Preprint
Article

Choosing Companions: Pet Adoption Preferences in Portugal – a Cross-Sectional Study

Altmetrics

Downloads

202

Views

173

Comments

0

Submitted:

05 January 2024

Posted:

08 January 2024

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
In the wake of Portugal's pivotal shift to animal welfare legislation in 2018, where the no-kill policy for population control came into effect, animal shelters have emerged as critical sanctuaries for stray or unwanted animals. Recognising the critical role that adopter preferences play in the successful placement of shelter animals and the pressing need for shelters to expedite adoptions to liberate valuable space to accommodate more animals in need of refuge, this study seeks to investigate the intricate matrix of factors guiding adopters' choices. A descriptive cross-sectional study was performed with a convenience sample of the Portuguese population to assess their preferences and opinions about pet adoption. An online questionnaire was completed by 415 participants from February to October 2021. Among participants selecting attributes for pet adoption, the top preferences were behaviour (52.8%), size (49.6%), vaccination and deworming status (44.3%), and age (36.1%). Conversely, breed (7.2%), sex (6.3%), and animal colour (5.1%) were the least prioritised. Combinations were established based on the most selected attributes. Certain participant characteristics, such as education level and pet ownership history, exhibited distinct preferences in animal attributes. Portuguese people show a marked preference for shelter adoption, prioritising behaviour, size, and health over breed and colour. This insight allows shelters to allocate resources effectively. While promising, the trend towards shelter adoption needs adaptive strategies to align with evolving adopter preferences.
Keywords: 
Subject: Public Health and Healthcare  -   Public Health and Health Services

1. Introduction

In Portugal, shelters have been playing a crucial role in caring for unwanted or stray animals since the practical implementation of the legal prohibition of culling for population control in 2018 [1]. These shelters provide temporary or permanent homes for these animals until they are adopted, find a permanent residence or, in some unfortunate cases, die or require euthanasia.
In the year 2022, a total of 41,994 animals were rescued from Portuguese territory and housed in shelters in addition to those already residing there. Among these sheltered animals, 24,721 found homes through adoption, while 2,378 had to be euthanised. This staggering influx of 14,985 animals in just one year underscores the important role that animal shelters play in the country [2]. Animal shelters are essential to animal welfare by providing temporary or permanent homes for needy animals. However, the residents of these facilities face numerous challenges, including the risk of contracting infections, developing negative behavioural traits, leading confined lives, and ultimately facing the possibility of death or euthanasia [3,4,5]. Recent research has shown that chronic stress can lead to ambilaterality (no paw preference) in sheltered dogs due to reduced functional cerebral asymmetry [6]. Consequently, shelters must grapple with the ongoing challenge of finding suitable adopters for their resident animals, as adopter preferences are crucial for animal shelters to consider as they significantly influence the adoption process and its success [7]. Numerous studies have delved into adopter preferences within animal shelters. While some studies have concentrated on the preferences of adopters regarding the animals they would adopt [7,8], much of the existing literature has focused on retrospective analyses [9,10,11] of animal characteristics and their ultimate fates, particularly those characteristics that seem to determine their adoption.
The significance of understanding adopter preferences in animal shelters is further underscored by the work of Weiss et al. (2012), who argue that matching the right animal with the right adopter is crucial for successful adoptions. Knowledge of adopter preferences can significantly aid in this matching process. The adoption of dogs encompasses a wide range of factors, including phenotype, behaviour, health, social influences such as breed popularity, socioeconomic and demographic factors, and prior pet ownership experience [12]. Recently, Kilgour and Flockhart (2022) identified four factors that impact the outcomes of cats in shelters: the physical attributes and health condition of the cat, the timing and location of intake, human influences before intake and experiences before outcomes.
Utilising surveys has long been recommended to enhance the understanding of adopter preferences, leading to improved matches between shelter animals and potential adopters, ultimately resulting in quicker and more efficient adoptions. This could free up shelter space, allowing for the accommodation of other animals [13].
Indeed, animals entering shelters have one of four destinies: reclaimed, adopted, passed away or euthanised. This research aimed to identify factors contributing to shelter animals' adoptability and uncover adopter preferences. The ultimate goal is to identify predictive adoption characteristics and reduce shelter animals' length of stay (LOS).

2. Materials and Methods

A cross-sectional study about pet adoption preferences was conducted from February to October 2021. A voluntary survey was carried out among a convenience sample of 415 participants from Portugal. Portugal is a medium-sized country on the European scale, with the mainland located in the Iberian Peninsula of southwestern Europe, including the Azores and Madeira Atlantic archipelagos. The Portuguese population corresponds to 10 344 802 residents [14].
Individuals were eligible to participate if they were at least 18 years old. After agreeing to participate in the study, each participant was asked to complete an anonymous, confidential 15-minute self-administered questionnaire distributed on online social media platforms. The sample size of this study was calculated according to a survey sample size calculation formula [15]. Assuming a 50% default prevalence, a 95% confidence level and a 5% absolute error, 384 participants were necessary to include in the study. To allow for a 10% non-response, the smallest sample size required was 415 participants.
The questionnaire was based on a literature review [8] and designed by the authors to obtain information. The questionnaire consisted of 16 closed or short-answer categorised questions. The questionnaire included questions about general knowledge and collected data, including demographic characteristics of the respondents (viz. gender, age, education level, residence, and occupation). The physical/mental occupation definitions were not present in the questionnaire but were based on the respondents' definitions. The study received ethical approval from the Ethics Commission of the University of Trás-os-Montes e Alto Douro (Doc6-CE-UTAD-2021).

Data analysis

Data were entered into an Excel database (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA) and exported and analysed using SPSS version 27.0 (IBM, New York, NY, USA). For descriptive purposes, the Pearson χ2 test was conducted for each variable in the study, looking at socio-demographic differences. Statistical significance was based on a p-value < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Demographics

Table 1 provides an overview of the demographics of the 415 participants in the study. The majority, 75.9% (n = 315), of respondents were female and 24.1% (n = 98) were male. Regarding age, 58.6% (n = 243) were less than 35 years, 40.0% (n = 166) were 35-65 years, and 1.4% (n = 6) were over 65 years. Concerning their professional situation, 75.2% (n = 312) were employed, 19.5% (n = 81) were students, 4.3% (n = 18) were unemployed, and 1.0% (n = 4) were retired. In relation to academic background, 75.9% of the respondents (n = 315) had completed higher education, and 24.1% (n = 100) had completed secondary education. Considering their family composition, 60.2% of respondents (n = 250) did not have children, 34.5% (n = 142) had 1-2 children, and 5.3% (n = 22) had three or more children. Regarding pet ownership, 71.3% (n = 296) of participants had one or more animals, with 47.5% (n = 197) owning cats and/or 49.2% (n = 204) owning dogs. Among dog owners, 81.4% (n = 166) had 1-2 dogs, and 18.6% (n = 38) had ≥ 3 dogs. Among cat owners, 75.1% (n = 148) had 1-2 cats, while 24.9% (n = 49) had ≥ 3 cats.

3.2. Adoption preferences

Respondents were asked about their preferences for acquiring dogs and cats (Figure 1). This was a multiple-response question. The most frequently chosen source for acquiring pets was an animal shelter, selected by 36.4% of respondents (n = 151). Friends were the second most common source, chosen by 26.7% (n = 111), followed by directly acquiring pets from the street at 15.7% (n = 65). Only a small percentage admitted buying pets from breeders (6.3%, n = 26) or pet shops (1.2%, n = 5).
Furthermore, 52.8% of participants (n = 219) reported never having visited an animal shelter, while 47.2% (n = 196) had visited shelters. Among those who had visited shelters, a significant proportion were cat owners (p < 0.001). Additionally, most participants (i.e. 84.6%; n = 351) had never adopted an animal from a shelter, while 15.4% (n = 64) had previously adopted from shelters. Most of those who had adopted from shelters were cat owners (p < 0.001).

3.3. Attributes chosen by participants

Participants were asked to select attributes they considered important when adopting a pet from a shelter. The results are summarised in Table 2:
The most selected attributes by participants when adopting from a shelter were behaviour (52.8%), size (49.6%), vaccination and deworming status (44.3%) and the age of the animal (36.1%). In contrast, the least selected attributes were breed (7.2%), sex (6.3%) and the colour of the animal (5.1%).

3.3.1. Associations regarding the most chosen attributes

The following associations were observed regarding the most chosen attributes: most participants who selected behaviour had higher education (p < 0.001); size was the most selected attribute among participants who had dogs (p = 0.035); those who chose the age attribute did not have cats (p = 0.035); participants who preferred vaccinated and dewormed animals had dogs (p = 0.008) or secondary education (p = 0.004); those who preferred microchipped and registered animals had higher education (p = 0.033); participants who chose the animal’s history had cats (p < 0.001). Regarding the least chosen attributes, these associations were observed: length of stay at the shelter was less chosen by participants who did not have children (p = 0.004) and by participants who had cats (p < 0.001); most participants who did not choose the breed attribute had cats (p < 0.001).

3.4. Top 10 most chosen response combinations

Ten response combinations were selected when 10 or more participants chose that combination, resulting in 81 different combinations. Table 3 below presents the ten most chosen response combinations for the three factors participants considered most important:
Table 3 displays the 10 most chosen response combinations of factors participants considered most important when adopting a shelter pet. The top four combinations were "Vaccinated and dewormed, Identified (microchip) and registered, Sterilized or will be for free" (9.2%), "Size, History of the animal, Behaviour" (8.2%), "Size, Age of the animal, Behaviour" (7.7%), and "Size, Vaccinated and dewormed, Behaviour" (4.8%). These combinations provide insights into the factors that participants prioritise when choosing a pet for adoption from a shelter.

3.5. Preference for Adoption:

The majority, i.e. 65.3% of participants (n = 271), preferred adopting dogs, while 34.7% (n = 144) preferred cats. Most of those who preferred to adopt dogs were dog owners (p < 0.001). Most of those who preferred to adopt cats were cat owners (p < 0.001). Participants with higher education preferred adopting dogs to cats (p = 0.033).

3.6. Breed preferences:

Regarding breed, 83.9% (n = 348) of participants were indifferent to the breed of the animal they would adopt, 10.6% (n = 44) wanted a defined breed, 4.8% (n = 20) preferred an undefined breed, 0.5% (n = 2) preferred crossbred animals and 0.2% (n = 1) preferred purebred. These differences were statistically significant (p = 0.032). Participants who preferred a defined or mixed breed indicated the following dog breeds: American Pit Bull Terrier, Beagle, Bernese Mountain Dog, Bulldog, Boxer, Cocker, Poodle, Transmontano Mastiff, German Wirehaired Pointer, Shiba Inu, Golden Retriever, Jack Russel Terrier, Labrador Retriever, German Shepherd, Portuguese Pointer, Estrela Mountain Dog, Pomeranian, and Yorkshire. For cats, the following breeds were indicated: Maine coon, Persian and Siamese.

3.7. Colour preferences

The majority (i.e. 85.8%; n = 356) were indifferent to the colour of the animal they would adopt, 9.9% (n = 41) preferred light-coloured animals, 4.1% (n = 17) preferred dark-coloured animals, and a single participant (0.2%) preferred grey-coloured animals.

3.8. Fur type preferences

Regarding the animal's fur to adopt, 63.1% (n = 262) considered the type of fur indifferent. Most participants who chose this option had dogs (p = 0.031). In this study, 24.1% (n = 100) preferred to adopt short-haired animals, 2.4% (n = 10) preferred long-haired animals, 8.2% (n = 34) preferred medium-haired animals, 1.2% (n = 5) preferred smooth-haired animals, 0.5% (n = 2) preferred wiry-haired animals, 0.2% (n = 1) preferred short- to medium-haired animals, and 0.2% (n = 1) preferred animals that did not shed fur.

3.9. Size preferences

Regarding the size of the animal to be adopted, 28.4% (n = 118) of participants did not have a specific preference for the size of the animal they would adopt, 33.3% (n = 138) preferred small-sized animals, and 30.4% (n = 126) preferred animals of medium size, and 8.0% (n = 33) preferred large size animals. Dog owners preferred to adopt small pets (p = 0.001).

3.10. Age preferences

Regarding the age of the animal to be adopted, 34.9% (n = 145) of participants were indifferent, 31.6% (n = 131) preferred young animals (3-11 months), 25.8% (n = 107) preferred very young animals (≤ 3 months), 5.3% (n = 22) preferred adult animals (1-6 years), and 2.4% (n = 10) preferred senior (> 6 years). Participants who owned cats chose the indifferent option more often (p < 0.001).

3.11. Length of stay preferences

Regarding the LOS in a shelter for the animal they would adopt, for 68.9% (n = 286) adopters it was indifferent, 18.3% (n = 76) preferred animals recently housed in a shelter, and 12.8% (n = 53) preferred an animal housed in a shelter for a long period. Participants who did not own cats were more likely to be indifferent about the LOS (p < 0.000).

4. Discussion

Exploring the territory of shelter medicine and pet adoption unveils a complex interaction of medical, ethical, and social elements, providing a subtle perspective on how adoption preferences shape and are moulded by the principal narrative of animal welfare and human-animal relationships.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study examining the knowledge towards pet adoption preferences among Portuguese residents in Portugal. Regarding data on the movement of animals in shelters, in Portugal, in 2022, 41,994 animals were collected from Portuguese streets to shelters, adding to those already there. Of all animals housed in Portuguese shelters, 24,721 were adopted, and 2378 were euthanised, representing an influx of 14,985 animals only in 2022 [2]. However, the analysis of these data must be carried out carefully as it can easily lead to error by not accounting for deaths due to illness or other causes, as well as restitution to the legitimate owners and animals already living in shelters.
Given the overwhelming number of animals entering shelters annually in addition to those living there, comprehending these adoption preferences is a pressing need [2]. Recognising these preferences can help shaping strategies to decrease shelter populations and, consequently, enhancing the welfare of many animals.
Pet acquisition has become a focal point of numerous studies, with the present study exploring the preferences of prospective pet owners, thereby revealing compelling patterns within pet acquisition sources. Strikingly, whilst 36.4% of respondents favoured adopting from a shelter, a substantial 84.6% confessed to never having adopted from one, with 52.8% never having visited one. This disparity between declared preference and prior action may unveil a complexity in attitudes and acquisition behaviours towards pets.
Many non-adopters are looking up but not planning to adopt when they visit a shelter or have no plans to adopt at all. Some plan to adopt but do not find what they want, especially a connection with the animal, and some did not plan to adopt when they visited the shelter but ended up adopting [16]. Such discrepancy, for instance, can be paralleled to studies by Garrison & Weiss [8], whereby, while shelter adoption was favoured, followed by acquisition from friends and breeders, a stark contradiction was visible in reference to surveys across 27 countries, wherein most pets were obtained from breeders (33.3%) or adopted from shelters/rehoming organisations (30.8%) [17]. Searching deeper into the scope of stated preferences whilst weighing the reality of participants' acquisition behaviours becomes vital. A social desirability bias, as hinted at by Bir et al. [18], might prompt answers that align with perceived social norms (adopting rather than purchasing), even if actual acquisition practices diverge from this norm, as verified by Garrison & Weiss [8], with 60% indicating shelters as their preferred source, but with only 39% adopting from a shelter. Of notice is the importance of variety among shelter animals, with people (40%) willing to travel almost 100 km to acquire pets that meet their preferences and 46% would delay or wait for the decision for adoption if the shelter did not have the pet correspondent to their preferences [8]. Moreover, our findings emphasise animal shelters and friends as prime sources for pet acquiring, aligning with previous research [18,19,20,21,22]. Specifically, the mere 6.3% admitting to purchasing from breeders underlines a growing inclination towards adopting or rescuing pets from shelters or other non-commercial sources [23]. A deeper reflection on the nuances driving these preferences and behaviours can provide more robust insights and inform effective strategies for promoting shelter adoptions since there is a large margin of growth for the adoption of shelter animals given the demonstration of preference or consideration by adopters of this source over the others.
An intriguing point is the apparent disconnect between expressed preference for shelter adoption (36.4%) and previous no shelter visitation (52.8%) and no adoption from shelter behaviours (84.6%), as reported by others [20,21,23,24]. Garrison & Weiss [8] discovered that 80% of dog owners acquired them from sources other than shelters despite most considered a shelter as the next source. Therefore, this manifestation of preference should be viewed with caution given that in another study a disconnection was noticed between what people said they would do and what they did, translated by 60% considering a shelter, but only 39% ending up acquiring a dog in a shelter [25]. Similar results have already been reported in a survey conducted by the American Humane Association in 2012, with 56% considering a shelter but only 22% adopting a dog from a shelter. This disconnect is justified by people considering that shelters do not have the type of animal sought or people looking for pure breeds [8].
What might be the perceived barrier that impedes shelter visits, even amongst those who prefer this source? It is also vital to ponder why a substantial percentage still acquires pets from friends or directly off the streets despite expressed preferences for shelter adoption.
The presented study evaluated the attributes prioritised by participants when adopting a pet from a shelter, revealing a distinctive preference for behaviour (52.8%), size (49.6%), and vaccination and deworming status (44.3%), alongside the age of the pet (36.1%). Curiously, breed (7.2%), sex (6.3%), and animal colour (5.1%) were the least prioritised. These results intriguingly contrast and partially align with existing literature, initiating a subtle exploration of potential adopter preference frameworks and the possible influences shaping them.
A divergence from the present study with Garrison & Weiss [8] becomes apparent, who ranked importance as follows: source, age, size, risk of euthanasia, breed, origin, and colour. However, resemblances concerning the minimal importance accorded to breed and colour, and the substantial emphasis on age are observed. Another critical observation involves the considerable weight given to behavioural attributes in the decision-making process. This aligns with Weiss et al. [7] findings that accentuated behaviour as crucial for cat adopters, in the way they approach and interact, and appearance being paramount for dog adopters, likely due to a wider variety of dog breeds. Although both attributes are important for both types of adopters. Even so, behaviour seems to play a greater role than phenotype since adopters attach greater importance to interaction with the animal.
Whilst previous literature has highlighted phenotype as a determinant in adoption choices [7,8,26,27], our findings bring out a nuanced perspective, suggesting behaviour surpasses phenotype in the adoption process [7]. Despite the fact that most of our respondents had dogs (49.2%), we found no evidence to corroborate that adopters might be further influenced by phenotypic characteristics and history than animal behaviour, as Protopopova et al. [26] suggested. This dichotomy deserves deeper exploration into how various factors, including the potential adopter experience and existing pet ownership status, may be shaping these preferences. For example, the observed associations – higher education with a preference for behaviour and microchipped, consistent with Brown et al. [28], secondary education and having dogs aligning with a preference for vaccinated and dewormed animals and owning cats correlating with valuing an animal’s history – lend weight to the hypothesis that existing interactions and experiences with pets shape future adoption preferences. Moreover, despite the notable importance of size, this factor appears to be contextually contingent, with divergent results recorded across various studies. For instance, while our results indicate a general preference for smaller sizes, Siettou et al. [11] and Garrison & Weiss [8] exhibited a more neutral position towards size, emphasising the non-uniformity in preference matrices across different adopter demographics and geographic locations. Additionally, our findings corroborate the significance of the pet behavioural attributes in adoption, analogous to numerous studies affirming the role of personality, temperament, and interaction in the adopter’s choices [7,11,29]. Cain et al. [9] indicated certain physical attributes, like being a non-brachycephalic puppy, coupled with a smaller size, potentially enhance adoption likelihood, especially under extended shelter stays, aligning with our findings. Concurrently, certain physical attributes such as colour and coat have been recorded as influential in adoption success in other contexts [13].
An imperative dialogue emerges regarding the relevance of age in adoption choices. Our results discern an association between a lack of cat ownership and an age preference. At the same time, both our findings and those of Garrison & Weiss [8] indicate age as a critical determinant in adoption choices. Interestingly, Siettou et al. [11] highlight the inverse relationship of age-adoption probability, underscoring adopter preferences' complexity and multidimensionality.
It is crucial to acknowledge that while specific attributes like colour, sex, and LOS in shelters may be minimally impactful in isolation [26], their cumulative interaction with other factors could shape adoption outcomes. Consequently, it is prudent to examine attribute interplay rather than isolating them since no single attribute seems to drive the decision-making process of adoption [8].
Our study corroborates Cohen & Todd [30], underscoring a general trend towards valuing behaviour over physical attributes. However, certain physical traits, such as skull type and predicted adult size, could influence adoption more than breed or breed group [9]. However, according to Siettou et al. [11], the classification of a dog as a purebred increases its chance of being adopted. The attribute "sex" was selected by 6.3% of participants as crucial in our study, positioning it as one of the less critical factors in the adoption decision process. In stark contrast, Cohen & Todd [30] indicated a considerably higher valuation of an animal’s sex at 36%, revealing a noteworthy disparity in the importance attributed to sex between different study contexts and suggesting potential variations in adopter prioritisation that diverse demographic or cultural factors might influence. Brown & Morgan [31] discovered that male cats and kittens experienced shorter LOS than their female counterparts. However, when investigating the preferences of adopters, Onodera et al. (2014) found that sex was the third most crucial factor in cat adoption choices, with over half of adopters showing a preference for female cats. Conversely, Janke et al. [32] observed in another study that male cats appeared more readily adopted than females.
Contemplating the nuanced consistency and variability amongst our findings and existing literature, future investigations might benefit from a multifaceted approach, exploring the synergistic interplay of adopter demographics, geographical/cultural contexts, and specific animal attributes in shaping adoption preferences.
This study also identified the most common response combinations among the three most important factors participants chose (Table 3). The four most chosen combinations were “Vaccinated and dewormed, Identified (microchip) and registered, Sterilized or will be for free”, “Size, Animal history, Behaviour”, “Size, Age, Behaviour”, and “Size, Vaccinated and Dewormed, Behaviour”. Predominantly, health conditions, including "Vaccinated and Dewormed", "Identified (microchip) and registered", and "Sterilized or will be for free", were highly favoured by respondents, echoing findings from previous research [7,21,28]. Clevenger and Kass [33] support this with a noted elevation in adoption rates linked to pre-adoption neutering, albeit contrasted by Janke et al. [32], who found neuter status insignificant in influencing adoption rates.
In another study, age and source were the two attributes with the most influence over choice [8], with puppy and animal shelter being the most positive features and senior and pet store the most negative features.
Moreover, response combinations that included "Size", "Animal History", and "Behaviour" also emerged as crucial within the decision-making process, corroborating earlier studies highlighting the influence of physical and behavioural traits on adoption [11]. Though age was only present in one of the top 10 response combinations, it might be perceived as a secondary consideration compared to behaviour and size, aligning with findings that downplay its significance in adoption decisions [21].
In the study by Garrison and Weiss [8], a medium-sized, dark-coloured, rare/unusual breed puppy with a high risk of euthanasia from the local community was the most popular profile, our study found no clear preference for any particular breed, colour, or size. Conversely, the least popular profile in that study was a large, brown-coloured, mixed-breed senior dog transported from out of state with no risk of euthanasia and obtained from a pet store.
A noteworthy 65.3% of participant preference towards dog adoption over cats (34.7%) aligns with prior research [7,8,11], potentially underscoring a partiality for adopting familiar or previously owned animals [10,12]. Since most of those who preferred to adopt dogs were already dog owners (p < 0.001), and most of those who preferred to adopt cats were already cat owners as well (p < 0.001).
Higher-educated participants showcasing a tendency towards dog adoption, hinting at a potential correlation between education level and adoption choices. This connection was observed in prior studies [28].
The results spotlight several vital factors influencing shelter pet adoption, providing further evidence that adopters consider multiple factors when adopting and that those have different weights in the complex matrix of preferences in adoption decisions.
The present investigation reveals a notable indifference toward breed selection in the animal adoption process, with 83.9% of participants expressing no breed preference. This aligns with Kwan & Bain [34], underscoring that breed often takes a backseat to other influential factors during adoption [34]. A mere 10.6% preferred a defined breed and 0.2% a purebred, similarly to Cohen & Todd [30], who found that mixed breed was preferred over pure breed (79% versus 21%, respectively), suggesting the breed may often be a subordinate factor in adoption decisions. Interestingly, cat adopters were even less concerned with breed, possibly due to a more limited aesthetic variance among cats than dogs [7].
Despite this, breed does not entirely escape consideration. The breed of both dogs and cats has been shown to influence adoption rates and LOS in shelters, particularly in purebred dogs [11,13,29,31,32,35,36,37]. Contrarily, certain breeds or physical characteristics, such as brachycephalic dogs, tend to experience lower adaptability [9], while exotic cat breeds might be adopted sooner than their native counterparts [31,32]. Breed-related stereotypes and public perception also play a role, potentially affecting adoption success variably for certain breed types [13,26,35,38]. Specifically, dogs resembling fighting breeds or those labelled as “blockhead” dogs could experience lowered adoption rates due to ingrained public perceptions [9,39] in a way that breeds misidentification can penalise adoptability [40] and this is a more problematic cause as even trained shelter professionals are unlikely to accurately characterise solely based on animal phenotype [38], suggesting that adoption success relies on public perception of the different breed types more than to phenotype and breed identification. Likewise, the intake mode, such as whether the pet was stray, confiscated or surrendered, influences adoption success and LOS, possibly due to prevailing public attitudes toward these categories [26,41].
Cain et al. [9] suggested that skull type, predicted adult size or coat length might be more predictive of outcome than breed alone. While the data implies that breed might be less critical, the intricate interaction between breed, physical characteristics, and public perception in adoption decision-making cannot be ignored. Further exploration is requisite to fully comprehend these dynamics and discern strategies to improve potential negative implications for certain breeds or physical types in adoption contexts.
Participant attitudes towards animal colour and fur type in adoption decisions manifest intriguing patterns. An extensive majority displayed indifference towards the colour of adoptable animals (85.8%), while only 9.9% leaned towards light-coloured, 4.1% towards dark-coloured animals, and only one participant expressed a preference for grey colour (0.2%). This (Table 3) aligns with existing research where colour tends to be a secondary consideration in adoption, often eclipsed by behavioural, size, and health considerations [11,27]. However, some studies exhibit varying data, suggesting that dogs with black or brindle coats might experience longer LOS and diminished adoption rates [35], whereas lighter coats can potentially expedite adoption [13,35,41]. Conversely, Cohen & Todd [30] found a preference for medium colours (light brown, red) followed by dark colours (black and dark brown) and light colours (white, grey, tan), further complicating the relationship between colour and adoption likelihood. Protopopova et al. [26] suggested that adopters may prefer unique colourations with varying preferences across different regions instead of being attracted to a specific colour. However, Brown et al. [28] reported that coat colour does not affect the LOS in shelters. Furthermore, in the jurisdiction of cat adoptions, Brown & Morgan [31] indicated that yellow-coloured cats had longer LOS but concluded that coat colour does not affect cat adoptability. Some noted prolonged adoption durations for black cats, corroborating the notable "black cat syndrome" [32,42], though other research studies have suggested white cats, followed by brown, grey and orange cats, more readily adopted than dark cats [43]. Also, those authors did not find improved outcomes during the month before Halloween, a phenomenon called “black cat bias”.
Regarding fur type, 63.1% (n = 262) of participants demonstrate indifference. Among those with preferences, 24.1% (n = 100) favoured short-haired animals, with only 2.4% (n = 10) leaning towards long-haired varieties. Such tendencies align with Weiss et al. [7], who documented a leaning towards short-haired dog adoption. Nevertheless, fur length occasionally lacks a significant impact on adoption decisions [44,45], with occasional disparities, such as Siettou et al. [11], highlighting a positive association between coat length and adoptability. Our findings insights illuminate that while physical attributes like colour and fur type can influence adoption (Table 3), substantial portions of adopters prioritise other factors like behaviour and personality over physical characteristics [7,11,26,27]. Nonetheless, the nuances within existing research underscore a necessity for further exploration, potentially elucidating varied preferences across different regions and demographic segments [26] and thereby refining strategic approaches for animal shelters to optimise adoption strategies and animal welfare. Consequently, integrating and understanding these varied findings and their applicability is crucial to strategically addressing adoption disparities and enhancing shelter outcomes.
A detailed look at adopter preferences for animal size reveals intriguing patterns and confirms previous findings in research. In the current study, a sizable group of participants, 28.4%, expressed no definitive preference regarding animal size for adoption. However, a distinct inclination towards smaller animals is evident, with 33.3% preferring small and 30.4% medium-sized animals. Only 8.0% leaned towards adopting larger animals. These findings mirror existing research, substantiating the recurrent theme of smaller animals typically experiencing higher adoption rates and shorter LOS [11,13,26,28]. Intriguingly, Cain et al. (2020) discovered a temporal shift in size preference, wherein small to medium-sized dogs initially enjoy higher adoption rates compared to larger counterparts, yet this advantage decreases over extended LOS. Conversely, Cohen & Todd [9,30] outlined a preference for medium-sized animals (59%) over small (34%) and large (33%) counterparts, hinting at potential variations in preferences within different demographic or geographic contexts and underlining the delicate complexity of adopter size preferences. The observed preference for smaller animals may originate from pragmatic considerations, such as residential space limitations or ease of management, particularly in urban living contexts [9]. Adopters potentially perceive smaller animals as more manageable, requiring fewer resources and being more adaptable to various living situations. In summary, while a significant inclination towards smaller animals in adoption preferences persists in general trends, divergences in data suggest potential influences from regional or demographic variables. Continuous research and localised studies are imperative to navigate the complex network of factors influencing adoption preferences, facilitating refined strategies to sustain adoption rates across all size categories in animal shelters. A strategic approach informed by a nuanced understanding of size preferences could increase adoption efforts and improve animal welfare outcomes across diverse sizes.
The connexion of age preference and adoption rates of animals deserves a detailed exploration. In the presented study, the age preferences were notably varied. This age preference merges with existing literature indicating a pronounced inclination towards younger animals among adopters [9,26,30,31]. For instance, Cain et al. [9] discerned that puppies experienced a higher adoption likelihood than adult dogs, albeit witnessing a reduction as the LOS in shelters increased. Meanwhile, senior dogs initially encountered lower adoption probabilities that improved with extended LOS. Notably, after a LOS of 50 and 80 days, their adoption probabilities matched and surpassed those of adults, respectively. This is probably due to adopters or rescue groups specifically seeking out senior dogs or shelters promoting their adoption. Intriguingly, seniors aged 8 years and above demonstrated slightly higher adoptability than their 5-7-year-old counterparts in a study by Siettou et al. [11], and in a context involving geriatric dogs, health condition was a pivotal determinant in adoptability, notwithstanding an average LOS of 89 days [46].
The dominant preference for younger animals could be partly elucidated by the “Kindchenschema”/baby schema effect, wherein juvenile mammals generate an intrinsic disposition to nurture in humans [47,48]. This phenomenon manifested even in children as young as 3 years, may potentiate the attraction of pets that embody neotenous (youthful) attributes and behaviours, fostering our desire to care for them [49,50].
Intriguingly, the human-animal dynamic extends beyond age to encompass behaviour, with some research implying that adopters might be more swayed by explicit prosocial behaviours in animals, such as rubbing, rather than subtle facial expressions in domestic cats [51]. Moreover, cat behaviours like slow blinking, which humans might interpret as a positive signal, as it shares certain features with the Duchenne smile (the one that reaches the eyes) in humans, as well as reaction to positive emotional contexts in other mammals, can also significantly impact adoption likelihood [52,53].
In a broader context, the dichotomy of prevalent preference for youthful pets versus the emergent, although modest, traction for older animals with extended LOS illustrates a multifaceted panorama. Over time, the incremental adoptability of older pets might be fuelled by specific adoption campaigns, rescue group initiatives or adopter demographics expressly seeking mature or senior animals. This intricate matrix of preferences and influences highlights animal shelters need to employ subtle, strategic approaches in promotion and adoption facilitation. Understanding oscillations of age preference informs effective shelter strategies and empowers initiatives to elevate the visibility and adoptability of often-overlooked, such as senior animals. Thus, shelters can holistically provide to the varied preferences of potential adopters, thereby optimising adoption rates across all age categories and ensuring improved welfare outcomes for animals irrespective of age.
Length of stay (LOS) within animal shelters emerged as a layered determinant in adoption, linking with factors like age and perceived animal characteristics. The current study, which underscores that a considerable 68.9% of participants were indifferent towards LOS during adoption, aligns with findings by Patronek et al. [21], where LOS was not a pivotal criterion for adopters. In contrast, 18.3% and 12.8% preferred recently housed animals and those with prolonged shelter stays, respectively. This bifurcation in preference, especially favouring newer shelter entrants, diverges from Weiss et al. (2012), wherein adopters leaned towards animals with shorter LOS.
Cain et al. [9] explored the evolving dynamics of adoption probabilities as LOS extends, observing a mutable impact of age, predicted adult size and skull type on adoption likelihoods with increased LOS, implying that the influence of phenotype changes. This comprehensive understanding is particularly intriguing when observing the adoption trajectories of puppies. While their initial adoption probabilities are notably high, an extended LOS diminishes this likelihood, even though puppies generally experience shorter LOS than older counterparts [8,28,29,35,39,54]. Interestingly, Siettou et al. [11] found older dogs (over 8 years) marginally more adoptable than adults, adding another dimension to the discourse on age, LOS, and adoptability.
For cats, a similar age-LOS-adoptability dynamic unfolds. Younger cats typically see higher adoption rates than older ones [31], and prolonged LOS generally attenuates adoptability [9]. Contrarily, Janke et al. [32] noted adult cats have higher adoption rates than kittens, albeit with an acknowledgement of methodological considerations influencing such outcomes. Additionally, cat colouration and breed have been observed to impact LOS and, by extension, adoptability [31]. The contrasting studies insinuate that while LOS, age and other factors are discernible in their impacts on adoption, their interplay can be modulated by diverse contexts and demographics within and across shelters. It might imply that factors such as shelter practices, local culture, community outreach, and marketing efforts potentially contour these adoption backgrounds and modulate the influences of age and LOS.
Understanding this complexity and variability necessitates animal shelters to adopt a flexible, adaptive approach to animal promotion and adopter engagement. Adapting interventions to the general and specific trends of adopter preferences, punctuated with thoughtful marketing and community engagement, may holistically enhance adoption outcomes across varying LOS and age groups. Further, incorporating strategic interventions for traditionally overlooked animals – such as older pets or those with extended LOS – might amplify their visibility and adoptability and enlighten potential adopters about the joys and merits of adopting these often-undervalued animals.
A thoughtful amalgamation of generalised trends, local nuances and adaptive strategies can facilitate a holistic, inclusive approach to animal adoption, ensuring every animal finds a loving, permanent home. Studies investigating the factors that affect adopter choice reveal the broad range of attributes that individuals consider when adopting. Furthermore, these attributes have numerous variations, such as behaviour, age, size, health, and sterilisation status, among many others. Variations in regional factors such as the population of stray animals, temperature and their survival, legislation regarding stray animals, spay-neuter requirements, leash laws, strictness of pet ownership laws, socioeconomic factors, culture, predominant breeds and phenotypes may impact adoption rates [9,55]. While attributes such as behaviour, age, and size emerge as predominant factors influencing adoption from shelters, it is crucial to approach these findings with a knowledge of the intricate, multifaceted web of influences shaped by the potential adopters’ demographic and experiential backgrounds. Consequently, future research exploring these complex interrelations promises to be a fertile ground for enhancing understanding and optimising adoption practices across various contexts.
Our results demonstrate that people have complex adoption frames of preferences as described by Garrison and Weiss [8], showing that individuals have varying preferences, indicating that diversity is present even in this aspect of pet adoption, which may be better matched if there is variety in the animals at the shelter. So, the authors suggest a comprehensive animal relocation program to increase the variety in shelters and thus better meet the requirements of adopters [8]. Overall, the results of this study provide further insight into the factors that influence adoption decisions and suggest that education level and previous pet ownership experience may be important factors to consider. These findings can inform animal shelters and adoption agencies about the attributes that potential adopters consider important and may help them better match animals with suitable homes. Another limitation of our study is not having analysed the existence of correspondence or dissonance – the “state-revealed preference gap” – between the preferences established by the Portuguese and the characteristics of the animals effectively adopted [30].

Study limitations

As per our previous discussion, some limitations of the study need to be acknowledged. Firstly, the study may have a narrow focus on specific attributes, which may impact the generalizability of the results. Secondly, there appears to be a discrepancy between the findings of this study and those of others, which could be due to methodological inconsistencies. Furthermore, the results may only apply to some populations or cultural settings. The study may also be affected by selection biases related to participant recruitment. Lastly, it is worth noticing that there could be temporal and regional preferences that were not considered, which may affect the results. It is important to admitting that the sample used for the study may not be fully representative of the general population and that the study only provides a snapshot of preferences among a specific population at a specific time.

5. Conclusions

Our study is the first to examine the pet adoption preferences of Portuguese residents in Portugal. Notably, most participants favoured adopting from shelters, followed by acquiring pets from friends or rescuing them directly from the street. These findings align with prior research, emphasising shelters and friends as common sources, while breeders and pet stores are less frequented. Moreover, only a minority admitted to purchase pets from breeders, reflecting the growing trend of adopting or rescuing animals. Although few have visited shelters or adopted from them, there is a positive shift toward shelter adoption. Participants valued behaviour, size, vaccination, deworming, and age when selecting shelter animals, while breed, sex, and colour held less importance.
Our results highlight the potential for promoting shelter adoption, aligning with current trends and research indicating rising awareness of its benefits. Shelters can utilise this data to enhance resource allocation and match animals with adopter preferences, boosting successful adoptions. This underscores the need for considering diverse populations and conducting cost-benefit analyses for feasible interventions. Understanding adopter preferences in shelters is crucial for successful adoptions, as living situations and prior pet ownership influence decisions. Ongoing research can further illuminate these preferences, aiding shelters in refining adoption processes. Understanding how shelters can expedite adoptions and increase live releases is essential for their mission's success.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.A., A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; methodology, P.A., A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; validation, P.A., A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; formal analysis, P.A., A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; investigation, P.A., A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; resources, P.A., A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; data curation, P.A. and L.C.; writing—original draft preparation, P.A.; writing—review and editing, A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; visualization, A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; supervision, A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; project administration, A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C.; funding acquisition, A.C.C., H.Q., and L.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the projects UIDP/00772/2020 and LA/P/0059/2020, funded by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT).

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of UTAD (process reference: Doc6-CE-UTAD-2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all the participants involved in this study for making this study possible.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Assembleia da República Lei n.o 27/2016, de 23 de Agosto. Diário da República 2016, 1.a série, 2827–2828.
  2. Departamento de Bem-Estar dos Animais de Companhia Relatório Anual: Atividade Dos Centros de Recolha Oficial – 2022; Lisboa, 2023.
  3. Dinnage, J.D.; Scarlett, J.M.; Richards, J.R. Descriptive Epidemiology of Feline Upper Respiratory Tract Disease in an Animal Shelter. J Feline Med Surg 2009, 11, 816–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Dybdall, K.; Strasser, R. Is There a Bias Against Stray Cats in Shelters? People’s Perception of Shelter Cats and How It Influences Adoption Time. Anthrozoos 2014, 27, 603–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Gouveia, K.; Magalhães, A.; de Sousa, L. The Behaviour of Domestic Cats in a Shelter: Residence Time, Density and Sex Ratio. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2011, 130, 53–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Salgirli Demirbas, Y.; Isparta, S.; Ozturk, H.; Safak, E.; Emre, B.; Piskin, İ.; Kaya, U.; Sagmanligil, V.; Akgul, B.; Da Graça Pereira, G. Functional Cerebral Asymmetry in Dogs Living under Different Environmental Conditions. Behavioural Processes 2019, 165, 4–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Weiss, E.; Miller, K.; Mohan-Gibbons, H.; Vela, C. Why Did You Choose This Pet?: Adopters and Pet Selection Preferences in Five Animal Shelters in the United States. Animals 2012, 2, 144–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Garrison, L.; Weiss, E. What Do People Want? Factors People Consider When Acquiring Dogs, the Complexity of the Choices They Make, and Implications for Nonhuman Animal Relocation Programs. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2015, 18, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cain, C.J.; Woodruff, K.A.; Smith, D.R. Phenotypic Characteristics Associated with Shelter Dog Adoption in the United States. Animals 2020, 10, 1959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Kilgour, R.J.; Flockhart, D.T.T. Direct and Indirect Factors Influencing Cat Outcomes at an Animal Shelter. Front Vet Sci 2022, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Siettou, C.; Fraser, I.M.; Fraser, R.W. Investigating Some of the Factors That Influence “Consumer” Choice When Adopting a Shelter Dog in the United Kingdom. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2014, 17, 136–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Holland, K.E. Acquiring a Pet Dog: A Review of Factors Affecting the Decision-Making of Prospective Dog Owners. Animals 2019, 9, 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Posage, J.M.; Bartlett, P.C.; Thomas, D.K. Determining Factors for Successful Adoption of Dogs from an Animal Shelter. J Am Vet Med Assoc 1998, 213, 478–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Instituto Nacional de Estatística Recenseamento Da População e Habitação; 2021.
  15. Hajian-Tilaki, K. Sample Size Estimation in Epidemiologic Studies. Caspian J Intern Med 2011, 2, 289–298. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  16. Southland, A.; Dowling-Guyer, S.; McCobb, E. Effect of Visitor Perspective on Adoption Decisions at One Animal Shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2019, 22, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Carroll, G.A.; Torjussen, A.; Reeve, C. Companion Animal Adoption and Relinquishment during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Peri-Pandemic Pets at Greatest Risk of Relinquishment. Front Vet Sci 2022, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Bir, C.; Widmar, N.; Croney, C. Stated Preferences for Dog Characteristics and Sources of Acquisition. Animals 2017, 7, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Bir, C.; Olynk Widmar, N.; Croney, C. Exploring Social Desirability Bias in Perceptions of Dog Adoption: All’s Well That Ends Well? Or Does the Method of Adoption Matter? Animals 2018, 8, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. New, J.C.; Salman, M.D.; King, M.; Scarlett, J.M.; Kass, P.H.; Hutchison, J.M. Characteristics of Shelter-Relinquished Animals and Their Owners Compared With Animals and Their Owners in U.S. Pet-Owning Households. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2000, 3, 179–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Patronek, G.J.; Glickman, L.T.; Beck, A.M.; McCabe, G.P.; Ecker, C. Risk Factors for Relinquishment of Cats to an Animal Shelter. J Am Vet Med Assoc 1996, 209, 582–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Shore, E.R.; Petersen, C.L.; Douglas, D.K. Moving As a Reason for Pet Relinquishment: A Closer Look. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2003, 6, 39–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Humane Society of the United States Adopt Don’t Shop: Why You Should Rescue Your Next Pet Available online: https://www.humanesociety.org/resources/adopt-dont-shop-why-you-should-rescue-your-next-pet.
  24. Mohan-Gibbons, H.; Weiss, E.; Garrison, L.; Allison, M. Evaluation of a Novel Dog Adoption Program in Two US Communities. PLoS One 2014, 9, e91959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Protopopova, A.; Gilmour, A.J.; Weiss, R.H.; Shen, J.Y.; Wynne, C.D.L. The Effects of Social Training and Other Factors on Adoption Success of Shelter Dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2012, 142, 61–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Weiss, E.; Slater, M.; Garrison, L.; Drain, N.; Dolan, E.; Scarlett, J.; Zawistowski, S. Large Dog Relinquishment to Two Municipal Facilities in New York City and Washington, D.C.: Identifying Targets for Intervention. Animals 2014, 4, 409–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Svoboda, H.; Hoffman, C. Investigating the Role of Coat Colour, Age, Sex, and Breed on Outcomes for Dogs at Two Animal Shelters in the United States. Animal Welfare 2015, 24, 497–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Brown, W.P.; Davidson, J.P.; Zuefle, M.E. Effects of Phenotypic Characteristics on the Length of Stay of Dogs at Two No Kill Animal Shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2013, 16, 2–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Kwan, J.Y.; Bain, M.J. Owner Attachment and Problem Behaviors Related to Relinquishment and Training Techniques of Dogs. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2013, 16, 168–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Brown, W.P.; Morgan, K.T. Age, Breed Designation, Coat Color, and Coat Pattern Influenced the Length of Stay of Cats at a No-Kill Shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2015, 18, 169–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Janke, N.; Berke, O.; Flockhart, T.; Bateman, S.; Coe, J.B. Risk Factors Affecting Length of Stay of Cats in an Animal Shelter: A Case Study at the Guelph Humane Society, 2011–2016. Prev Vet Med 2017, 148, 44–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lepper, M.; Kass, P.H.; Hart, L.A. Prediction of Adoption Versus Euthanasia Among Dogs and Cats in a California Animal Shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2002, 5, 29–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Onodera, N.; Uchida, K.; Kakuma, Y. Association between Characteristics of Cats and Satisfaction of Owners Who Adopted Cats from an Animal Hospital in Japan. Journal of Veterinary Medical Science 2014, 76, 729–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sinski, J.; Carini, R.M.; Weber, J.D. Putting (Big) Black Dog Syndrome to the Test: Evidence from a Large Metropolitan Shelter. Anthrozoos 2016, 29, 639–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Olson, K.R.; Levy, J.K.; Norby, B.; Crandall, M.M.; Broadhurst, J.E.; Jacks, S.; Barton, R.C.; Zimmerman, M.S. Inconsistent Identification of Pit Bull-Type Dogs by Shelter Staff. The Veterinary Journal 2015, 206, 197–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Patronek, G.; Crowe, A. Factors Associated with High Live Release for Dogs at a Large, Open-Admission, Municipal Shelter. Animals 2018, 8, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Gunter, L.M.; Barber, R.T.; Wynne, C.D.L. What’s in a Name? Effect of Breed Perceptions & Labeling on Attractiveness, Adoptions & Length of Stay for Pit-Bull-Type Dogs. PLoS One 2016, 11, e0146857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Wells, D.; Hepper, P.G. The Behaviour of Dogs in a Rescue Shelter. Animal Welfare 1992, 1, 171–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kogan, L.R.; Schoenfeld-Tacher, R.; Hellyer, P.W. Cats in Animal Shelters: Exploring the Common Perception That Black Cats Take Longer to Adopt. Open Vet Sci J 2013, 7, 18–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Carini, R.M.; Sinski, J.; Weber, J.D. Coat Color and Cat Outcomes in an Urban U.S. Shelter. Animals 2020, 10, 1720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Diverio, S.; Boccini, B.; Menchetti, L.; Bennett, P.C. The Italian Perception of the Ideal Companion Dog. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 2016, 12, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. King, T.; Marston, L.C.; Bennett, P.C. Describing the Ideal Australian Companion Dog. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2009, 120, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Cohen, S.E.; Todd, P.M. Stated and Revealed Preferences in Companion Animal Choice. Behav Res Methods 2019, 51, 1498–1509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hawes, S.; Kerrigan, J.; Morris, K. Factors Informing Outcomes for Older Cats and Dogs in Animal Shelters. Animals 2018, 8, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Lorenz, K. Die Angeborenen Formen Möglicher Erfahrung. Z Tierpsychol 1943, 5, 235–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lehmann, V.; Huis in‘t Veld, E.M.J.; Vingerhoets, A.J.J.M. The Human and Animal Baby Schema Effect: Correlates of Individual Differences. Behavioural Processes 2013, 94, 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Borgi, M.; Cirulli, F. Children’s Preferences for Infantile Features in Dogs and Cats. Hum Anim Interact Bull 2013, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Borgi, M.; Cirulli, F. Pet Face: Mechanisms Underlying Human-Animal Relationships. Front Psychol 2016, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Caeiro, C.C.; Burrows, A.M.; Waller, B.M. Development and Application of CatFACS: Are Human Cat Adopters Influenced by Cat Facial Expressions? Appl Anim Behav Sci 2017, 189, 66–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Humphrey, T.; Stringer, F.; Proops, L.; McComb, K. Slow Blink Eye Closure in Shelter Cats Is Related to Quicker Adoption. Animals 2020, 10, 2256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Humphrey, T.; Proops, L.; Forman, J.; Spooner, R.; McComb, K. The Role of Cat Eye Narrowing Movements in Cat–Human Communication. Sci Rep 2020, 10, 16503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Vitulová, S.; Voslářová, E.; Večerek, V.; Bedáňová, I. Behaviour of Dogs Adopted from an Animal Shelter. Acta Veterinaria Brno 2018, 87, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Reese, L.A. Community Factors and Animal Shelter Outcomes. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2022, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Sources considered by participants for adopting an animal.
Figure 1. Sources considered by participants for adopting an animal.
Preprints 95619 g001
Table 1. Participants' demographic characteristics.
Table 1. Participants' demographic characteristics.
Demographic variable Percent (%) of all respondents (n = 415) Percent (%) of all respondents reportedly owning a pet (n = 296) Percent (%) of all respondents reportedly not owning a pet (n = 119)
Sex
Female 75.9% 80.1% 65.5%
Male 24.1% 19.9% 34.5%
Age group
< 35 years 58.6% 61.5% 51.3%
35-65 years 40.0% 37.8% 45.4%
> 65 years 1.4% 0.7% 3.3%
Professional situation
Employed 75.2% 74.7% 76.5%
Students 4.3% 20.3% 17.6%
Unemployed 19.5% 4.3% 4.2%
Retired 1.0% 0.7% 1.7%
Education Background
Higher Education 75.9% 75.3% 77.3%
Secondary Education 24.1% 24.7% 22.7%
Number of children
0 60.2% 60.8% 58.8%
1-2 34.5% 32.4% 39.5%
3 or more 5.3% 6.8% 1.7%
Table 2. Attributes chosen by participants.
Table 2. Attributes chosen by participants.
Attribute Frequency (n) Percentage %
Behaviour 219 52.8
Size 206 49.6
Vaccinated and dewormed 184 44.3
Age 150 36.1
History 137 33.0
Sterilized or will be for free 120 28.9
Be identified (microchip) and registered 103 24.8
Length of Stay (LOS) 41 9.9
Adoption cost 34 8.2
Breed 30 7.2
Sex 26 6.3
Colour 21 5.1
Table 3. Distribution of the ten most chosen response categories for the three most selected factors selected by the participants as the most important in case they adopt in a shelter.
Table 3. Distribution of the ten most chosen response categories for the three most selected factors selected by the participants as the most important in case they adopt in a shelter.
Response categories for the three most selected factors Frequency (n) Percentage %
Vaccinated and dewormed, Identified (microchip) and registered, Sterilized or will be for free 38 9.2%
Size, History, Behaviour 34 8.2%
Size, Age of the animal, Behaviour 32 7.7%
Size, Vaccinated and dewormed, Behaviour 20 4.8%
Age, Animal history, Behaviour 16 3.9%
Size, Vaccinated and dewormed, Identified (microchip) and registered 16 3.9%
Size, Vaccinated and dewormed, Identified (microchip) and registered 16 3.9%
Vaccinated and dewormed, Sterilized or will be for free, Behaviour 15 3.6%
Animal history, Behaviour, Length of stay 13 3.1%
Vaccinated and dewormed, Age of the animal, Behaviour 12 2.9%
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated