Odorant binding proteins (OBPs) have been described as essential molecules for scent recognition [
33]." OR "Odorant binding proteins (OBPs) have been described as some of the most important molecules for scent recognition [
33]. These proteins, after participating in the recognition of environmental compounds, bind to a transmembrane receptor, generating a stimulus in the form of a synapse that is interpreted in the brain; thus, it is understood that these proteins act as the first bridge between the environment and insects [
33]. Their role in behavioral responses has been evidenced in
Drosophila melanogaster Meigen mutants where OBP genes have proven to be responsible for ethanol perception being silenced, resulting in insects that are deficient in this protein, therefore showing abnormal behaviors with respect to aroma compared to wild insects [
34]. A similar case has been described in other insects, such as
Spodoptera litura F., where the gene coding for the pheromone binding proteins (PBP) SlitPBP1 and SlitPBP2 was silenced; thus, the insect showed lower behavioral responses. The mutant insects in this case were less attracted to the pheromone used as a stimulus, which was associated with a decrease in the perception and olfactory sensitivity to specific compounds perceived by the PBPs [
35]. We have previously demonstrated that the presence of DWV-A at high loads in the antennae generated a reduction in olfactory perception evaluated in EAG assays, and a reduction in the expression of the genes Amobp5 and Amobp11 coding for OBP5 and OBP11 proteins [
22]. Additionally, it has been studied that these genes are significantly expressed in antennae of 10- and 15-day-old bees [
7,
8]. These results have allowed linking these proteins to nurse bee behavior, in which the insects are dedicated to the task of caring for larvae, considering that the OBP5 protein has a high affinity for compounds of the alarm pheromone of diseased larvae, including phenethyl acetate, phenylethanol and benzyl alcohol, with benzyl alcohol being the most competitive for the recognition site of the protein [
7]. Therefore, the decreases in olfactory preferences observed in our study may be partially explained by the gene knockdown of OBP5 and OBP11 proteins in the antennae, which are responsible for the recognition of benzyl alcohol. Thus, young honey bees inoculated with the DWV-A showed a reduction in their ability to detect compounds associated with these proteins, as a consequence of the decrease in the gene expressions coding for olfactory proteins. It is important to understand that insect behaviors are complex responses to environmental recognition; thus, honey bees depend on neural processes to perform these complex behaviors, including foraging, hive coordination and hygienic behavior [
36]. Therefore, alteration of the normal functioning of neural processes compromises insect behaviors in response to perceived stimuli in the environment [
37]. Young honey bees artificially inoculated with DWV have been shown to exhibit learning and memory impairment [
37]. In addition, Morfin et al. [
24] demonstrated that co-infection of the ectoparasite
V. destructor and deformed wing virus (DWV) decreases memory and learning ability in PER assays 48 h post viral inoculation; they also observed that bees with high viral loads, co-infected with
V. destructor decreased the gene expressions associated with pre- and postsynaptic proteins AmNrx-1 (neurexin) and AmNlg-1 (neuroligin). Thus, they associated the decrease in these genes with the loss and reduction in the learning capacity of bees with high viral loads that are also parasitized with
V. destructor. Nevertheless, our study demonstrates that the increase in the viral load in the insect brain in the absence of the
V. destructor mite causes decreases in these pre- and post-synaptic gene expressions. This is one of the few studies to date that evidenced this consequence with only the presence of high viral loads.
Shah et al. [
23] reported that DWV alters the cell structure of critical regions of the brain, including the neuropils responsible for vision and olfaction, and is also capable of dividing different sections of the sensory perception lobes, which could compromise physiological functions provided that this pathogen alters the expression of different genes. Therefore, there is evidence that DWV affects the capabilities and physiological functioning of the sensory organs of bees [
21,
22,
38], which could explain why this alteration in behavioral responses was observed in our study. Furthermore, the logistic regression analysis showed a direct relationship between the viral load detected in the body (
Table S1 and S2, Supplementary Material) and the probability of preference to specific aromas by young honey bees (
Figure 2). This study indicated that a high level of DWV-A in bees reduced the probability that these bees choose the pheromone compound. Nevertheless, the reduction in the behavioral responses of these bees may also be the consequence of multiple altered factors resulting from the presence of the DWV, suggesting that an alteration in the sensory system, specifically the decrease of sensory perception in the antennae and alterations in the processes of interpretation of synaptic signals, specifically in the genetic alteration of neuronal genes, could lead these insects to present altered and reduced behaviors. Though their behaviors were not totally deficient because we observed behavioral responses in both treatments, in bees that were infected and non-infected with DWV-A.
The decrease in the behavioral response of DWV-A infected bees could be related to the observed decrease in OBPs and synaptic genes. The decline in the response of healthy 20-day-old bees exposed to the pheromone and essential oil component could be explained, in part, by the age-related distribution of tasks within the hive, known as temporal polyethism [
39,
40]. The existence of four castes within the hive was proposed in 1986, including cell cleaning (1 to 3 days old), care and maintenance of the larvae by nurse bees (4 to 12 days old), food processing and nest maintenance, which were known as middle-aged bees (13 to 20 days old) and foraging (more than 21 days old) [
41]. Johnson [
42] subsequently confirmed the existence of the four castes within the hive, indicating that as the bees matured their tasks became more limited, thus the 4-12-day-old bees focused solely on brood care tasks, while the 12-20-day-old bees specialized in nectar processing and nest maintenance in preparation for foraging outside the hive. Therefore, it is logical to expect that bees between 15 and 20 days old (in this study) would be more attracted to plant-derived compounds associated with foraging, such as the essential oil of
M. piperita, than to the pheromone component that was more related to the larval scents. This could explain, in part, why the behavioral response and preference of inoculated and non-inoculated bees was the same at the end of the trial (20-day-old bees), since non-inoculated bees had a greater preference for the aroma of the plant-derived essential oil over the pheromone compound (
Table 3). Therefore, the low preference for the pheromone compound would be equal to the preference of the infected bees, and this behavior would be expected given the physiological development of the species itself and the castes within the hive, as described by Johnson [
42]. Nevertheless, in bees inoculated with DWV-A we did not observe a significant preference for the essential oil in 20-day-old bees. Therefore, we thought that the infection caused by this pathogen would have serious consequences on the physiological development of the insects, affecting the social specialization associated with temporal polyethism, which provides benefits at the group level due to the division of labor that permits increased productivity and reliable task performance. We demonstrated here that DWV-A significantly affects young bees (10-15 days-old) in the recognition or preference of a specific compound (benzyl alcohol) related to the tasks of nurse honey bees. Therefore, DWV-A infected nurse bees demonstrated a low perception of the compounds released by brood that are necessary to maintain their care, which indicates that this brood is poorly cared for, possibly leading to a delay in the development of the colony or its total decline. It is also still unknown whether DWV affects the preference or attraction for other compounds or pheromones within the colony or aromas present in the flowers that attract foraging honey bees. Therefore, at least two questions remain. Do DWV-A and/or other DWV variants significantly affect foraging honey bees and their ability to recognize aromas in the field? Can DWV cause disorientation in the field leading to a loss in foraging bees because they are unable to locate their colony of origin? Further studies are required to answers these questions.