Introduction
The brain models I’ll refer to are the anatomic ones, which may or not have a developmental basis; the prosomeric model is eminently developmental, being based on embryonic transverse neuromeric units as fundamental constituents of the brain. Morphologic models of the brain postulate a particular viewpoint on its basic structural constitution. This implies giving both an
ontology of its fundamental parts, and a general description of the
mutual arrangements of these parts usually represented in a schema. In the case of the brain, which is an elongated structure, a length
axis orienting our positional descriptions and section planes needs to be
defined and
justified (any arbitrary line traced along the schema, or particularly through the ventricular fluid filled cavity, is not sufficiently realistic; the axis is a fundamental morphologic causal concept that needs to be based on solid developmental processes). The prosomeric model fundaments its length axis along the median floorplate, induced early on by the notochord (a process emerged evolutionarily at the start of vertebrates; see
Figure 1).
In any case, human curiosity tends not to be satisfied with a mere schema of parts. If you were given a detailed list of the parts of a clockwork mechanism, accompanied by abundant schemata of how each mechanical part relates wheel to wheel to the others, you probably would wonder about the general function of the whole mechanism, or the specific functions of each subset of parts. A morphological brain model similarly is not an end by itself, as a schematic map could be, but often implies predictions or expectations about the function of the various interactive parts. Here developmental models refer to developmental processes and adult fates, whose adult consequences refer to functions. The functional question thus emerges in neurobiology, particularly when novel brain structural models are proposed: ¿Which are the novel developmental and/or functional implications of this brain model you want me to work with? Does it offer deeper or more abundant insights than preceding models? It is expected of any minimally interesting new model that it demonstrates both a strong underpinning of its structural details by convincing morphological and developmental evidence (establishing credibility of the model itself), and novel functional implications of some importance (interest of the model).
Surprisingly, many neurobiologists or neuroclinicians are not much aware of which neuromorphological model they are using in their professional work. Even when they have a notion about it, their comprehension of the relevant assumptions and implications may be quite foggy. Some experts actually think, wrongly, that they are not using any specific brain model (they think they just deal with known facts about the brain). It turns out that everybody necessarily uses a model, knowingly or not, since otherwise we cannot deal with the complexity of the brain, nor understand its nomenclature. Moreover, most professionals also do not know precisely how many different brain models exist that may be mutually compared, in order to choose among them for optimal guidance in personal professional use or for teaching purposes. This mental hole partly occurs because professionals only are exposed superficially to a brain model in the very first hours of their long past studies of neuroanatomy, and they never saw afterwards a deep discussion on that subject.
Indeed, textbooks of neuroanatomy and neuroscience treatises invariably use a specific brain model chosen among a variety of them available, but do not present it openly as an optional construct, identified under the name of ‘model this or that’. Such a presentation would require explaining at least its basic assumptions and main underpinning data, thus opening it to criticism and contrary evidence, a desirable, but hardly practiced option. This sort of open and non-dogmatic presentation rarely occurs in practice. The usually unnamed model followed by the book authors is instead introduced as a dogmatic definition briefly telling the reader that the brain is made of a number of parts x, y, z. The briefness of the approach apparently intends to orient the reader towards placing his/her attention on other more important aspects of the book’s message. The major aim lies in giving a minimum of conventional names needed for reading further about these brain parts, disdaining consideration of their structural arrangements, axial relationships, and possible functions. Sometimes the brain length axis postulated in the book’s model is mentioned or illustrated, but is never discussed causally or comparatively. There is no analysis comparing the existing alternative brain models, which are not even mentioned. The reader is thus attracted subliminally to having faith in the particular model used by the author without being offered a corresponding rationale (as the author probably was in his own time). The message is therefore unwittingly transmitted across generations that the presented basic structural knowledge is not a target for attention, since its core items have apparently been repeatedly corroborated by all major experts over many years, so that they must represent a widely accepted consensus on which you can depend without further thought. These defective and irrational introductions to neuromorphology are more of a habit or tradition than a consciously selected approach, since nobody intends to fix an eternal dogma. Simply, old ideas tend to become fixed as if they are facts, which only need to be briefly stated; no arguments about underpinning evidence are needed. The existence of such defective introductions of brain models represents an easily verifiable fact; just check the beginning of any major neurobiological source you may have around to identify the dogmatic nature of the unnamed structural brain model given therein.
The human brain happens to be the most complex structural system composed of interactive parts we know about on Earth. The neuroanatomic source literature on brain models is manifestly opaque and seems therefore oddly unhelpful from a didactic viewpoint. Neurophysiology and neuropsychology are derived neurobiological sciences that study specifically brain functional phenomena at different levels of complexity, but they traditionally attend even less to brain structural models than the neuroanatomic mother science.
There are possibly two additional explanations of this traditional lack of epistemological attention to brain models. One possibility is that, historically, humanity needed several millennia in order to achieve a sufficiently complete and reliable list of the diversity of anatomic parts present in the human brain, largely because the initial methods of analysis were too crude (see historical works such as Clarke and O’Malley 1968 and Meyer 1971 to get a glimpse on that aspect). The intrinsic insecurity felt in available anatomic knowledge did not make easy having a consensus on which are the most meaningful primary structural and functional units of brains in general. Note we may distinguish three successive epochs of brain knowledge up till the present state. These epochs were dominated successively by, first, anatomic dissection and naked-eye analysis, second, microscopic neurohistology and neurophysiology and, third, molecular biology plus neuropharmacology.
A recent expanded ontology of the developing mouse brain assessing all its named anatomic compartments irrespective of size (distinct neuronal brain subregions) was elaborated by the present author for the Allen Developing Mouse Brain Atlas during 2008-2011. It was found that over 2500 differently named places apparently exist per each brain halve, so that there are roughly 5000 such parts functioning together in the developing mouse brain. This schema of parts is thought to be shared by most mammals including humans, though the latter apparently generate more subdivisions at given places like the cerebral cortex. At the end of 2023 the Allen Institute has presented a new mouse brain map generated with single-cell transcriptomic molecular methodology (based on analysis of which genes are differentially active in different neuronal populations, depending on their localization). This map is called The Whole Mouse Brain Atlas. It distinguishes some 5300 different cell populations in each brain half, without entering into the many cortical subdivisions, which might lift the number to 5500, thus bringing the approximate total of the brain to 11.000 molecularly distinguishable parts (doubling in 2023 our estimate of 2011). It is not possible to generate a realistic brain model with 5500 parts (models usually map one half of the brain).
Introductory texts of neuroanatomy clearly do not approximate at all this presently existing level of knowledge, giving maximally only a few dozen anatomic names for the basic orientation of readers. Due to their mostly ancient roots in research from the 19th century (200 years ago), a good number of these basic concepts are not strictly consistent with the novel molecular knowledge; anatomic brain parts identified some hundreds of years ago were mere crude approximations to reality as we see it today. This historical contrast reveals that most present-day professional neurobiologists are handicapped as regards appraising the full structural and functional complexity of the mammalian brain. Active researchers normally specialize in particular subregions of the brain, as a way to deal with such complexity, and are thus expert only on given functional subsystems. They consequently tend to pay a price in their diminished perspective on the overall scenario and thus do not control a whole brain model even if they so desired. I know this from personal experience. It took me many years of exploration to be able to visualize mentally the whole brain.
The columnar model
An additional explanation of the described poor scenario is conceivable. It relates to the fact that neuroscientists, book authors, and brain atlas makers have basically been using the same brain model - known to experts as ‘the columnar model’- for the last 100 years or so. This model was originally proposed by C.J.Herrick in 1910 on the background of important comparative studies performed around the end of the 19th century on the sorts of functionally different fiber constituents of the spinal and cranial nerves and their selective connections with anatomically and functionally distinct sets of neurons found in the brainstem and spinal cord of vertebrates (Gaskell 1889; Johnston 1902; Herrick 1903). These then newly identified source or target neuronal groups were described as forming longitudinally disposed (and dorsoventrally stacked) sensory and motor neuronal columns, which were held to represent the long sought fundamental structural parts of brain functional organization (thus ‘the columnar model’ name; Herrick 1910). Moreover, this author extrapolated speculatively this columnar structure notion also to then relatively unexplored forebrain parts, namely the diencephalon and the telencephalon (note both regions are devoid of nerves providing sensorimotor in- or outputs, if we except olfactory input).
The 1910 columnar model in essence postulated that the whole brain was made of a few dozens of neuronal columns distributed over a few anatomic regions (forebrain, midbrain, hindbrain, and spinal cord), where each unitary column would embody a different sensory, motor, or sympathetic/parasympathetic function. The primary sensory columnar units would intercommunicate with a hierarchy of integrative higher-level centers in order to knit sensory input and analysis into neural representational models of the body and the world which would serve for planning efferent motor output to produce reflex or voluntary behavior. So-called ‘higher’ mental functions were thought to emerge collaterally with increased evolutionary encephalization (disproportionate growth of particular higher brain parts such as the basal ganglia and the cerebral cortex, or the cerebellum, relative to the animal’s body weight) and consequent acquisition of more complex networked aspects of internal connectivity. Herrick was thus an important author that gave cogent attention to the functional implications of his brain model in his research articles (usually examining in great detail simple amphibian brains such as newts and salamanders), numerous functional divulgation articles published throughout his carrier, and several books (Herrick 1924, 1926, 1931, 1932, 1948, 1949, 1956; see the list of functional publications in this last reference). Somewhat surprisingly for a neuroanatomist, Herrick argued vehemently that ‘neuromorphology should be functional’; for instance, he openly discrepated with Kuhlenbeck (1927, 1973), a comparative neuroanatomist and embryologist who likewise defended the ‘columnar model’ after studying the major lineages of vertebrates on the basis of ‘strictly formanalytic’ developmental analysis (i.e., embryologic studies devoid of functional applications). Herrick clearly preferred the parallel morphofunctional studies on amphibians of his friend Coghill (whose biography he wrote and commented in detail; Herrick 1948, 1949, 1956). A similar primarily functional outlook was demonstrated in a then revolutionary book where Herrick’s colleague J.B.Johnston (1908) presented the early columnar functional theses.
After the first and second world wars the American school of neuroanatomy grew considerably around Herrick’s columnar model and the latter gained much scientific ascendancy and became implicitly or explicitly accepted by members of European or Asiatic neuroanatomy schools. This widespread support eventually led by the end of the twentieth century to the gradual crystallization of the ‘functional’ columnar model assumptions as representing undoubted facts, rather than remaining tentative theoretical constructs, as they were at the beginning (and in fact continued being 100 years afterwards). This worldwide scenario probably was the immediate cause of the cursory and dogmatic treatment given to this model in most source books presently in use, and thus possibly also explains why most present-day neurobiologists ignore not only that they are using the original columnar model of 1910 (or one of its subsequent minor variants; see e.g., Swanson 2012) in their professional activities and teaching, but also ignore whether the columnar modelling of the brain has turned out to be satisfactory in the long run or not.
Modern physiologists only partly describe their results in terms of the classic columns, and physiology books normally leave out large parts of the brain whose connections and functions remain hardly understood. One may point out as Cinderella-like brain places which are hardly mentioned in functional perspective: the preoptic area, many hypothalamic subregions, the prethalamus, the pretectum, the prepontine hindbrain, as well as large parts of the telencephalon. After more than 100 years of research with progressively more potent methodologies these brain areas have not been explained functionally by the columnar modelling approach, and some of the apparently available partial explanations are subject to recent critical comments (see Puelles 2022 on the visual system).
Over the years some minor variants of the columnar model were published, often without discussing in detail their differences or similarities with Herrick’s original 1910 proposal. The most recent variant of this model appears in Swanson (2012). In general, columnar-based models are characterized by arbitrary (causally unexplained) postulation of a forebrain axis that ends in the telencephalon (this is a paired vesicle; the exact ending place therein of the postulated axis is not indicated, nor do we know developmental or molecular reasons for this assumption, rather the contrary; Puelles and Rubenstein 2015). The natural incurvations of the embryonic neural tube, notably the cephalic flexure, are largely not taken into account to determine what is longitudinal, that is, columnar. The adult columns are officially delimited by ventricular sulci said to run parallel to the nondescript axis, though these variable landmarks are not easily visualized, particularly in embryos. The columns are thus assumed to be delimited by supposedly longitudinal sulci, though various authors have stressed that these -as mapped- seem to run orthogonal to the bent natural axis. The brain’s length dimension and the ‘longitudinal’ nature of its forebrain columns is thus a very vague concept in this model. In contrast, the longitudinal nature of the brainstem columns is not problematic, representing the most valid contribution of the columnar approach. Even in this case, the developmental formation of the brainstem columns has been qualified substantially by the modern experimental and molecular rediscovery of neuromeres (transverse, rather than longitudinal, structural units).
The fundamental practical problem of the columnar model is that its columnar units represent late histogenetic phenomena defined as originating at late embryonic stages (after the nerve connections are established). As a result, columnar interpretations leave aside practically all developmental considerations on the early neural tube (patterning, differentiation, cell migrations). The columnar model, designed by Herrick for providing adult functional implications, lacks any factual underpinning dealing with causal processes active during early developmental stages (e.g., molecular positional patterning, proliferation patterns, early bulging of vesicles or neuromeres, neurogenetic patterns, cell migration, areal regionalization, stratification and layering of neurons, axonal navigation, synaptogenesis). This rather outdated model thus essentially cannot deal with how the brain is patterned (regionalized) and constructed as regards acquiring its structural and functional complexity. Importantly, it deals with too few brain parts, some of them badly defined. The columnar model simply does not contemplate at all how a supposedly initially homogeneous column is transformed into a non-homogeneous adult column (or which are the functional implications of a thoroughly compartmented column such as the modern hypothalamus, for instance). Given that columnar model-based theory stops at the level of the entire column and its possible overall functional relationship with sensory-motor or sympathetic-parasympathetic peripheral connections in the body, it traditionally does not admit interpretations based on the finer constituent histogenetic phenomena occurring inside the columns. Note there are only a few columns and we already know about at least 5000 different subregional parts in the mouse brain, as stated above. The columnar model is therefore extremely simplistic as regards both subjacent developmental causal processes and functional parts. This implies that the functional implications deduced by use of the columnar model also must be simplistic (for instance, the widespread notion that basal ganglia are merely motor centers is simplistic). In particular, given that all postulated columns are either sensory, motor or autonomic regulatory (sympathetic or parasympathetic control of peripheral autonomously functioning ganglia), it surely must be doubted that all functions performed by the brain belong necessarily to these categories (e.g., compare with evaluation of experience in the amygdala, or the largely unknown function of the septum).
In the present scenario we have the problem that the outdated and excessively simplistic columnar model still continues to be used dogmatically in introductory texts (without being acknowledged by name), but it has ceased to be scientifically useful since at least some 50 years now, particularly with regard to the numerous molecular, causal, and complex functional data accrued in this period and the modern appreciation that by the number of distinctly populated parts, the brain is much more complex than was appreciated by Herrick in 1910. The columnar model therefore has become by its widespread implantation a cryptic mental superstructure blocking perception of more complex patterns. It is cryptic because most modern users of this model are not aware of its existence, nor of its outdated assumptions. Scientists that take this model as providing realistic knowledge about the brain are often blinded to the molecular-level fine phenomena of brain construction and function, which are not organized in a columnar fashion. The era of experimental, molecular, and genetic embryologic approaches, which is now some 50 years old, has made it clear that models of the brain are no longer particularly meaningful or useful if they are not based on developmental molecular causal phenomena. They must allow us to understand from the beginning how the brain is constructed during development (this construction can go wrong in many directions, sometimes giving rise to subtle malformative consequences having functional impact, such as the Down or Asperger syndromes).
The main developmental models available
The oldest brain models recorded in the literature reflect conclusions of technically imperfect anatomic dissection of adult brains, either in animals or in the human. They were consequently very tentative when not speculative as regards the brain parts thought to exist, their relationships, or the potential functions attached to them. For instance, until experimental anatomic and physiological study of connections in the brain was possible, it was frequent to postulate what later was discovered to be the wrong direction of signal transmission in many brain tracts (e.g., false ascending signals in the pyramidal tract towards the cortex and/or the corpus callosum, or efferent signals from the brain into the eye through the optic tracts). The plurimillenial long first historical epoch based largely on anatomic dissections led to a second epoch of clarifying neurohistological microscopic work after the development of cellular theory and the advance of microscopy and staining methods. This line of analysis resolved a number of classic problems and was soon complemented by comparative neurobiology, neurophysiology, and neuroembryology (first descriptive and then experimental). Arrival of Darwin’s theory of evolution in 1858, with subsequent developments in genetics, allowed to gradually come to understand that evolutive steps are produced by subtle genomic changes translated by embryos into modified brain forms via molecularly regulated mechanisms of development. Our understanding of highly evolved brains such as the human one thus depends significantly on our knowledge of how it develops.
Embryological studies performed along the second half of the 19
th century produced at least two developmental brain models, which can be seen as having introduced complementary concepts that later were integrated into a synthetic concept: one of the initial notions was the ‘neuromeric model’, which considered
transversal developmental units called ‘neuromeres’ (divided according to the major ‘vesicles’ where they appeared as prosomeres (forebrain), mesomeres (midbrain), rhombomeres (hindbrain) and myelomeres (spinal cord) (see
Figure 1). The other notion was represented by the ‘longitudinal dorsoventral zonal model’, also known as ‘His’s model’, which considered
longitudinal developmental units, called ‘plates’ or ‘zones’ (the floor, basal, alar, and roof plates if His 1893, 1895, 1904).
The neuromeric model
The ‘neuromeric model’ centered attention on the early existence of series of transverse outpouchings (vesicles) in the lateral wall of the embryonic neural tube, which were delimited by transverse constrictions (producing inner ridges in the ventricular relief). These pouches generate an apparent ‘serial segmentation’ of the neural primordium, organized orthogonally to its length axis. Some of these outpouchings in the brainstem and all of them in the spinal cord were clearly related to the root of given cranial or spinal nerves. These neural outpouchings were eventually christened ‘neuromeres’ by Orr 1887 (note the Greek suffix ‘mere’ implies a ‘repeated partition’ and relates to the descriptor ‘metameric’, which refers to serially repeated units or partitions (it is understood that such repeated units each show an identical structural organization, in the case of neuromeres the shared floor, basal, alar and roof plates). Neuromeres are thus metameric units, likewise as somites, branchial arches, vertebrae, ribs, etc…). Consistent numbering of the neuromeres was a problem for a time, due to technical problems (irregular embryonic series and delayed fixation, which introduced postmortem fixation artefacts). However, these problems were gradually resolved, leading to an important general review (von Kupffer 1906 and Ziehen 1906; the latter author wrote the missing chapter on mammals after the death of von Kupffer), covering what seemed comparable neuromeric phenomena in all vertebrate lineages. This crucial work written in German was never translated into English, thus limiting considerably its ulterior diffusion. Note it was contemporaneous with Herrick (1910).
According to the neuromeric model, the neural tube registers at early embryonic stages a serial transversal vesiculation along its axial dimension, establishing a number of repeated metameric units called neuromeres, out of which the adult brain is built, with cryptic maintenance of the early interneuromeric boundaries. In the molecular era (after the nineteen eighties) this concept was enriched by recognition of the fact that each neuromere is patterned differentially to have a singular molecular profile, that is, to express a partly shared but in fact unique set of developmental genes, which determines subsequently after various molecularly guided regionalization steps the particular fate of each neuromere within the brain. There are 20 cerebral neuromeres (
Figure 1), plus variable numbers of spinal neuromeres in different species (for instance, many more spinal neuromeres in snakes than in frogs; there are as many as spinal nerves, according to recent counts).
Note the old authors usually counted less cerebral neuromeres, because they only counted those showing well delimited bulges, but modernly a number of cryptic neuromeres were identified by gene mapping methods (Tomás-Roca et al. 2016); these do not bulge out, but are still comparable with standard bulging units in their molecular and developmental properties. The bulging seems to be due merely to an eventual disproportion between maximal central proliferation versus slower proliferation at the constricted limits. This is however a local and variable consequence of neuromery, not strictly needed for the formation (molecular specification) of a neuromere. Modernly we learned that what establishes the neuromeres and their differential fates is their unique molecular profile, irrespective of the overall form, with more proliferative central parts bulging or not. Serial neuromery formed initially part of an ampler theory on head segmentation phenomena (considering also so-called head or cranial vertebrae, metameric cranial nerves, blood vessels, and somites, and metameric branchial arches). This theory reached its acme of interest at the end of the nineteenth century but was gradually left aside afterwards, jointly with the ‘neuromeric brain model’, roughly coinciding with Herrick’s 1910 publication of his ‘columnar model’ (see however a recent report on blood vessels and neuromeres; Puelles et al. 2019).
A criticism addressed at that time to the neuromeric model was that the neuromeres were not known to have any functional implications. Moreover, their visibility as transverse bulges of the brain diminished with time, so that they seemed to become effaced as the neural wall differentiated and became functional (that is when the longitudinal columns formed instead). The columns were thus concluded to overlie and efface the neuromeres (a doubtful notion; the supposed mechanism of deletion was never demonstrated). This notion actually turned out to be false, since we can now visualize perfectly neuromere-derived domains in the adult mouse brain with transgenic labeling methods (e.g.,
Figure 2 and
Figure 3). However, at the time, no adult functional consequences seemed to relate to the early neuromeric repetitive units. The subsequent ascent in popularity of the columnar model led to practical disappearance of the notion of ‘neuromere’ in the literature. Only a few occasional authors continued to report neuromeric descriptions; see recent review and reappraisal of modern neuromeric literature in Amat et al. (2022).
His’s model
A second developmental brain model was conceived by the neuroembryologist. The Swiss neuroembryologist Wilhelm His discovered the early subdivision of the developing human brain into primary longitudinal ‘floor, basal, alar, and roof plates’, each extending in parallel along the whole neural tube, and adapting to its morphogenetic axial bends (His 1893, 1895, 1904). This is usually known as ‘His’s model’, but it might also be described as the ‘dorsoventral longitudinal zonal model’, since the four longitudinal plates of His are stacked dorsoventrally (roof to floor). This model ascribed neurogenesis (neuronal production) mainly to the basal (ventral) and alar (dorsal) plates. W.His noted that the precociously differentiating basal plates thicken out early on, forming a longitudinal bulge in the ventricular (inner) relief of the brain wall. This bulge is limited by a longitudinal alar-basal sulcus, known as the sulcus limitans of His. The latter can be followed across the neural tube flexures to a rostral end in the neighborhood of the optic chiasma (implying an axial end in the hypothalamus, rather than in the telencephalon, as was subsequently held arbitrarily in the columnar model; Herrick never discussed the contradiction between his arbitrary forebrain length axis and the developmentally well-supported and very well-known one of His). These longitudinal plates of His agreed with the underlying notochord, which was later shown to induce the floor plate (and indirectly, interactively with roof plate signals, the other two parallel longitudinal zones).The basal plate (containing the motor columns) essentially produces neurons involved functionally in motor mechanisms (motoneurons and associated interneurons plus reticular motor elements), whereas the later born neurons of the alar plate (forming the sensory neuronal columns) receive all sensory nerve inputs and thus are functionally related to initial sensory analysis. This functional characterization of the primary longitudinal plates of His was not far removed from the notion of alar-basal sensorimotor reflex circuits conceived by physiologists (e.g., Sherrington, 1906).
The longitudinal plates of His reflected early differential fate specification (patterning) of the basal and alar plate derivatives (as well as of the floor and roof ones). It was relatively consistent with the functional notions of the columnar model for the brainstem and spinal cord, since the columnar authors merely divided the alar plate into viscerosensitive and somatosensitive columns, and the basal plate similarly into visceromotor and somatomotor columns. However, his’s model discrepated with columnar axial notions with its postulated (and demonstrated) rostral end of the longitudinal zones in the hypothalamus (the telencephalon was seen in His’s model as a secondary dorsal outgrowth of the hypothalamus; see recent review in Ruiz-Cabrera et al. 2023). This implied different morphologic, causal and functional interpretations of the forebrain areas (not so for the brainstem). His’s model did survive in embryologic and anatomic textbooks after the ascent of the columnar model, insofar as its functional implications were consistent in the brainstem with those of the columnar model. The authors following the predominant columnar school nevertheless usually ignored His’s model as regards its different forebrain axial assumptions. However, its solid developmental basis was the only one available (the columnar model having no embryology at the beginning; Kuhlenbeck 1927, 1973 provided that missing aspect a bit later in a somewhat opinionated or tendentious way). In any case, His’s model finally resulted supported by modern gene expression data and experimental patterning studies, once these arrived in the late nineteen eighties and nineties.
The major problem of His’s model subdivision of the dorsoventral dimension of the neural tube into four longitudinal zonal units is that it in principle does not contemplate the embryonic neuromeric phenomena that serially divide the anteroposterior axial dimension of the whole neural tube. His (1893, 1895, 1904) did postulate a few roughly defined anteroposterior transverse subdivisions (but these only agree in one neuromere with the modern list of 20 neuromeres). As a result of this weak point, His’s model similarly as the columnar model is incapable of explaining the fine regionalization phenomena occurring within the longitudinal zones or plates due to early neuromeric transverse partition phenomena, which turned out to be underpinned by solid molecular data. These transverse units have reemerged importantly during the current molecular/genetic epoch of neuroscience (these last 50 years or so).
The Nordic synthetic school of neuromery
An alternative developmental brain model unifying purposefully both the transverse neuromeric and the longitudinal zonal approaches of the 19th century emerged during the early 20th century in the hands of the so-called Nordic school (Palmgren 1921; Rendahl 1924; Bergquist and Källén, 1954; Vaage 1969, 1973; Gribnau and Geijsberts 1985). The first three cited authors (P, R, B) were pupils of the Swedish neurohistologist Niels Holmgren, who apparently developed the logical notion of a pattern of checkerboard-like areas of the brain wall that are sharply delimited in the neural tube wall by the intersection of the series of transverse neuromeric limits with the longitudinal zonal boundaries of His’s dorsoventral zones (using also His’s ‘natural’ longitudinal axis). The other authors were independent followers of that school in Sweden (Källén), Denmark (Vaage) and Holland (Gribnau and Geijsberts). According to what I call here their ‘synthetic neuromeric’ model each neuromeric unit would be divided dorsoventrally into floor, basal, alar, and roof histogenetic areas. Such square intersection areas, studied by these authors with cytoarchitectonic methods and occasional histochemistry, were modernly corroborated with molecular markers, giving rise to the updated molecularly based ‘prosomeric model’ of Puelles et al. (1987) and Puelles and Rubenstein (1993, 2003, 2015).
The square domains were first identified as ‘migration areas’ (Bergquist and Källén 1954), referring to their central maxima of mitotic cells and major central production and radial migration of newborn neurons. Later, they were described as ‘radial histogenetic areas’ (Puelles et al 1987), a term emphasizing the overall radial organization of its collection of ventricular progenitor cells plus the derived subset of neurons and glia cells born within each area. More recently a third name was proposed, that of ‘fundamental morphological units’ (FMUs of Nieuwenhuys and Puelles 2016), thus identifying them as the fundamental developmental brain components, whose individual histogenesis and morphogenesis summed up jointly produces the complex structure of the mature brain. 20 cerebral neuromeres divided each into 4 FMUs makes a total of 80 FMUs as the basic building blocks of the brain. It is clear that in order to reach 5000 parts in the adult, several rounds of further regionalization of these 80 FMUs need to be considered. However, a model encompassing 80 basic parts may be handled profitably by students of the brain.
The neuroepithelium of each FMU differentiates secondarily via added neurogenesis and gliogenesis into ventricular, mantle, and marginal zones parallel to the ventricular and pial surfaces. The ventricular zone contains the somata of the proliferating subpopulation of ventricular progenitor cells. It presents a variable thickness during development as its activity first increases to a maximum and then decreases, finishing as the monocellular ependymal lining of the ventricle (still capable of regeneration after a lesion). The mature mantle zone is the thickest of each FMU and contains most newborn neurons and glia cells, and later, after axogenesis (axonal outgrowth) and axonal navigation and fasciculation occur, displays diverse more or less organized cellular or fiber-rich strata. At least periventricular, intermediate, and superficial strata are distinguished in most places of the mature brain in a pattern characteristic of each FMU. The superficial neuronal stratum in its turn may form a multi-laminar cortical structure. This stratified differentiation of the mantle is one way for additional regionalization (80 FMUs x 3 main neuronal strata = 240 pronuclei). Each pronucleus may later subdivide into several smaller nuclei or layers.
Independently, each FMU may continue registering molecular signaling processes leading to subdivisional patterning of the FMU area, generating different so-called ‘microzones’ along the dorsoventral or anteroposterior dimensions. Each of these behaves as a mini-unit that may produce one specific cell population, or, sequentially, various specific types of neurons. This final process is the one that elevates the number of recognizable small brain parts represented by single cell types to the highest numbers detected recently. Each basic FMU is therefore a stratified composite of microzones that results in many combined unique cell types. Individual postmitotic neuronal types may move variously within the FMU, becoming roughly intermixed with other types, or forming a compact assembly of a single type. Eventually groupings of several cell types may aggregate as characteristic layers of corticoid assemblies (this is for example how the layered spinal dorsal horn is produced, with diverse neuron types each originated in different microzonal domains).
The existence of numerous examples of tangential migration in the developing brain implies that some neuronal populations originated at one specific FMU may spread out via active tangential migration (with or without preceding axonal outgrowth) to another FMU or several other FMUs (close or distant). There the migrated neurons participate in specific local circuitry according to the molecularly regulated adhesive selectivity of each neuronal type. Defining completely the functional role of a FMU is thus a complex task, needing to distinguish its intrinsic neuronal constituents (locally produced neuron types) from those originated there but migrated elsewhere (emigrated neurons) and those it receives as immigrants from other FMUs (immigrant neurons). This diversification of neuronal types is what explains that the 80 FMUs produce finally over 5000 neuron types throughout the brain (a promedium of 6 neuron types pro FMU).
The functional properties of the His’s longitudinal zones (motor versus sensory functions) and of the neuromeres (e.g., reflex or other dorsoventral modular integration of sensorimotor response mechanisms at different segmental levels) would accordingly be distributed with different degrees of specificity over the ‘fundamental morphological units’ or FMUs (Nieuwenhuys and Puelles 2016). Note each FMU is initially molecularly unique, having a peculiar molecular profile, with a number of shared genetic determinants and a set of non-shared differential genes (normally several hundreds of activated or repressed genes, with their eventual downstream cascades). The FMUs may thus express both some common histogenetic properties with adjacent units -e.g., those useful for forming a plurisegmental column, that is, attracting a particular subtype of afferent nerve fiber-, as well as some unique singular properties differentiating its components from all others. The finer levels of neuromeric regionalization occur in the so-called microzones. Their occurrence reveals that the initially molecularly unitary FMU later becomes subdivided in its specific pattern of microzones (since the latter are also molecularly unique). They might be called as well microFMUs.
More complex pathways and functions would be explained via selective connections, and higher development and diversification of collaborating cell populations over evolutionary time, with emergence and new functional role adoption of centers such as the cerebellum and the telencephalic cortex, subpallium, septum, and amygdala. The theory of ‘fundamental morphologic units’ implying differentially fated neuromere-derived subareas of the embryonic neural tube wall already allows to explain many detailed aspects of adult structure and function within columnar macrodomains on the basis of the selective properties manifested by modular columnar subunits belonging to different neuromeres and neuronal subpopulations residing within the diversely stratified FMU derivatives of each neuromeric module. However, the early neurohistologic notions of the mid-fifties could not be developed in detail until advances occurred in molecular biology and genetics, and resulting developmental patterning studies, which could underpin more precisely
how the FMUs got their respective fates specified and subsequently developed their different molecular profiles step by step into different cellular and histic fates (nuclei, layers, etc…; see
Figure 2 and
Figure 3). Detailed functional analysis will need to wait until the classic columnar functions are redefined in terms of the FMUs, their internal circuitry, and their external connective properties.
The earliest modern response to these questions arose in the recent early nineties, once developmental genes started to be mapped, first in Drosophila (review in Alexander 1992) and later in vertebrate models such as the mouse (or zebrafish, frog, chick, etc…; e.g., Kaufman 1993, or Davidson 2001).
The birth of the molecular prosomeric model
A new molecularly-based synthetic neuromeric brain model known as the ‘prosomeric model’ was produced, which was essentially a variously updated and technically perfected molecular expansion of the notions previously advanced by the ‘Nordic synthetic neuromeric model’ of the twenties-eighties, with a shared basis on both the original old ‘neuromeric model’ and ‘His’s model’ of the 19
th century. The modern ‘prosomeric model’ was developed in progressive stages in continuing collaboration by the Puelles lab in Murcia, Spain and the Rubenstein lab in San Francisco, California, starting in 1991. A preliminary chick version of such a synthetic molecular model, though still lacking gene data (Puelles et al. 1987) was based on whole-mount histochemical mappings of acetylcholinesterase-positive young neurons in the developing chick brain (performed between 1977 and 1987). The results obtained on the heterochronic topography of neurogenetically advanced or retarded areas were inconsistent with the reported homogeneity of the hypothetic diencephalic columns formed across effaced interneuromeric limits, as described by Kuhlenbeck (1927, 1973). These mappings of AChE-positive young neurons revealed instead the existence of cryptic persistent transverse neuromeric boundaries across the supposedly homogeneous columns. It was subsequently shown that such neurogenetic areal limits coincide with limits of neuromeric gene expression patterns (
Figure 2 and
Figure 3). Amat et al. (2022) recently reexamined in retrospect (after 30 years) these and other AChE neurogenetic chick results (comparable lizard and rat embryo data), and partly reinterpreted them in the light of more recent data.
Rubenstein and Puelles started to collaborate after a casual encounter at the 1991 Anaheim meeting of the American Society of Neuroscience, which led to discussing the advantages of alternative brain models for morphologically meaningful morphologic interpretation of gene expression patterns. They started to work on the neuromeric interpretation of diencephalic and telencephalic developmental gene patterns in mouse embryos, leading subsequently to several high impact publications presenting the mouse ‘prosomeric model’ (e.g., Bulfone et al. 1993; Puelles and Rubenstein 1993; Rubenstein et al. 1994, etc.; the model was subsequently revised and updated in Puelles and Rubenstein 2003; Puelles et al. 2012, Puelles and Rubenstein 2015, and Puelles 2018; Puelles 2021 provided an account of how this model emerged).
The model was thus updated several times in recent years, largely as a result of improvements in the causal (molecular or experimental) definition of the longitudinal dimension of the forebrain, as manifested in molecular and fate-mapping studies of the rostral ends of the floor, basal, alar, and roof plates, as well as of the alar-basal boundary. Puelles et al. (2012) added the new concept of a transverse ‘acroterminal area’, conceived as the rostromedian end of the forebrain (this hypothalamic area -already suggested by His 1893, 1895- connects the rostral end of the floor with the rostral end of the roof plate across the intervening fused rostral ends of the basal and alar plates; review in Puelles and Rubenstein 2015; Puelles 2018; Amat et al. 2022). Clarification and causal molecular correlation of the brain’s axial dimension read at different dorsoventral positions introduced more precision than had been possible before in the molecular and anatomic definition of the interneuromeric boundaries (see also Puelles and Rubenstein 2003; Puelles 2003, 2017, 2018).
Between 2008 and 2011 the prosomeric model (
Figure 1) was incorporated by L.P. in the reference atlas drawings he developed for the Allen Developing Mouse Brain Atlas (Allen Institute for Brain Science; Seattle, WA; developingmouse.brain-map.org; this is a public database with some 4000 developmental genes mapped across 4 prenatal and 3 postnatal stages plus the adult brain; see Thompson et al. 2014). The Developing Mouse Brain Ontology developed by L.P. jointly with this project elicited 2500 identifiable embryonic sites in each halve of the mouse brain, as was mentioned above (note this ontology considers not only transverse and longitudinal divisions, but also radial ones stratified in the mantle, a feature of the more advanced stages of histogenetic development). The Allen Developing Mouse Brain Atlas has become since a substantial aid for developmental brain studies around the globe.
In essence, the ‘prosomeric model’ holds to the synthetic view that the primary developmental units (fate units) of the brain primordium are represented by the checkerboard-like intersections between the anteroposterior series of transverse neuromeres and the dorsoventral series of His’s longitudinal zones, defining what may be indistinctly called primary migration areas, radial histogenetic areas, or fundamental morphogenetic units (FMUs). This means that each neuromere produces its own set of primary floor, basal, alar, and roof plate areas, which may show either invariant (shared) or differential characteristics when corresponding portions of neighboring neuromeres are compared (
Figure 2 and
Figure 3). Each primary area acquires a characteristic molecular profile via anteroposterior and dorsoventral molecular patterning of the progenitor neuroepithelial cells (corroborated in many cases by published gene mappings). Importantly, each of these primary or fundamental structural units may be further regionalized secondarily into a number of smaller microzonal areas or
microzones. The latter are neuroepithelial partitions within the primary FMUs that represent progenitor areas for
specific neuronal populations (i.e., they represent true minimal units of brain structure) and each of them shows likewise a retarded differential molecular specification. Once the early molecular patterning phase occurring at the level of neural progenitors is settled (activating given genes and repressing others in a combinatorial unique positional code), the resulting molecular profiles start to operate differentially at each locus (by activating different gene cascades, etc.). Networked downstream final genes produce synthesis of multiple coded proteins and this leads to early particular phenomena of patterned proliferative growth and cellular differentiation (neurogenesis, gliogenesis), in a long developmental process. The latter effects are expanded by various other special molecular mechanisms in different FMUs, that jointly lead to the structural morphogenesis of each particular site (bigger or smaller) of the adult brain, with permanent conservation of the neuromeric complexes and their transverse limits (
Figure 2 and
Figure 3).
How many unitary organogenetic parts do we have then in the brain, according to this model? Well, we start early on with the single neural plate, but this soon starts to show incipient patterning of its dorsoventral zones (4 zones) and anteroposterior neuromeric subdivisions (20 neuromeres in the forebrain and hindbrain, plus the acroterminal area, and the likewise segmented but species-variable spinal cord). This makes a minimum of 4x20=80 primary structural intersected blocks (note we are leaving out the spinal cord neuromeres, there are over 30 of them in Man). We expect a substantially higher number of definitive microzones and individual mantle strata or cortical layers. We ignore yet whether all primary FMUs subdivide into a similar pattern of microzones (but this does not seem to occur according to present partial data). At spinal and hindbrain levels, where existing data are more abundant and precise, there seem to be about 10-11 dorsoventral microzones per neuromere. Generalizing tentatively this pattern to the forebrain and hindbrain we may have up to 20 neuromeres x10 microzones =200 different dorsoventral progenitor microzones in the brain. Each of them is going to produce via developmental histogenesis and morphogenesis a separate and molecularly (typologically) distinct neuronal population of the final brain anatomy (whose number ascends presently to some 5300 (Yao et al. 2023). We know in addition that some neuromeric alar FMUs also become subdivided into anteroposterior microzones (see Ferran et al. 2009; Puelles et al. 2020), but we also ignore yet how general this pattern is.
In any case, we cannot have a complete brain without the whole set of developing units performing their individual morphogenetic roles. The different neuronal types produced in the microzones may combine into adult nuclei or layered cortical structures (both of which usually have mixed subpopulations). Varied adhesive properties will guide the axons of the projection neurons as well as those of interneurons across their respective navigation patterns, to reach their more or less distant targets. The rough calculations above suggest that the sum of all microzones probably produces several thousand neuronal types, as has been corroborated transcriptomically, part of which are recombined topographically by tangential migrations. Individual FMUs and microzones will each grow diversely, so that some of them may remain rather small (particularly if they lose many neurons by migration), whereas others will grow more, and still others may grow enormously (and probably produce additional tertiary or quaternary subdivisions, as happens in the cerebral cortex).
General functional implications of the prosomeric brain model
I’ll address here only non-conventional functional properties arising from the postulated neuromeric subdivision of the brain of vertebrates. These properties have been only exceptionally considered in the physiological literature, but interest in them is increasing in recent years.
The 20 cerebral neuromeres contemplated in the prosomeric model provide numerous functional possibilities due to both their shared structural and cell typological aspects joined to their additional unique individual differential aspects. The following analysis applies as well to the spinal cord, which was always thought to have a metameric segmental structure, due to the obvious presence of a spinal nerve pair in each neuromeric unit. Clinicians have been aware for a long time of the segmental significance of objective dermatome and myotome bands in the body of their patients, serving to determine the spinal level of their injuries.
Precisely, one of the shared functional aspects of nerves, both afferent and efferent, is how they distribute their peripheral terminals in the body in a strict somatotopic pattern, forming topologically transverse bands called dermatomes in the skin and myotomes in the muscles. Interestingly, successive adjacent nerves always mix their terminals to a degree. This feature is held to aid the central tying together of the data vehiculated by multiple nerves. When we hold a glass of water in our hand, we simultaneously obtain hundreds of different somatosensory signals running through diverse nerves. Their collective input is collected and analyzed centrally, where our unified perception of the object we are handling arises.
Curiously, rather than targeting a point-like locus in a central map of the body, most afferent nerve fibers bringing in information from a particular sensory field distribute it to an elongated receptive field of secondary neurons distributed over a number of neuromeric units. Indeed, in the spinal cord and brainstem, afferent sensory nerve roots bifurcate into ascending and descending branches as they enter the brain. The descending branches connect with postsynaptic neurons in two segments below their level of entrance, whereas the ascending ones do the same along four overlying segments. Each segment accordingly receives input from 6 nerves, and thus information from 6 dermatome loci. The next segments above or below it will share 5 of these inputs; each will differ only by one input among 6, and this pattern is reproduced segment by segment along the whole representation of the body. The slight mixing of peripheral origins of the inputs that the terminals of one nerve obtain in a dermatome with those spread out in the immediate dermatomes is multiplied by 6 centrally. This systematic organization clearly forces the existence of secondary computing circuitry reproduced (shared) in all segments for detecting what sort of stationary or moving stimulus is impacting their part of the body.
As we see, the neighboring neuromeric units involved in any perceptive task share most of the data they get from 6 successive nerves (5/6), and differ uniquely with adjacent neuromeric units by one differential nerve input added above or below within the set of its 6 afferent nerves. For many purposes, any segment probably can do most of what its neighbors do, and can thus eventually substitute for them with little loss of information. In the dorsal horn module of each spinal segment, a modular part of the larger plurisegmental columnar dorsal horn structure that receives the trunk and limb somatosensory signals, the 6 sets of nerve terminals are represented in somatotopic order along a mediolateral distribution. Each modular segment thus has its own small partial map of the somatic dermatomal periphery attended by the 6 potentially similar or different inputs it receives. The dorsal horn also disposes of abundant interneurons and both the intrasegmental and intersegmental circuitry needed for cross-correlating the different inputs across the series of partial maps and filtering out a conclusive stimulation pattern for the projection neurons which will relay signals to the next, more integrative, analytic part of the central nervous system, eventually the thalamus, where a cohesive central representation of the entire body surface can be obtained in an analogous summatory and internally cross-correlated way.
Such patterns of partial plurisegmental central analysis of peripheral somatosensory nerve inputs occurs as well in the brainstem via the trigeminal, facial, glossopharyngeal and vagus afferent fibers. The principal somatosensory nerve, the trigeminus, displays a small ascending root distributing to its main sensory nucleus, restricted to rhombomere 2, and a long descending root that distributes terminals to the descending trigeminal sensory column along the rest of the more caudal brainstem (pontine, retropontine and medullary levels; rhombomeres 3-11), which additionally penetrates into the cervical spinal cord, reaching apparently down to its 6th segment, intermixing face data with neck data. Each dot-like signal originating from any place in the face, eyes, or mouth is thus copied to redundant circuitry in 16 successive segments (10 rhombencephalic, 6 spinal) across brainstem and adjoining spinal cord! The three peripheral branches of the trigeminal nerve (ophthalmic, maxilar, mandibular) are each represented by terminals in dorsoventral somatotopic order throughout this extent. The corresponding signals are thus computed redundantly in all these segmental units. In this case we know that the different rhombomeres have each a differential molecular profile due to early anteroposterior patterning (Hox genes, gradients, etc…). This causes that the neuronal types produced at each unit may be at least in part qualitatively different, so that the modular analytic computers that receive the 16 copies of trigeminal sensory signals each are subtly different! Each module may attend to different variables or combinations of variables of the signals received, and probably generates via unique aspects of intrinsic microcircuitry distinct characteristic output signals capable of targeting different postsynaptic sites in the thalamus. The richness of varied analysis thus made possible by a neuromeric structure of the primary sensory inputs stands out.
Less information is available about the mode of distribution of visceral afferent signals, but the solitary tract is composed of primary ascending and descending afferent fibers from several cranial nerves (facial, glossopharyngeal and vagus) and we know that the column of the solitary tract is also a plurisegmental entity (distributed across rhombomeres 7-11) and has specialized functions in some of its modules (e.g., gustatory analysis at its rostralmost module).
The cochlear sensory column extends over rhombomeres 2-6. We know that auditory afferent fibers bifurcate into ascending and descending branches that synapse in a tonotopic redundant pattern over all these segmental units. It is also known that different types of neurons are characteristic of each of the sensory cochlear modules (e.g., bushy, stellate, and octopus cells), and each cell type produces a characteristic output signal addressed to a particular hierarchically superior target. The same may be said of the vestibular sensory column, which extends across rhombomeres 2 to 10. The vestibular afferents also bifurcate into ascending and descending branches and connect in an ordered redundant way with all these modules, where specialized sorts of neurons are contacted, and a variety of output pathways are generated.
The optic tract distributes retinal output signals of several subtypes (depending on the ganglion cell types that originate them) to retinorecipient centers distributed over hypothalamic, diencephalic and midbrain prosomeres (hp1-2; dp1-3; m1; review in Puelles 2022). Some kinds of functionally dedicated retinal output go to dorsoventrally or anteroposteriorly different target neuropiles (e.g., the signals of melanophotoreceptor cells to the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus – at the hp2 segment- or the olivary pretectal nucleus -dp1). Some other retinal fibers (note all of them are descending, since the eyes and optic chiasm lie at the rostralmost acroterminal domain of the forebrain) distribute terminals to several prosomeres, and establish multiple separate retinotopically organized synaptic fields having differential analysis properties and projection targets.
The olfactory input is peculiar in many ways. All its primary projections coming from the sensory cells in the nasal olfactory placodes fall into a single synaptic target which forms a specialized part (the olfactory bulb) of a specialized part (the telencephalon) of a single hypothalamo-telencephalic prosomere (hp1). Actually, the whole olfactory pathway into cortical analyzers (signals only reach the thalamus collaterally, in the dp2 prosomere) is also characterized by redundant projection of the same signal to different specialized telencephalic olfactory areas, each of which performs its particular computation and projects to its more or less unique targets, in this single case strictly without recourse to multiple neuromeres.
We have thus seen that developmental serial segmentation produces repeated modules with some degree of differential molecular specification (and inner secondary regionalization), together with some degree of sharedness in the connective properties needed to receive in a particular topologic order signals from diverse types of afferent channels vehiculated by sensory nerves. The resulting neuromeric structure is modular and redundant in various ways, but achieves nevertheless thanks to the accompanying molecular differentiation of variant sorts of neurons in each module, the capacity to compute differently the copied messages received, producing peculiar outputs serving different purposes. A single sensory receiving center for each modality would not be able to extract as much biologically relevant information as the series of variant neuromeric modules does. Moreover, the existence of various modules which are partially similar in their analytic properties establishes a reserve for cases of punctual lesion or functional failure of one module, whose essential functions possibly can be saved by an increase in the functionality of its companions.
What about the motor centers? Are they also organized into redundant segmental modular entities? Both cranial and spinal motoneurons have been mapped to aggregates (nuclei) collecting all neurons that innervate specific muscles. Some of these aggregates, particularly in the spinal cord, reportedly extend across several segments. Even though some cranial nerves (e.g., the trigeminus, or the vagus and the hypoglossus) also vehiculate motor axons from motoneurons lying in different neuromeres, generally these target specifically different muscles with characteristic functions (e.g., all trigeminal motoneurons in rhombomere 2 collaborate to close the mouth -the mandibles- with more or less force, using various muscles; in contrast, those sitting in rhombomere 3 apparently serve the easier task of opening the mouth, also with several muscles). In any case, the innervated muscle fibers are aligned across diverse muscles within myotome bands, which is a peripheral segmental pattern.
Most movements imply the activation of multiple muscles, which may change as the task proceeds in a quite dynamic pattern. The important feature is the activation of a particular proportion of the fibers of particular muscles (these will automatically alternate activity with other fibers of the same muscle in similar proportion to obtain transient repose; similarly, some muscles partly substitute for others across the same articulation, at least part of the time, when lesions or significant tiredness occur). Often the motoneurons innervating the same muscle are mutually connected via electric junctions; these cause that they discharge in a synchronized way, at least above a certain signaling threshold. Here, again, we have the junction of several actors -now the peripheral effectors, the muscles- that share a generic functional capacity at a particular articulation, but also have particular specialty or variant properties (e.g., various angles of action) that are conveniently used (chosen) when it is needed
Apparently, the striatum is an important telencephalic center for the selection, dimensional organization, and heterochronic activation of the different muscles involved in an action, whose inhibitory output is vehiculated across the likewise inhibitory pallidum and the excitatory thalamus to the cortical motor pathway (see below). The descending pyramidal fibers apparently give out collaterals at different segmental (neuromeric) levels and along the whole spinal cord, allowing the cortex to act freely upon the entire keyboard of segmental motoneuron groups.
It is not yet clear precisely how the central nervous system ‘thinks’ about the distribution of the action intended across the segments in which the multiple relevant motoneurons are placed and the myotome units where the final targets, the individual muscle fibers, are found (all this being in equilibrium with the entire acting body and the outside world).
In an initial phase, one presumes that the premotor cortex conceives and selects the action to be performed in a broad context wherein many other alternative actions are conceivable; this generates a sketchy draft of a plan (e.g., you think of ‘going out for a walk’, without counting and planning the steps or corners implied). This sketch apparently passes via corticostriatal projections through the striatum, which selects a more detailed plan involving specific muscular units in a precise dimensional context (are we moving only a finger, a limb, or the entire body?) and ads planned exertion of force by each unit over time, according to apparent cortically envisioned necessities and helped by recorded memories of previous similar actions (non-practiced actions are more difficult to plan and are uncertain of success). The striatal updated action plan passes to the thalamus translated into a set of disinhibition signals via the striato-pallidal pathway, modulated by parallel excitatory signals from the subthalamic nucleus. The disinhibited thalamic neurons relay excitatory trains of signals that modulate the current output of the primary motor cortex, itself pre-stimulated by the previous premotor plan. The thalamus also relays relevant vestibular, visual, auditory, tactile and propioceptive sensory inputs serving to orient the action in the world and the body, as well as crucial standing cerebellar corrections of the precision with which the plan in course achieves its objective (this is a feedback mechanism activated by collateral copies of the intended movement that reach the cerebellum via the pons apart of relevant parallel propioceptive, visual and vestibular cerebellar inputs; the cerebellum compares current state dynamics with the motor plan and generates error correcting signals for the thalamus to send to the cortex). Associative cortical networks integrate the different sets of data transmitted about the specific action being performed in the changing context of other concurrent mental contents.
All these connections needed to organize any simple movement use selected groups of neurons sitting at different neuromeric modules, which thus collaborate on demand more or less, depending of the case. Descending motor orders generally reach massively a series of reticular neurons distributed across most rhombomeres and only a selection of these units (those which result more strongly activated by the casual coincidence of diverse concurrent inputs) release in their turn relevant reticulospinal pathways doing the same in the spinal cord (redundant multisegmental signaling to a surplus of segmental units). Spinal premotor interneurons do the final local refined filtering and selection of motor signals thus obtaining the final motor instruction for the motoneurons.
In a way, therefore, the availability of multiple downstream neuromeric units, all of which have their own sensory inputs and capacity to organize details of reflex or voluntary motoneuronal output in mutual interaction, eliminates the need for the cortex to compute absolutely all details of intended actions. This system also has the potential to learn over time habitual action mechanisms with many subtle variants, which later can be selected from and repeated automatically with modulated force and velocity with surprising success. This bottom capacity for adjustment on the run of any sufficiently practiced motor act is called ‘mechanisation of movement’ and is largely dependent on the repetitive, redundant properties of series of neuromeric units that share considerable information among them, but subtly offer alternative variant options among which the higher centers can easily choose. Most of the detailed computing is pre-prepared. This allows the enormous functional capacity of the cerebral cortex to be dedicated instead to aspects of creation, motivation, combination, and intentional design. A concert pianist at work thinks about the music he wants you to hear, not about his own finger movements. The neuromeres and other sophisticated auxiliary mechanisms are covering that duly mechanized aspect.
The case of vision
The sense of vision is peculiar in that its receptive organ, the neural retina is itself an evaginated part of the wall of the neural tube. Evolutionarily and ontogenetically, the retina starts as a single median patch at the rostral end of the neural plate. Subsequent action of signals originated from the median prechordal plate inhibits the retinal fate at the midline and leaves bilateral primordia that evaginate producing the optic nerves and eye vesicles, a central part of which differentiates as the neural retina. This evaginated patch of the neural wall produces a layered neuronal structure that analyzes light-mediated visual input that reaches the deep retinal photoreceptors. The retinal output is emitted along the optic nerve, optic chiasma and central optic tract (divided into a principal lateral part and an accessory basal tract) by the diverse types of retinal ganglion cells. Thanks to neuromeric specializations, a general function, such as ‘vision’ is decomposed into many partial functions occurring in parallel, but mutually coordinated, in different parts of the brain. Neuroscience books often comment about the two cortical visual ‘pathways’, namely those computing on one hand ‘what’ you see, and on the other hand, ‘where/how’ you see it, to which can be added the ‘color’ cortical pathway. These complex cortical processes do not have much to do with neuromeres, but many other underlying visual mechanisms occur at subcortical neuromeric level. Among the seven forebrain prosomeres (2 in the hypothalamus, 3 in the diencephalon -these include respectively the prethalamic, thalamic, and pretectal regions- and 2 in the midbrain), six of them contain visual centers (some of them several centers each, to a total of some 20 subcortical visual centers; Puelles 2022;
Figure 4). Textbooks usually refer only to a handful of subcortical centers involved in visual function. The lateral geniculate and lateral posterior thalamic nuclei (specializations of prosomere dp2; LG, LPc in
Figure 4) are cited obligatorily, because they crucially relay retinal data about both ‘what’ and ‘where’ to primary and associative visual cortex. If you lose these data, you are blind.
Another frequently cited subcortical visual center is the midbrain superior colliculus (derivative of the rostral m1 prosomere; SC in
Figure 4). This large center maps the binocular retinal visual field with respect to your head and shoulders and your auditory field and computes therein ‘where you should look’ (i.e., which binocular eye movements are needed to jump to a new place in or outside of the vision field). This can be done under voluntary cortical control (you look where you decide), or partly automatically, responding by reflex to ‘novelty’, that is, to mapped sorpresive visual, auditory or tactile stimuli. Collicular output coordinates indirectly, via dedicated preoculomotor reticular cells in the dp1 prosomere or in rhombomere 5, the degree of activation of each of the oculomotor neurons connected via three cranial nerves to six muscles for each eyeball. These oculomotor neurons also receive parallel vestibular and cerebellar input (via other rhombomeres), which independently adjust your eyes to the accidental movements of your head (for instance in a ship moved by the sea, or riding a horse). If everything works, both your eyes jump rapidly (head movement corrected to eschew apparent movement of the world) to the new mapped points of space that interest us (or at least our superior colliculus) because nothing was expected to occur there. We actually ‘explore’ constantly visual and auditory space for such new stimuli of interest. It’s an activity that saves your life in the wild and also in the midst of heavy traffic. You need a handful of diencephalic, midbrain, and rhombencephalic neuromeres to perform this apparently modest visuomotor jumping function. Many times, it is not enough to move the eyes in their sockets, and we have to move as well the head on our neck, or gyrate our entire torso to fix the meaningful target (e.g., a dog growls behind you; better have a look). This of course requires descending collicular pathways targeting other oculomotor neurons sitting in the brainstem (rhombomeres 0 and 5), or within cervical and thoracic spinal neuromeres. The superior colliculus also connects retinotopically in ascending direction with many of the other diencephalic visual centers in dp1-dp3, apparently contributing its mapped computation of ‘where’ in space the things we see are, and how they move relative to us, irrespective whether we look at them or not. Thus, such an apparently easy function like ‘looking’ in voluntary or automatic modes needs a very complex point-to-point interconnected multiplicity of collaborative visual neuromeric centers with associated integration of diverse other sensorial inputs from other neuromeres (vestibular, propioceptive, tactile, auditory), finishing in complex bilateral motor control of two independent eyes so that their lines of sight converge unfailingly (and rapidly, without searching) at the same point in space. This selects the signal to be studied by the cortex and simultaneously resolves many underlying urgent practical problems not worth cortical attention, which needs time. All the axonal pathways involved in this complex network refer their precise growth and synaptic targeting to neuromeric molecular coordinates, neuromeric differential adhesivity properties, and recognition of relative positions of targeted neurons within their respective neuromeric frameworks. You probably are starting to understand now why the columnar theory of the brain does not provide sufficient explanation of brain functions, and we need not only neuromeric perspective but also intraneuromeric microzonal regionalization to make a dent on the knowledge task ahead of us.
The pretectal region (in dp1;
Figure 4) contains apart from preoculomotor reticular cells a retinorecipient visual center, the olivary pretectal nucleus (OPT in
Figure 4), which is involved in the adjustment of the diameter of our eye pupil to the level of light that incides on the eye (the pupil closes automatically proportionately to the level of light). This apparently is a simple sensorimotor reflex task (we regulate the pupil with a constrictor muscle controlled via the parasympathetic ciliary ganglion lying close to the eye) also involves at least a coordination of the three diencephalic neuromeres, each of which has a center (different in terms of neuronal types) receiving via the optic tract a copy of the ‘how much light’ signal from the retina, so that the resulting reflex output via a single nerve is either summatory, or can be independently triggered from three different retinorecipient sites, or is triggered in three diverse functional situations (e.g., you suddenly dilate your pupils when you are insulted or otherwise attacked, irrespective of the level of illumination, because your parasympathetic tone decreases as the sympathetic tone increases suddenly; the sympathetic tone is regulated from the thoracic spinal cord). You need analysis by 3 diencephalic neuromeres, a midbrain neuromere for initial output to the muscle, and autonomic parasympathetic and sympathetic output ganglia for understanding this simple circuit.
Daily time cycles are measured by a central neural clock, the suprachiasmatic hypothalamic nucleus (SCH in
Figure 4). This receives separately information from the retina about the amount of ambient light to which we are exposed (maximal at noon). This signal is translated and projected widely to the forebrain and hindbrain neuromeres, with widespread effects on all metabolic, temperature-control, hormonal, activity, and sleep/awakeness cycles that configure in ways that change along the day our body state.
I have explained briefly the 5 best known subcortical visual centers, whose specialized functions related to retinal signals commonly involve interactions with specialized vision-related neurons in various other neuromeres. There are however actually about 20 such subcortical visual centers (review in Puelles 2022;
Figure 4), most of which are still uncharacterized about which functions they perform and which are the relevant interneuromeric circuitries involved. There are particular visual functions which have been partly studied physiologically, but we still ignore which is the contribution of particular neuromeres to them. That is the case of ‘accommodation’, that is, the focusing of our perceived sight according to the relative distance from us of the object of interest. Unfocused objects look blurry or duplicated. Important knowledge has been gained about the cortical mechanism that examines binocular images and
identifies quantitatively any apparently duplicated mental images implying an unfocused single object (rather than resulting from real double objects). This diagnosis can only be performed in the visual cortex. It is assumed that the cortex consequently sends a descending correcting signal to some subcortical visual center in the diencephalon and/or midbrain (5 neuromeres in those two regions) and this center would translate the cortical correcting input and send a corresponding motor output perhaps via reticular cells to the oculomotor motoneurons involved in ocular convergence (innervating the medial rectus muscle of both sides). Their activation brings the visual focus closer to us (or obtains via inhibitory interneurons a relaxation of the tone of the same mechanism for focusing more distant objects). There is no insight yet of where this subcortical accommodation mechanism lies, but I would not be surprised if it also involves collaborative modular neuromeres.
Any other major neural function can be similarly decomposed into a set of different neuromeric collaborative networks that interact to contribute partial functions that collectively achieve and apparently ‘make easy’ the performance of quite complex neural tasks (Redies and Puelles 2001). There is current physiological research on neuromeric centers involved in respiratory rhythms, cardiac rhythms, vocalization, temperature control, sleep/awakeness cycles, hunger/satiety cycles, or vestibular, cochlear, and trigeminal neuromeric functional modules (e.g., Diaz et al. 2003; see also Nieuwenhuys and Puelles 2016). The mechanism involving ‘place cells’, ‘border cells’, ‘head orientation cells’ and ‘spatial reticle cells’ in the hippocampal system apparently are underpinned by various neuromere-related propioceptive, vestibular, visual, hypothalamic (mamillary) and thalamic subcortical subsystems (e.g., Wiener and Taube 2005; Mizumori 2008). We know that various central modulating neuronal systems, such as the dopaminergic, noradrenergic and serotonergic systems are organized neuromerically (see, e.g., Puelles and Medina 1994; Alonso et al. 2012).
In the beginnings of neuromeric embryological studies none of these modern physiological questions could be addressed, leading to attacks upon the interest of neuromeric phenomena noted by morphologists because no general function could be ascribed at the time to these developmental units, wrongly thought, moreover, to be transient. Now we know that neuromeres do not disappear, becoming instead, as tested by transgenic molecular fate-mapping (
Figure 2 and
Figure 3), fundamental primordia (FMUs) of entire adult brain portions. Such embryonic regions obtain early on a differential molecular specification on top of fundamental shared metameric structural aspects (i.e., a sensory alar plate and a motor basal plate). These initial conditions end up establishing a crucial role for neuromeres and their inner microzonal subdivisions. The latter each generates a functionally special type of neuron. As development advances the specialized neurons collectively produce complex computing structures and associated circuitries intrinsic to each neuromere that underpin specific sensorimotor reflex properties, as well as more or less extended longitudinally projecting axons that interconnect plurineuromeric sensory and motor columns, which always have a segmental modular skeleton related to partial functions. Columns, when they exist, are never neuronally homogeneous, as was mistakenly concluded by Kuhlenbeck (1927, 1973), because they are composed of a set of neuromeric modules participating in a common computation (for instance, the trigeminal column integrates 16 neuromeres in the computation of rich and varied facial somatosensory feelings). The classic columnar ‘thalamus’ was not a column, but actually referred to badly studied prethalamic, thalamic and pretectal neuromeric modules receiving direct visual input, involving as we now know over a dozen differential visual microzones serving particular subfunctions of vision via specific plurineuromeric collaboration, connected bidirectionally of course with the visual cortex functions via the visual parts of the thalamic neuromeric unit.
Modern mechanistic understanding of all major cerebral functions, even those involving the cerebral cortex, the hypothalamus, the thalamus, and the cerebellum, need attention to the subcortical set of neuromeres that provide essential functional support in the realms of sensory analysis, reflex circuitry, or motor organization. The prosomeric model, which takes into account the smallest structural units emerging in the brain (as components of neuromeric macrounits), has become an essential guidance model for the mapping and causal understanding of functional circuits and pathways in the brain.
Acknowledgments
The institutional help of the Univ. of Murcia to LP (emeritus professor) is appreciated. -Work supported by research grant 21925/PI/22 to E.García-Calero as IP. Work financed by the Autonomous Community of Murcia through its Grants to Projects Developing Scientific and Technical Research by Competitive Groups, forming part of the Regional Program for the Promotion of Scientific and Technical Research (Actuation Plan 2022) of the Séneca Foundation/Agency for Science and Technology of the Murcia Region. -There is no conflict of interest associated to this work. -All image materials used are extracted from previous publications of the author.
Abbreviations
4N |
trochlear motor nucleus |
ac |
anterior commissure |
AH |
anterior hypothalamic nucleus |
aot |
accessory optic tract |
APt |
anterior pituitary (adenohypophysis) |
APT |
anterior pretectal nucleus |
BIC |
interstitial nucleus of the inferior collicular brachium conjunctivum |
BSC |
interstitial nucleus of the superior collicular or tectal brachium |
Cb |
cerebellum |
cc |
corpus callosum |
chr |
chorioidal roofplate |
DCIC |
dorsal cap of inferior colliculus |
Di |
diencephalon |
dp1-dp3 |
diencephalic prosomeres 1-3 |
DPal |
dorsal pallium |
DR |
dorsal raphe nucleus |
DT |
dorsal terminal nucleus of the accessory optic pathway |
fr |
fasciculus retroflexus (habenulo-interpeduncular tract) |
hc |
hippocampal commissure |
hp1 |
hypothalamo-telencephalic prosomere 1 |
hp2 |
hypothalamo-telencephalic prosomere 2 |
HyA |
hypothalamo-amygdalar corridor |
IC |
inferior colliculus |
icc |
intercollicular commissure |
IGL |
intergeniculate leaflet |
IP |
interpeduncular nucleus |
IPN |
interpeduncular nucleus |
is |
isthmic rhombomere (r0) |
isth |
isthmus |
IVe |
inferior vestibular nucleus |
ivf |
interventricular foramen |
LG |
lateral geniculate nucleus |
ll |
lateral lemniscus tract |
lot |
lateral optic tract |
LPal |
lateral pallium |
LPc |
lateral posterior nucleus, caudal part (pulvinar homolog in mouse) |
LT |
lateral terminal nucleus |
LVe |
lateral vestibular nuleus |
M |
mamillary body |
m1-m2 |
midbrain prosomeres 1-2 |
mes |
mesencephalon (midbrain) |
MG |
medual geniculate nucleus |
MiTg |
microcellular tegmental nucleus |
mlf |
medial longitudinal tract |
mp1-mp2 |
midbraon prosomeres 1-2 |
MPal |
medial pallium |
MT |
medial terminal nucleus of the accessory optic pathway |
OB |
olfactory bulb |
och |
optic chiasma |
on |
optic nerve |
OPT |
olivary pretectal nucleus |
Ov |
ovoid retinorecipient nucleus |
p1-p3 |
diencephalic prosomeres 1-3 |
PalSe |
pallial septum (now interpreted as ‘subpallial dorsal septum’) |
PBG |
parabigeminal nucleus |
pc |
posterior commissure |
PG |
pregeniculate nucleus |
pHb |
parahabenular retinorecipient nucleus |
Pi |
pineal gland |
PLi |
posterior lineal thalamic nucleus |
POA |
preoptic area |
PP |
posterior peduncular nucleus |
PPa |
peduncular paraventricular area |
PPt |
posterior pituitary (neurohypophysis) |
PSO |
parasupraoptic retinorecipient nucleus |
PSPa |
peduncular subparaventricular hypothalamic area |
PThE |
prethalamic eminence |
r0-r11 |
rhombomeres 0 (isthmus) to 11 |
Rbd |
rhabdoid nucleus |
RM |
retromamillart área |
RP |
retropeduncular nucleus |
Rt |
reticular nucleus |
Sag |
sagular nucleus |
SC |
superior colliculus |
SCH |
suprachiasmatic nucleus |
SNC |
substantia nigra compacta |
SO |
supraoptic nucleus |
SPallSe |
subpallial septum |
SubB/Cnf |
subbrachial nucleus/cuneiform nucleus or area |
SuG |
subgeniculate nucleus |
SVe |
superior vestibular nucleus |
tc |
tectal commissure |
TG |
tectal gray retinorecipient formation |
THy |
terminal hypothalamus |
TPa |
terminal paraventricular area |
TSPa |
terminal subparaventricular hypothalamic area |
VIIm |
facial motor nucleus (7th cranial nerve) |
VIIn |
facial nerve root |
VLL |
ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus |
VLPO |
ventrolateral preoptic nucleus |
VPal |
ventral pallium |
xscp |
decussation of superior cerebellar peduncle |
Zi |
zona incerta |
References
- Alexander PA. 1992. The Making of a Fly: The Genetics of Animal Design. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
- Alonso A, Merchán P, Sandoval JE, Sánchez-Arrones L, Garcia-Cazorla A, Artuch R, Ferrán JL, Martínez-de-la-Torre M, Puelles L. 2012. Development of the serotonergic cells in murine raphe nuclei and their relations with rhombomeric domains. Brain Struct Funct 218:1229-1277. [CrossRef]
- Amat JA, Martínez-de-la-Torre M, Trujillo CMª, Fernández B, Puelles L. 2022. Neurogenetic heterochrony in chick, lizard and rat mapped with wholemount AChE and the prosomeric model. Brain Behav Evol. [CrossRef]
- Bergquist H, Källén B. 1954. Notes on the early histogenesis and morphogenesis of the central nervous system in vertebrates. J Comp Neurol 100:627–659. [CrossRef]
- Bulfone A, Puelles L, Porteus MH, Frohman MA, Martin GR, Rubenstein JLR. 1993. Spatially restricted expression of Dlx-1, Dlx-2 (Tes-1), Gbx-2 and Wnt-3 in the embryonic day 12.5 mouse forebrain defines potential transverse and longitudinal segmental boundaries. J Neurosci 13: 3155-3172. [CrossRef]
- Clarke E, O’Malley CD. 1995. The Human Brain and Spinal Cord. A Historical Study Illustrated by Writings from Antiquity to the Twentieth Century. 2nd Edit, revised and enlarged (1st Edit in 1968). San Francisco: Norman Publishing.
- Davidson EH. 2001. Genomic Regulatory Systems. Development and Evolution. San Diego: Academic Press.
- Díaz C, Glover JC, Puelles L, Bjaalie J. 2003. The relationship between hodological and cytoarchitectonic organization in the vestibular complex of the 11-day chicken embryo. J Comp Neurol 457:87–105. [CrossRef]
- Di Bonito MA, Studer M, Puelles L. 2017. Nuclear derivatives and axonal projections originating from rhombomere 4 in the mouse hindbrain. Brain Struct Funct 222:3509-3542. [CrossRef]
- Gaskell WH. 1889. On the relation between the structure, function and origin of the cranial nerves, together with a theory of the origin of the nervous system of vertebrates. J Physiol 10: 153–211. [CrossRef]
- Gribnau AAM, Geijsberts LGM. 1985. Morphogenesis of the brain in staged Rhesus monkey embryos. Adv Anat Embryol Cell Biol 91:1-69. [CrossRef]
- Haller B. 1900. Vom Bau des Wirbelthiergehirns. III. Theil. Mus. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.
- Herrick CJ. 1903. The doctrine of nerve components and some of its applications. J. Comp. Neurol. 13:301–312. [CrossRef]
- Herrick CJ. 1910. The morphology of the forebrain in Amphibia and Reptilia. J. Comp. Neurol. 20:413–547. [CrossRef]
- Herrick CJ. 1924. Neurological Foundations of Animal Behavior. New York: Holt.
- Herrick CJ. 1926. Brains of Rats and Men. Chicago: Univ.of Chicago Press.
- Herrick CJ. 1931. An Introduction to Neurology. 5th Edit. Philadelphia: Saunders.
- Herrick CJ. 1932. The Thinking Machine. 2nd. Edit. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press.
- Herrick CJ. 1948. The Brain of the Tiger Salamander, Ambystoma tigrinum. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press.
- Herrick CJ. 1949. George Elliott Coghill, Naturalist and Philosopher. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press.
- Herrick CJ. 1956. The Evolution of Human Nature. Austin: Univ.of Texas Press.
- His W. 1893. Vorschläge zur Eintheilung des Gehirns. Arch. Anat. EntwicklGesch. 1893:173–179.
- His W (1895) Die Anatomische Nomenclatur, Nomina Anatomica. Neurol Suppl Bd, Arch Anat Entwickl-Gesch Jahrg 1895:155-177.
- His W. 1904. Die Entwicklung des Menschliches Gehirns während der Ersten Monaten. Leipzig: Hirzel.
- Johnston JB. 1902. An attempt to define the primitive functional divisions of the central nervous system. J Comp Neurol 12:87–106. [CrossRef]
- Johnston JB. 1908. The Nervous System of Vertebrates. London: John Murray.
- Kaufman SA. 1993. The Origins of Order: Self-organization and Selection in Evolution.
- Kuhlenbeck H. 1927. Vorlesungen über das Zentralnervensystem der Wirbeltiere. Eine Einführung in die Gehirnanatomie auf vergleichender Grundlage. Jena: Gustav Fischer Verlag.
- Kuhlenbeck H. 1973. The Central Nervous System of Vertebrates. Vol.3. Part II: Overall Morphological Pattern. Basel: S.Karger.
- Meyer A. 1971. Historical Aspects of Cerebral Anatomy. London: Oxford Univ. Press.
- Mizumori SJY. 2008. Hippocampal Place Fields. Relevance to Learning and Memory. Mizumori SJY (ed), Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.
- Nieuwenhuys R., Puelles L. 2016. Towards a New Neuromorphology. Berlin, Springer Verlag. ISBN 978-3-319-25692-4.
- Orr H. 1887. Contribution to the embryology of the lizard, with special reference to the central nervous system and some organs of the head, together with observations on the origin of vertebrates. J Morphol 1:311–372. [CrossRef]
- Palmgren A. 1921. Embryological and morphologic studies on the midbrain and cerebellum of vertebrates. Acta Zool 2:1–94.
- Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ. 2013. The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. 4th edit. Amsterdam-London: Academic Press/Elsevier.
- Puelles L, Amat JA, Martínez de la Torre M. 1987. Segment-related, mosaic neurogenetic pattern in the forebrain and mesencephalon of early chick embryos. I. Topography of AChE-positive neuroblasts up to stage HH18. J Comp Neurol 266:147-268. [CrossRef]
- Puelles L, Martinez-de-la-Torre M, Bardet S, Rubenstein, JLR. 2012. Hypothalamus. In: “The Mouse Nervous System”. Watson C, Paxinos G, Puelles L (eds). Academic Press/Elsevier; pp 221-312.
- Puelles L., Martínez-Marin R., Melgarejo-Otalora P., Ayad A., Valavanis A., Ferran JL. 2019. Patterned vascularization of embryonic mouse forebrain and neuromeric topology of major human subarachnoidal arterial branches: Prosomeric mapping. Front. Neuroanat. [CrossRef]
- Puelles L, Rubenstein JL. 2015. A new scenario of hypothalamic organization: rationale of new hypotheses introduced in the updated prosomeric model. Front Neuroanat. Mar 19;9:27. [CrossRef]
- Puelles L, Rubenstein JLR. 1993. Expression patterns of homeobox and other putative regulatory genes in the embryonic mouse forebrain suggest a neuromeric organization. Trends Neurosci 16:472-479. [CrossRef]
- Puelles L, Medina L. 1994. Development of neurons expressing tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine in the chicken brain: a comparative segmental analysis. In: “Phylogeny and Development of Catecholamine Systems in the CNS of Vertebrates”, Reiner A and Smeets WJAJ (eds), pp381-406. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ.Press.
- Puelles L, Rubenstein JLR. 2003. Forebrain gene expression domains and the evolving prosomeric model. Trends Neurosci 26:469-476. [CrossRef]
- Puelles L. 2013. Plan of the developing vertebrate nervous system relating embryology to the adult nervous system (prosomere model, overview of brain organization), In: “Comprehensive Developmental Neuroscience: Patterning and Cell Type Specification in the Developing CNS and PNS” Rubenstein JLR, Rakic P (eds), Amsterdam: Academic Press, pp. 187-209.
- Puelles L. 2017. Role of secondary organizers in the evolution of forebrain development in vertebrates. In: “Handbook of Evolutionary Neuroscience” Shepherd SV (ed) Blackwell-Wiley: Chichester, UK. pp. 350-387.
- Puelles L. 2018. Developmental studies of avian brain organization. Int J Dev Biol 62: 207-224. [CrossRef]
- Puelles L. 2021. Recollections on the origin and development of the prosomeric model. Brain Behav Evol. 24 December 2021. [CrossRef]
- Puelles L. 2022. Prosomeric classification of retinorecipient centers: a new causal scenario. Brain Struct Funct. [CrossRef]
- Redies C, Puelles L. 2001. Modularity in CNS development. Bioessays 23:1100-1111. [CrossRef]
- Rendahl H. 1924. Embryologische und morphologische Studien über das Zwischenhirn beim Huhn. Acta Zool 5:241–344. [CrossRef]
- Rubenstein JLR, Martínez S, Shimamura K, Puelles L. 1994. The embryonic vertebrate forebrain: the prosomeric model. Science 266: 578-580. [CrossRef]
- Ruiz-Cabrera S, Pérez-Santos I, Zaldivar-Diez J, Garcñia-Cabezas MÁ. 2023. Expansion modes of primate nervous system structures in the light of the prosomeric model. Front Mamm Sci 2:1241573. [CrossRef]
- Sherrington 1906. The Integrative Action of the Nervous System 1st edit. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Swanson LW. 2012. Brain Architecture. Understanding the Basic Plan. 2nd Edit. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.
- Tomás-Roca L, Corral-San-Miguel R, Aroca P, Puelles L, Marín F. 2016. Crypto-rhombomeres of the mouse medulla oblongata, defined by molecular and morphological features. Brain Struct Funct 221:815-838. [CrossRef]
- Vaage S. 1969. The segmentation of the primitive neural tube in chick embryos (Gallus domesticus). A morphological, histochemical and autoradiographical investigation. Ergebn. Anat. EntwGesch. 41:1–88.
- Vaage S. 1973. The histogenesis of the isthmic nuclei in chick embryos (Gallus domesticus). I. A morphological study. Z Anat EntwGesch 142:283–314. [CrossRef]
- Von Kupffer K. 1906. Die Morphogenie des Zentralnervensystems. In: Handbuch der Vergleichenden und Experimentellen Entwicklungslehere der Wirbeltiere. Bd.2, Teil 3. pp 1-272, Hertwig O (ed), Jena: Gustav Fischer.
- Watson C, Shimogori T, Puelles L. 2017a. Mouse FGF8-Cre lineage analysis defines the territory of the postnatal mammalian isthmus. J Comp Neurol 525:2782–2799. [CrossRef]
- Watson C, Mitchelle A, Puelles L. 2017b. A new mammalian brain ontology based on developmental gene expression. In: “Evolution of Nervous Systems” 2nd edit, Vol. 2, pp 53–75, Kaas J (ed). Oxford: Elsevier.
- Watson C, Kirkcaldie M, Puelles L. 2017c. Developmental gene expression redefines the mammalian brain stem. In: “Evolution of Nervous Systems” 2nd edit, Vol. 2, pp 467–475, Kaas J (ed). Oxford: Elsevier.
- Wiener SI, Taube JS. 2005. Head Direction Cells and the Neural Mechanisms of Spatial Orientation. Wiener SI, Taube JS (eds), Cambridge, Massachusetts-London: A Bradford Book; MIT Press.
- Ziehen T. 1906. Die Morphogenie des Zentralnervensystems der Säugetiere. In: Handbuch der Vergleichenden und Experimentellen Entwicklungslehere der Wirbeltiere. Bd.2, Teil 3. pp 273-394, Hertwig O (ed), Jena: Gustav Fischer.
Figure 1.
Schemata illustrating embryonic (a) and adult (b) location and abbreviations for neuromeres within the prosomeric model. (a) Updated schema taken from Watson et al. (2017b), in which the forebrain prosomeric fields (including the secondary prosencephalon [red/blue], diencephalon [pink], and midbrain [green]) appear under the control of the Otx2 transcription factor. In contrast, prepontine rhombomeres r0/is and r1 relate to the area of influence of the secreted FGF8 morphogen and the Gbx2 transcription factor, while the pontine (r2–r4), retropontine (r5,r6), and medullary (r7–r11) hindbrain rhombomeres plus the spinal cord obey to differential Hox gene signals. These markers can be used to identify the different neuromeres in the mature brain (Tomás-Roca et al., 2016; Watson et al., 2017a,c). (b) Schema extracted from Puelles (2021) representing the whole set of prosomeric units in the adult brain (the vermis of the cerebellum belongs to r0—the hemispheres to r1). Note the large evaginated telencephalic development corresponding to hp1 (red), while the rostral end of the brain corresponds to the rostromedian acroterminal domain within hp2 (orange). The bipartite hypothalamus is divided into peduncular hypothalamus (PHy) within hp1 and terminal hypothalamus (THy) within hp2. The axis of the brain bends ventrally at the cephalic flexure, where a number of interneuromeric boundaries converge ventralwards, and also shows a less marked dorsal bending at pontine levels, causing there also some convergence of neuromeric boundaries at the ventricular surface. See list of Abbreviations.
Figure 1.
Schemata illustrating embryonic (a) and adult (b) location and abbreviations for neuromeres within the prosomeric model. (a) Updated schema taken from Watson et al. (2017b), in which the forebrain prosomeric fields (including the secondary prosencephalon [red/blue], diencephalon [pink], and midbrain [green]) appear under the control of the Otx2 transcription factor. In contrast, prepontine rhombomeres r0/is and r1 relate to the area of influence of the secreted FGF8 morphogen and the Gbx2 transcription factor, while the pontine (r2–r4), retropontine (r5,r6), and medullary (r7–r11) hindbrain rhombomeres plus the spinal cord obey to differential Hox gene signals. These markers can be used to identify the different neuromeres in the mature brain (Tomás-Roca et al., 2016; Watson et al., 2017a,c). (b) Schema extracted from Puelles (2021) representing the whole set of prosomeric units in the adult brain (the vermis of the cerebellum belongs to r0—the hemispheres to r1). Note the large evaginated telencephalic development corresponding to hp1 (red), while the rostral end of the brain corresponds to the rostromedian acroterminal domain within hp2 (orange). The bipartite hypothalamus is divided into peduncular hypothalamus (PHy) within hp1 and terminal hypothalamus (THy) within hp2. The axis of the brain bends ventrally at the cephalic flexure, where a number of interneuromeric boundaries converge ventralwards, and also shows a less marked dorsal bending at pontine levels, causing there also some convergence of neuromeric boundaries at the ventricular surface. See list of Abbreviations.
Figure 2.
Coronal (a) and sagittal (b) sections through adult mouse brains reacted with LacZ histochemical reaction to demonstrate the co-expression of the LacZ code inserted transgenically into the Fgf8 coding sequence (extracted from Watson et al. 2017b). This gene is restricted at initial neural tube stages to expression in the isthmic (r0) rhombomere, which is the rostralmost hindbrain neuromeric unit, limiting with the midbrain (mes; represented here by SC and IC). The isthmic neuromere also forms the vermal (median) part of the cerebellum (b; note there is some artefactual overlap between the labeled cerebellum and the inferior colliculus). The isthmic neuromere delimited at early developmental stages by Fgf8 gene expression is visualized in the adult brain as blue LacZ reaction product. The unit clearly persists as its adult derivative, still forming a transverse rostral domain of the hindbrain that limits rostrally with the midbrain and caudally with rhombomere 1 (r1). Both neighboring domains show some dispersed blue cells that are interpreted as isthmic-derived cells that migrated tangentially into the neighboring areas. The blue r0 expression domain forms a radially complete (ventricular to pial surface in a) transverse ring of the brain; dorsoventrally it clearly extends from the cerebellar roof to the floorplate interpeduncular nucleus (IPN in b). The mamillary body lying rostral to the retroflex tract (fr) also shows blue cells, but these are due to separate intrinsic expression of Fgf8 at that hypothalamic locus. See list of Abbreviations.
Figure 2.
Coronal (a) and sagittal (b) sections through adult mouse brains reacted with LacZ histochemical reaction to demonstrate the co-expression of the LacZ code inserted transgenically into the Fgf8 coding sequence (extracted from Watson et al. 2017b). This gene is restricted at initial neural tube stages to expression in the isthmic (r0) rhombomere, which is the rostralmost hindbrain neuromeric unit, limiting with the midbrain (mes; represented here by SC and IC). The isthmic neuromere also forms the vermal (median) part of the cerebellum (b; note there is some artefactual overlap between the labeled cerebellum and the inferior colliculus). The isthmic neuromere delimited at early developmental stages by Fgf8 gene expression is visualized in the adult brain as blue LacZ reaction product. The unit clearly persists as its adult derivative, still forming a transverse rostral domain of the hindbrain that limits rostrally with the midbrain and caudally with rhombomere 1 (r1). Both neighboring domains show some dispersed blue cells that are interpreted as isthmic-derived cells that migrated tangentially into the neighboring areas. The blue r0 expression domain forms a radially complete (ventricular to pial surface in a) transverse ring of the brain; dorsoventrally it clearly extends from the cerebellar roof to the floorplate interpeduncular nucleus (IPN in b). The mamillary body lying rostral to the retroflex tract (fr) also shows blue cells, but these are due to separate intrinsic expression of Fgf8 at that hypothalamic locus. See list of Abbreviations.
Figure 3.
Two parasagittal sections through the same adult mouse brain transgenically labeled with yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) code attached to the Hoxb1 codon (a,b; a lies medial to b; extracted from DiBonito et al. 2017). The presence in the adult brain of neurons and axons containing YFP was detected by immunoreaction (brown reaction product). The Hoxb1 gene is selectively expressed from very early neural tube stages onwards in the r4 rhombomere. Accordingly, in the adult the YFP labeling indicates neurons born inside r4 and their axonal projections, as well as cells born within r4 and migrated tangentially into other brain loci. Facial motor neurons migrate early on into the r6 alar plate (VIIm in a), and the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus (VLL), present in rhombomere 1 of the propontine hindbrain along the ascending route of the lateral lemniscus towards the inferior colliculus is also migrated out of r4; it is accompanied by ascending auditory fibers in the lateral lemniscus (VLL,ll in a,b). The latter come from the labeled cochlear module of the auditory column corresponding to r4 (labeled periventricular area above LVe in a). The labeled lateral lemniscus fibers penetrate both the IC and SC, and some of them even follow the brachium of the inferior colliculus into the diencephalon, where they reach the medial geniculate auditory thalamic nucleus (red arrowheads; IC, SC, MG; a,b). A distinct labeled transverse dorsoventral strip represents strictly the adult derivative of r4, identified as well by the presence of the facial nerve root (VIIn in b). There is also evidence of r4 projections into the vestibular nuclei (SVe, LVe, IVe), the cerebellum (Cb) and the spinal cord (red arrow in b). See list of Abbreviations.
Figure 3.
Two parasagittal sections through the same adult mouse brain transgenically labeled with yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) code attached to the Hoxb1 codon (a,b; a lies medial to b; extracted from DiBonito et al. 2017). The presence in the adult brain of neurons and axons containing YFP was detected by immunoreaction (brown reaction product). The Hoxb1 gene is selectively expressed from very early neural tube stages onwards in the r4 rhombomere. Accordingly, in the adult the YFP labeling indicates neurons born inside r4 and their axonal projections, as well as cells born within r4 and migrated tangentially into other brain loci. Facial motor neurons migrate early on into the r6 alar plate (VIIm in a), and the ventral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus (VLL), present in rhombomere 1 of the propontine hindbrain along the ascending route of the lateral lemniscus towards the inferior colliculus is also migrated out of r4; it is accompanied by ascending auditory fibers in the lateral lemniscus (VLL,ll in a,b). The latter come from the labeled cochlear module of the auditory column corresponding to r4 (labeled periventricular area above LVe in a). The labeled lateral lemniscus fibers penetrate both the IC and SC, and some of them even follow the brachium of the inferior colliculus into the diencephalon, where they reach the medial geniculate auditory thalamic nucleus (red arrowheads; IC, SC, MG; a,b). A distinct labeled transverse dorsoventral strip represents strictly the adult derivative of r4, identified as well by the presence of the facial nerve root (VIIn in b). There is also evidence of r4 projections into the vestibular nuclei (SVe, LVe, IVe), the cerebellum (Cb) and the spinal cord (red arrow in b). See list of Abbreviations.
Figure 4.
Graphic lateral projection mapping of the set of retinorecipient forebrain centers over hypothalamus, diencephalon and midbrain, according to the prosomeric model and the Paxinos and Franklin 2013 Mouse Brain Atlas (extracted from Puelles 2021). This lateral view map shows the course of the optic tract in yellow, the alar–basal boundary in red, and blue lines representing the interprosomeric boundaries (the prosomeres are identified at the top or at the bottom of the map). The diverse visual centers of each prosomere are given the same background color, to better see the groupings. It is noted that relevant neuromeres have more than one retinal projection field, in most cases in the alar plate, but sometimes also in the basal plate. There is no observable columnar pattern at all (columns should be parallel to the optic tract). The noncolored structures do not receive retinal afferents (but the mesodiencephalic substantia nigra, SNC, was exceptionally marked in grey, for clarity). See list of Abbreviations.
Figure 4.
Graphic lateral projection mapping of the set of retinorecipient forebrain centers over hypothalamus, diencephalon and midbrain, according to the prosomeric model and the Paxinos and Franklin 2013 Mouse Brain Atlas (extracted from Puelles 2021). This lateral view map shows the course of the optic tract in yellow, the alar–basal boundary in red, and blue lines representing the interprosomeric boundaries (the prosomeres are identified at the top or at the bottom of the map). The diverse visual centers of each prosomere are given the same background color, to better see the groupings. It is noted that relevant neuromeres have more than one retinal projection field, in most cases in the alar plate, but sometimes also in the basal plate. There is no observable columnar pattern at all (columns should be parallel to the optic tract). The noncolored structures do not receive retinal afferents (but the mesodiencephalic substantia nigra, SNC, was exceptionally marked in grey, for clarity). See list of Abbreviations.
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).