation to narrative skills, structural elements are responsible for narrative coherence. These factors are important to build meaning for thematic maintenance and to present a coherent outcome related to the story problem situation that shows the relationship between the events being narrated [42]. They are also important in the ability to relate internal states, which involve not only linguistic deficits, but also the limitations of social and emotional knowledge [32].
Another important aspect is to consider the relationship between EF and language skills, which are closely related and interact in different ways [46]. In our selection of articles, only 5 included measures of EF in addition to language measures, which is particularly striking given that the type of participants included in the studies (DLD and ADHD) would be expected to have deficits in EF.
It is generally interesting to consider socio-demographic variables, such as gender or age, as these may have an impact on the performance of the proposed tasks. Gender stereotypes may affect the diagnosis of girls with DLD and/or ADHD. There are gender differences in ADHD, with boys more likely to be diagnosed than girls. Despite similar symptomatology, evidence-based sex and gender differences (in prevalence, presentation, symptom expression, progression, and impairment) are emerging. Girls with ADHD may experience greater functional impairment and have different long-term outcomes than boys with ADHD [47].
Gender also plays a role in DLD, with boys being at higher risk of the disorder and more likely to receive speech and language therapy services. A strong gender effect has been consistently reported, with higher prevalence of language delay and DLD in males than in females [48,49]. All of this shows how important it is to consider gender.
Finally, the age of the sample needs to be considered, because it can influence children's language development. Children's language skills continue to develop throughout childhood, and gestational age at birth can also influence language development. In fact, children develop many of the oral language skills that help them to learn to read when they go to school, and their language skills continue to develop throughout childhood [50].
The 'age effect' also needs to be considered in the field of language impairment, especially at an early age. One example is the development of metapragmatic awareness happens recursively, in that as children get older, they are not only able to identify the error between the context of the communicative act and the context of the communicative act and the linguistic expression, but also to explain it, thus achieving a reflexive control of language [33].
These results suggest the need for a standardized assessment tool that covers the different language domains, along with the importance of assessing pupils' language skills in a general way that could allow early identification of children at risk of these disorders.

5. Conclusions

This study has important implications for the field of language competences in DLD and/or ADHD. The results contribute to the understanding of language difficulties in these neurodevelopmental disorders by highlighting gaps in knowledge that may be addressed to improve the identification and intervention.
However, it is also important to note the limitations of the study. The use of different terminology when talking about idiopathic speech problems makes it difficult to identify the research carried out. Similarly, the number of studies that included participants with comorbidity between DLD and ADHD was very small, even though they are two disorders that often co-occur. Additionally, the small number of studies and methodological heterogeneity reduce the precision and generalizability of the findings. Hence, this review underscores the fact that further research is needed to clarify the relationship between language competence and these two neurodevelopmental disorders.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, I.M.F. and D.A.; methodology, D.A. and I.M.F.; investigation, I.M.F., C.R. and D.A.; resources, C.R. and D.A; writing—original draft preparation, I.M.F.; writing—review and editing, D.A.; supervision, D.A.; project administration, C.R.; funding acquisition, I.M.F and C.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by a SPANISH NATIONAL GOVERNMENT PROJECT (MCINN-22-PDC2022-133411-I00) and a pre-doctoral grant from the SEVERO OCHOA PROGRAM in the University of Oviedo (BP21-046).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Leonard, L.B. 200-year history of the study of childhood language disorders of unknown origin: Changes in terminology. Perspect ASHA Spec Interest Groups 2020, 5, 6–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. McGregor, K.K. How we fail children with developmental language disorder. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch, 2020; 51, 981–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bishop, D.V. Which neurodevelopmental disorders get researched and why? PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e15112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Redmond, S.M. Clinical intersections among idiopathic language disorder, social (pragmatic) communication disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2020, 63, 3263–3276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Redmond, S.M. Markers, models, and measurement error: Exploring the links between attention deficits and language impairments. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2016, 59, 62–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Stanford, E.; Delage, H. The contribution of visual and linguistic cues to the production of passives in ADHD and DLD: Evidence from thematic priming. Clin Linguist Phon 2021, 37, 17–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Chiswick, B. R., Miller, P. W. Language Skill Definition: A Study of Legalized Aliens. Int Migr Rev 1998, 32, 877–900. [CrossRef]
  8. Koutsoftas, A.D. School–age language development: Application of the five domains of language across four modalities. N. Capone-Singleton and BB Shulman (Eds.), Language development: Foundations, processes, and clinical applications, 2013; pp.,215-229.
  9. Riad, R., Allodi, MW, Siljehag, E., Bölte, S. Language skills and well-being in early childhood education and care: A crosssectional exploration in a Swedish context. Front Educ 2023, 8, 963180. [CrossRef]
  10. Parks, K. M., Hannah, K. E., Moreau, C. N., Brainin, L., Joanisse, M. F. Language Abilities in Children and Adolescents with DLD and ADHD: A Scoping Review. J Commun Disord 2023, 106, 106381. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Bishop, D. V. Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained language problems. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2014, 49, 381–415. [CrossRef]
  12. Bishop, D. V., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., Adams, C. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1068–1080. [CrossRef]
  13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ahufinger, N., Guerra, E., Ferinu, L., Andreu, L. and Sanz-Torrent, M. Cross-situational statistical learning in children with developmental language disorder. Lang Cogn Neurosci 2021, 36, 1180–1200. [CrossRef]
  15. Sansavini, A.; Favilla, M.E.; Guasti, M.T.; Marini, A.; Millepiedi, S.; Di Martino, M.V.; Vecchi, S.; Battajon, N.; Bertolo, L.; Capirci, O. Developmental Language Disorder: Early Predictors, Age for the Diagnosis, and Diagnostic Tools. A Scoping Review. Brain Sci 2021, 11, 654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., Mckean, C., Mensah, F.K., Morgan, A., Goldfeld, S., Nicholson, J.M., Wake, M. Specific language impairment: A convenient label for whom? Int J Lang Commun Disord 2014, 49, 416–451. [CrossRef]
  17. Stanford, E., Delage, H. Executive functions and morphosyntax: Distinguishing DLD from ADHD in French-speaking children. Front Psychol 2020, 11, 551824. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Korrel, H., Mueller, K. L., Silk, T., Anderson, V., Sciberras, E. Research Review: Language problems in children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder–a systematic meta-analytic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017, 58, 640–654. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Crisci, G., Caviola, S., Cardillo, R., Mammarella, I. C. Executive functions in neurodevelopmental disorders: Comorbidity overlaps between attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and specific learning disorders. Front Human Neurosci 2021, 15, 594234. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. El Wafa, H. E. A., Ghobashy, S. A. E. L., Hamza, A. M. A comparative study of executive functions among children with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and those with learning disabilities. Middle East Curr Psychiatry 2020, 27, 1–9. [CrossRef]
  21. Morgan, P. L., Li, H., Farkas, G., Cook, M., Pun, W. H., Hillemeier, M. M. Executive functioning deficits increase kindergarten children's risk for reading and mathematics difficulties in first grade. Contemp Educ Psychol 2017, 50, 23–32. [CrossRef]
  22. Redmond, S. M., Ash, A. C., Hogan, T. P. Consequences of co-occurring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on children's language impairments. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 2015, 46, 68–80. [CrossRef]
  23. Morsanyi, K., van Bers, B. M., McCormack, T., McGourty, J. The prevalence of specific learning disorder in mathematics and comorbidity with other developmental disorders in primary school-age children. Br J Psychol 2018, 109, 917–940. [CrossRef]
  24. Rodríguez, C., Areces, D., García, T., Cueli, M., Gonzalez-Castro, P. Neurodevelopmental disorders: An innovative perspective via the response to intervention model. World J Psychiatr 2021, 11, 1017. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Nitin, R., Shaw, D. M., Rocha, D. B., Walters, C. E., Jr, Chabris, C. F., Camarata, S. M., Gordon, R. L., Below, J. E. Association of Developmental Language Disorder With Comorbid Developmental Conditions Using Algorithmic Phenotyping. JAMA Netw Open 2022, 5, e2248060. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Adlof, S.M. Promoting reading achievement in children with developmental language disorders: What can we learn from research on specific language impairment and dyslexia? J Speech Lang Hear Res 2020, 63, 3277–3292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Spanoudis, G. C., Papadopoulos, T. C., Spyrou, S. Specific language impairment and reading disability: Categorical distinction or continuum? J Learn Disabil 2019, 52, 3–14. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Hawkins, E., Gathercole, S., Astle, D., Calm Team, Holmes, J. Language problems and ADHD symptoms: How specific are the links? Brain Sci 2016, 6, 50. [CrossRef]
  29. Paredes-Cartes, P.; Moreno-Garcia, I. Linguistic competences at schools. Comparison of students with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, specific language impairment and typical development. Rev de Pedagog 2021, 79, 497–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Medici, D., Varela, M. M. S. Language and school performance in children with diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Rev Mex de Neurocienc 2017, 18, 89–99.
  31. Page, M.J; McKenzie, J.E; Bossuyt, P.M.; Bouton, I.; Hoffmann, T.C; Mulrow, C.D; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 2021; 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Brinton, B., Fujiki, M., Asai, N. The Ability of Five Children With Developmental Language Disorder to Describe Mental States in Stories. Commun Disord Q 2019, 40, 109–116. [CrossRef]
  33. Delgado, V. G., Herrera, D. P., Ruiz, F. B. Metapragmatic awareness in children between 7 and 12 years-old, with specific language disorder, attention deficit disorder and typical language development. Pragmalinguistica 2018, 26, 109–130. [CrossRef]
  34. El Sady, S. R., Nabeih, A. A. S., Mostafa, E. M. A., Sadek, A. A. Language impairment in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in preschool children. Egypt J Med Hum Genet 2013, 14, 383–389. [CrossRef]
  35. Gooch, D., Sears, C., Maydew, H., Vamvakas, G., Norbury, C. Does Inattention and Hyperactivity Moderate the Relation Between Speed of Processing and Language Skills? Child Dev 2019, 90, E565–E583. [CrossRef]
  36. Helland, W. A., Helland, T., Heimann, M. Language profiles and mental health problems in children with specific language impairment and children with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2014, 18, 226–235. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Kaganovich, N., Schumaker, J., Christ, S. Impaired audiovisual representation of phonemes in children with developmental language disorder. Brain Sci 2021, 11. [CrossRef]
  38. O’Neill, S., Thornton, V., Marks, D. J., Rajendran, K., Halperin, J. M. Early language mediates the relations between preschool inattention and school-age reading achievement. Neuropsychol 2016, 30, 398–404. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Ralli, A. M., Chrysochoou, E., Roussos, P., Diakogiorgi, K., Dimitropoulou, P., Filippatou, D. Executive function, working memory, and verbal fluency in relation to non-verbal intelligence in greek-speaking school-age children with developmental language disorder. Brain Sci, 2021; 11. [CrossRef]
  40. Staikova, E., Gomes, H., Tartter, V., McCabe, A., Halperin, J. M. Pragmatic deficits and social impairment in children with ADHD. J Chil Psychol Psychiatry 2013, 54, 1275–1283. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Vassiliu, C., Mouzaki, A., Antoniou, F., Ralli, A., Diamanti, V., Papaioannou, S., Katsos, N. Development of Structural and Pragmatic Language Skills in Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Commun Disord Q 2023, 44, 207–218. [CrossRef]
  42. Zenaro, M. P., Rossi, N. F., Souza, A. L. D. M. d., Giacheti, C. M. Oral narrative structure and coherence of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. CoDAS 2019, 31, 1–8.
  43. Marini, A., Piccolo, B., Taverna, L., Berginc, M., Ozbič, M. The complex relation between executive functions and language in preschoolers with Developmental Language Disorders. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020, 17, 1772:1–1177:14.
  44. Park, J., Miller, C. A., Mainela-Arnold, E. Processing speed measures as clinical markers for children with language impairment. J Speech, Lang Hear Res 2015, 58, 954–960. [CrossRef]
  45. Peter, M. S., Durrant, S., Jessop, A., Bidgood, A., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F. Does speed of processing or vocabulary size predict later language growth in toddlers? Cogn Psychol 2019, 115, 101238:1–101238. [CrossRef]
  46. Shokrkon, A., Nicoladis, E. The directionality of the relationship between executive functions and language skills: A literature review. Front Psychol, 2022; 13, 848696. [CrossRef]
  47. Slobodin, O., Davidovitch, M. Gender differences in objective and subjective measures of ADHD among clinic-referred children. Front Hum Neurosci 2019, 13, 441. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Rakhlin, N., Kornilov, S. A., Grigorenko, E. L. Gender and agreement processing in children with Developmental Language Disorder. J Child Lang 2014, 41, 241–274. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Chilosi, A. M., Brovedani, P., Cipriani, P., Casalini, C. Sex differences in early language delay and in developmental language disorder. J Neurosci 2023, 101, 654–667. [CrossRef]
  50. Vermeij, B. A., Wiefferink, C. H., Knoors, H. E. T., Scholte, R. H. Effects in language development of young children with language delay during early intervention. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2023, 103, 106326:1–106326:12. [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flowchart of the article selection process.
Figure 1. Flowchart of the article selection process.
Preprints 90352 g001
Table 1. General characteristics of selected studies.
Table 1. General characteristics of selected studies.
Author Year Journal Country Language
Brinton et al. [32] 2018 Communication Disorders Quarterly USA English
Delgado et al. [33] 2018 Pragmalingüística Chile Spanish
El Sady et al. [34] 2013 The Egyptian Journal of Medical Human Genetics Egypt Arabic
Gooch et al. [35] 2019 Child Development UK English
Helland et al. [36] 2014 Journal of Attention Disorders Norway Norwegian
Kaganovich et al. [37] 2021 Brain Sciences USA English
O'Neil et al. [38] 2016 Neuropsychology USA English
Paredes-Cartes y Moreno-Garcia [29] 2021 Revista Española de Pedagogía Spain Spanish
Ralli et al. [39] 2021 Brain Sciences Greece Greek
Redmond et al. [22] 2015 Language speech and hearing services in schools USA English
Staikova et al. [40] 2013 The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry USA English
Stanford y Delage [6] 2021 Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics Switzerland French
Stanford y Delage [17] 2020 Frontiers in Psychology Switzerland French
Vassiliu et al. [41] 2022 Communication Disorders Quarterly Greece Greek
Zenaro et al. [42] 2019 Communication Disorders, Audiology and Swallowing Brazil Portuguese
Table 2. Summary of studies selected (n=15).
Table 2. Summary of studies selected (n=15).
Author(s) Sample Comorbidity Condition Instrument(s) Measures Summary of Results
Brinton et al. (2018) [31] Age range: 5-10 years
Group(s):
n =5 DLD
Gender:
2 females/ 3 males
No Language skills:
Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument (ENNI)
Narrative skills: internal response expressions in spontaneous and prompted condition. Production and accuracy of internal plan
Spontaneous condition: Expressions were appropriate.
Prompted condition: high production but the accuracy decreased.
Production and accuracy of emotion words:
Spontaneous condition: children describe few emotions.
Prompted condition: greater variety of emotion words.
Delgado et al. (2018) [33] Age range: 7 -12 years
Group(s):
n = 6 DLD
n = 6 ADHD
n = 6 TD
No Language skills: Metapragmatic
Consciousness Assessment Test
Metapragmatic Awareness TD group had the highest results.DLD group had the worst outcome.
Age effect in the ADHD group
Significant differences between the DLD group and the TD group.
El Sady et al. (2013) [34] Age range: 3 – 6 years
Group(s):
n = 36 DLD + ADHD
n = 25 DLD
Gender:
9 females /27 males
9 females/ 16 males
Yes Language skills:
Language testing of Arabic speaking children
Receptive age
Expressive age
Semantic
Syntax and phonology
Pragmatics
Receptive age quotient
Expressive age quotient
Significant difference in the receptive age and the receptive age quotient: DLD + ADHD children had poorer reception than DLD + no ADHD children.
Hyperactivity was the most important factor affecting language in ADHD.
Gooch et al. (2019) [35] Age range: 5 -8 years
Group(s):
n = 129 DLD
n = 370 TD
No Language skills:
Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test (EOWPVT)
Receptive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWPVT)
School-Age Sentence Imitation Test-English 32 (SASIT-E32)
Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG)
Assessment of Comprehension and Expression (ACE)
Executive functions:
Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN)
Visual Search (Apples Task)
Coding (WPPSI-III)
Simple Reaction Time
VocabularyGrammar
Narrative recall and comprehension
Speed of processing:
Children with DLD score lower on Speed of Processing (SOP) than their TD peers.
The DLD group have elevated symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity commonly associated with ADHD.
Symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity moderate the effect of SOP on language, but SOP does not predict later language in formal schooling
Helland et al. (2014) [36] Age range: 6- 12 years
Group(s):
n = 19 DLD
n = 21 ADHD
n = 19 TD
Gender:
2 females/17 males3 females/ 21 males
2 females/17 males
No Language skills:
Children’s Communication Checklist–Second Edition (CCC-2)
Speech, syntax, semantics, coherence, inappropriate initiation, stereotyped language, use of context, nonverbal communication, social relations, interests. Communication impairments were as pronounced in the ADHD group as in the DLD group.
The ADHD group was as impaired as the DLD group on the scale measuring semantics. Language structure was more impaired in the DLD group.
The ADHD group was more impaired on the interest scale.
Kaganovich et al. (2021) [37] Age range: 7-13 years
Group(s):
n = 18 DLD
n = 18 TD
Gender:
5 females/13 males7 females/ 11 males
No Language skills:
Photographic Expressive Language Test—2nd Edition (SPELT-II)
Photographic Expressive Language Test—Preschool 2 (SPELT-P2)
Core Language Score (Concepts and Following Directions, Recalling Sentences, Formulated, Sentences, Word Structure and Word Classes-2 Total (WC-2,
9–12-year-olds only), and Word Definitions (WD, 13-year-olds only).
Executive Functions:
Verbal working memory and nonword repetition test.
Number Memory Forward.
Number Memory Reversed subtests of the Test of Auditory Processing.
Skills—3rd edition (TAPS-3).
General linguistic aptitude
Working memory
Children with TD, but not children with DLD, can incorporate visual information into long- term phonemic representations
O’Neil et al. (2016) [38] Age range: 4 – 8 years
Group(s):
n = 90 ADHD
n = 60 TD
No Language domain of the NEPSY.
Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, Second Edition (WIAT-II): Word Reading, Pseudoword, Decoding, Reading Comprehension, and Spelling subtest.
Vanderbilt Assessment Scale, Teacher Informant: Reading and Written Expression performance in school.
Language (NEPSY)
Reading
Writing
At 4-6 years of age, there was no significant
association between the severity of pre-schoolers’ hyperactivity/impulsivity and their language skills.
at 4-6 years.
At 8 years, language ability mediated the pathway from preschool inattention (but not hyperactivity/impulsivity).
Paredes-Cartes y Moreno-Garcia (2021) [29] Age range: 7 – 12 years
Group(s):
n = 47 DLD
n = 48 ADHD
n = 47 TD
Gender:
20 females/27 males
39 females/ 9 males
19 females/ 28 males
No Language skills: The Objective and Criterion-referenced Language Suite (BLOC)
The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III)
Language: morphology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics.
Receptive vocabulary
Significant differences were found between the three groups in semantic and pragmatic language skills.
Children with ADHD had fewer problems with semantic competence than children with DLD.
Pragmatic competence: ADHD group had lower scores than the DLD group and the control group.
Ralli et al. (2021) [39] Age range: 8 yearsGroup(s):
n = 29 DLD
n = 29 TD
Gender:
14 females/ 15 males
17 females/ 12 males
No Language skills:
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-WISC III: vocabulary scale.
Athena Test: the sentence completion subtest.
Executive functions:
- Working Memory Test Battery for Children.
- n-back task.
- Flanker task.
- Borella’s task.
- “How many—What number task”
- Semantic fluency test.
Phonological fluency test.
Expressive vocabulary
Sentence completion
Working memory
Updating
Inhibition
Switching
Verbal fluency
Phonological fluency
Children with DLD were outperformed by their TD peers on measures of WM capacity, updating, monitoring (mixing cost) and verbal fluency (phonological and semantic).
Redmond et al. (2015) [22] Age range: 7 – 9 years
Group(s):
n = 19 DLD
n = 19 ADHD+DLD
n = 19 TD
Gender:
10 females / 9 males
Yes Language skills:
English Nonword Repetition Task.
Test of Early Grammatical
Impairment (TEGI).
Nonword repetition
Sentence recall
Tense marking
No significant differences were found between the ADHD and DLD + ADHD groups.
A modest positive correlation was found between the severity of
ADHD symptoms and their sentence recall performance: children with higher levels of ADHD symptoms performed better than those with lower levels.
Staikova et al. (2013) [40] Age range: 7 – 11 years
Group(s):
n = 28 ADHDn = 35 TD
No Language skills: Children's Communicative Checklist, Second Edition (CCC-2).
Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language (CASL).
Test of Pragmatic Language, Second Edition (TOPL-2)
Narrative Assessment Profile: Discourse Analysis (NAP)
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4)
Pragmatic language
General language
Children with ADHD had poorer pragmatic language skills relative to peers across all measures, even after controlling for general language abilities.
Stanford and Delage (2021) [6] Age range: 8 years
Group(s):
n = 20 DLD
n = 20 ADHD
n = TD
No Language skills:
Bilan Informatisé du Langage Oral (BILO)
Phonological loop.
Executive functions:
Conners CBRS: inattention, hyperactivity.
Syntax
selective attention, central executive, processing speed, attentional flexibility.
Children with DLD were more sensitive than children with ADHD to visual cues (DLD > ADHD), which were more implicit than the linguistic cues and may have required more attentional resources.
For linguistic cues, which required syntactic processing, the opposite pattern was true (ADHD > DLD).
Stanford and Delage (2020) [17] Age range: 8 yearsGroup(s):
n = 20 DLD
n = 20 ADHD
n = 20 TD
No Language skills:
Bilan Informatisé du Langage Oral (BILO).
Probe test.
Executive functions:
Sky Search task (TEA-ch)Digit recall task (WISC-IV)
Opposite Worlds task (TEA-ch)
MorphosyntaxSelective attention
Working memory
Attention shifting
Different EF and morphosyntactic profiles in children with ADHD and DLD.
ADHD group: higher-order EF weakness and difficulty with the omnibus morphosyntax task.
DLD group: lower- and higher-order limitations and struggled with both morphosyntax tasks.
Deficits in morphosyntax are not characteristic of ADHD: their performance can mimic morphosyntactic impairment.
Vassiliu et al. (2022) [41] Age range: 4 – 8 yearsGroup(s):
n = 25 DLD
n =29 ADHD
n = 29 TD
Gender:
8 females /17 males
11 females / 18 males
10 females / 19 males
No Language skills:
Logometro tasks
Structural language
Vocabulary: receptive and expressive
Morphosyntax: expressive and receptivePragmatic language
ADHD children face difficulties with language skills and especially with structural language: they performed significantly lower than their TD peers but significantly higher than the DLD group.
In pragmatics, ADHD children performed numerically lower than any other group but no statistical significance was found.
Zenaro et al. (2019) [42] Age range: 6 – 10 years
Group(s):
n = 20 ADHD
n = 20 TD
Gender:
6 females /14 males
No Language skills:
Frog, Where are you?
Narrative skills The ADHD group presented lower scores on the structural elements of “theme/ topic” and “outcome”
and a narrative with a lower degree of coherence than the TD group.
Note. DLD= Developmental Language Disorder; ADHD= Attentional Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; TD= Typical Development.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Alerts
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2025 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated