Preprint
Article

Emotional and Behavioural Factors Predisposing to Internet Addiction: The Smartphone Distraction among Italian High School Students

Altmetrics

Downloads

185

Views

102

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

30 January 2024

Posted:

31 January 2024

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
In a digitally oriented society, smartphones provide continual online accessibility to daily life, while simultaneously predisposing adolescents to engage in prolonged connections for various purposes, thus escalating the risk of Internet Addiction (IA). Cognitive processes such as multitasking and attentional shifting are frequently associated with smartphone activities. Additionally, online engagements may serve as emotional strategies for regulating negative states (e.g., boredom and distress), redirecting attention towards more gratifying activities, such as social media content. This study delves into cognitive-emotional processes (emotion regulation, attention impulsiveness, online vigilance, and multitasking) and emotional/behavioural factors (emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationships, and prosocial behaviours) that may be implicated in smartphone activities and technology addiction among adolescents. A community sample (N=676) of Italian high school students (42.2% females) completed the Smartphone Distraction Scale (SDS), the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire for internalizing/externalizing symptoms (SDQ), and the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) to assess the presence and severity of IA. The scores on the Smartphone Distraction Scale were found to be positively associated with IA levels. Furthermore, students exhibiting higher internalizing/externalizing symptoms, particularly those with traits of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), are more likely to manifest problematic smartphone usage. The implications for screening adolescents more susceptible to developing IA symptoms and for implementing preventive interventions are discussed.
Keywords: 
Subject: Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Introduction

Media devices have become integral to the daily activities of young individuals, with the smartphone swiftly supplanting the Personal Computer as the preferred means of communication (e.g., WhatsApp or Telegram messaging), social interaction (e.g., Facebook, Instagram, TikTok), learning (e.g., homework), and entertainment (such as listening to music or watching videos). It is estimated that nearly all children and adolescents (e.g., 95% in the U.S., 80% in Europe, and 90% in Asia) utilize their smartphones to access the Internet, and they report being “daily” or “almost all the time” connected online through their devices [1,2,3]. Data from an Italian survey indicate that children estimate spending over 3 hours (41%) or over 2 hours (29%) each day on Internet and Social Networking Sites [4]. Nevertheless, they frequently underestimate the duration of on-screen activities to the extent of losing track of time [5].

1.1. Digital connectivity and excessive smartphone usage

The increasing prevalence of smartphones, coupled with constant internet accessibility, has resulted in a heightened incidence of Internet Addiction (IA) among adolescents [6]. Montag and Reuter [7] define IA as any online-related behaviour that detrimentally affects various aspects of a user's life, such as family conflicts, loss of friends, and diminished work/school performance. Numerous studies have consistently linked elevated daily smartphone usage to compromised family and peer relationships, challenges in school and learning, and reduced personal well-being [8,9,10]. Especially during the COVID-19 pandemic, prolonged internet activities have been observed in young individuals, yielding adverse effects on both physical and psychological health [11,12]. However, adolescents themselves appear cognizant of the negative consequences associated with excessive engagement in online activities [13]. Results from an Italian survey indicate that distractions during homework (24%), eyes burning (21%), neck/back pain (12%), sleep disturbances (10%), and mood alterations (7%) are among the most commonly reported issues by adolescents [4].
Hence, the increasing body of evidence concerning the adverse outcomes associated with widespread smartphone use underscores the imperative to investigate factors linked to problematic engagement among young individuals, despite the current absence of a precise definition for “smartphone addiction.” While some authors characterize excessive smartphone use as a manifestation of Internet Use Disorder [14], the diagnostic criteria for Internet Addiction (IA), Mobile Addiction, or Social Media Addiction are currently not incorporated into DSM-5 [15]. Nevertheless, certain problematic behavioural patterns have been identified in the use of these devices, encompassing withdrawal (e.g., feeling anxious or irritable when the smartphone is disconnected), tolerance (i.e., escalating smartphone use), and loss of control (e.g., incessant checking or messaging), all of which detrimentally affect daily activities, emotional well-being, and social interactions. These patterns closely resemble those observed in other behavioural or substance addictions. Furthermore, a well-established body of scientific literature attests that certain symptoms associated with IA are commonly observed in cases of excessive smartphone use, including impulsiveness, depression, anxiety, mood alterations, low self-esteem, and impaired cognitive functions [16]. Consequently, researchers have increasingly directed their attention toward exploring the consequences of problematic engagement in online activities, rather than toward the classification or diagnostic issues.

1.2. Smartphone distraction

Researchers agree that the advent of new technologies has fundamentally altered individuals' interactions with their environment (e.g., [17,18,19]). As individuals engage in prolonged and diverse online activities, primarily through their smartphones, their attentional systems contend continually with potential overload from external factors (e.g., multitasking or focusing on notifications) and internal stimuli (such as contemplating social networks; [20]). Attention, as a cognitive process enabling individuals to process information in their environment, is inherently a limited resource. The continual use of digital devices places upon users both the demand for prolonged attentional efforts (such as watching videos) and the necessity to distribute cognitive resources among multiple stimuli simultaneously (i.e., attentional shifting). Nevertheless, protracted on-screen engagements or conflicting activities (such as checking notifications while studying or making phone calls while driving) can result in information overload, distraction, and interference with overall performance.
In accordance with Throuvala and colleagues [21], distraction is delineated as “an expression of attentional loss associated with smartphone use” (p. 12). Distraction can be characterized as a response to both external stimuli (e.g., auditory/visual cues like smartphone notifications) and internal stimuli (e.g., boredom, stress, or concerns related to news on social media), diverting the individual's attentional resources away from ongoing activities. Distraction may also manifest when internal and external cues are conflicting, leading the individual to struggle against interferences and maintain attentional focus on pertinent stimuli (i.e., selective attention) and goal-directed behaviours. Notably, Throuvala and colleagues [21] present a multidimensional model of smartphone distraction, distinguishing its dimensions as emotive, cognitive, and behavioural processes (i.e., Emotion regulation, Attention impulsiveness, Online vigilance, and Multitasking). The underlying assumption is that digital mobile technology affords individuals various activities that support and mediate distraction processes. However, the features of smartphones (e.g., portability, connectivity, etc.) facilitate distraction, potentially contributing to problematic smartphone use, such as prolonged online activities [22].
In the model proposed by Throuvala and colleagues [21], distraction functions as a strategy for Emotion Regulation when directed towards smartphone activities that alleviate negative states, primarily boredom, anxiety, and stress. As such, distraction serves as a coping mechanism, providing relief from negative emotions, while also facilitating the experience of positive emotions, such as gratification from gaming or receiving support through social media. Consequently, maintaining a constant connection becomes a strategic approach for managing unpleasant emotions in daily life. However, this may also give rise to a potential vicious circle, sustaining excessive smartphone use [23,24,25]. Following this conceptual framework, negative internal states may act as precursors to both distraction and smartphone overuse [21]. In alignment with this perspective, research indicates associations between excessive smartphone use and common emotional experiences such as anger, anxiety, depression, and loneliness [16]. Particularly during the COVID-19 pandemic, adolescents exhibited an increased disposition to use the internet as a means of managing depressive moods and thoughts, heightening the risk of engaging in Internet abuse behaviours [26]. Additionally, Yildiz [27] found that difficulties in regulating emotions are correlated with Internet Addiction (IA) and problematic smartphone use among adolescents. Similarly, in a study involving high school students, Extremera and colleagues [28] discovered that problematic smartphone users reported higher scores on maladaptive emotional regulation strategies (such as rumination, self-blame, etc.) compared to non-problematic users.
The second dimension, Attention Impulsivity, delineates a diminished control over attentive processes and an individual's proclivity to impulsively engage in activities with the smartphone (e.g., frequent checking of notifications or scrolling through Instagram). Individuals characterized by impulsivity often display a limited ability to control their impulses, compelling them towards the immediate need to interact with the smartphone (e.g., hastily clicking "Likes"). Additionally, the mechanisms of gratification and sensitivity to immediate rewards elucidate how impulsivity predisposes and perpetuates problematic online behaviours [29]. Empirical studies affirm that impulsivity constitutes a robust individual factor correlated with behavioural addiction and excessive smartphone usage [15,26]. Furthermore, heightened attentional impulsivity has been found to be associated with overuse of social media and Facebook among adolescents [30].
Online vigilance is defined as a "cognitive preoccupation and orientation toward social media content" ([21], p. 13) and manifests in behaviours such as incessantly monitoring the smartphone (e.g., interrupting tasks to check messages), being on alert, or contemplating social media content. The inclination towards online vigilance is often associated with the psychological need for reassurance, particularly in maintaining social support and communication through social media channels. This mode of distraction is further heightened by the Fear of Missing Out (FOMO), signifying the apprehension of missing out on important information or experiences within the realm of social media. Adolescents appear to be notably susceptible to FOMO, leading to a desire to uphold social connectivity, a continuous online presence, and a propensity to routinely check social media [31]. This perspective has prompted certain authors [32] to posit that FOMO can be regarded as a manifestation of Internet Addiction (IA), primarily affecting children and adolescents.
Finally, the Multitasking dimension manifests when simultaneous activities are conducted on a smartphone (media multitasking, such as using different applications) or when diverse behaviours (e.g., walking or talking) are carried out while utilizing the smartphone. Multitasking with smartphones is an exceedingly prevalent and pervasive phenomenon in daily life, to the extent that it is commonly perceived as synonymous with distraction [21], a viewpoint shared by educators and parents alike. Indeed, research demonstrates the adverse effects of media multitasking on the performance of primary tasks (e.g., students' homework). These consequences include increased execution times (due to repeated interruptions) and elevated error rates [33]. However, adopting a more optimistic standpoint [34], not all multitasking behaviours necessarily compromise performance. The impact depends, for in-stance, on the type of task-switching (e.g., visual and/or verbal) or the nature of attention (i.e., focused versus distributed attention) involved. Nevertheless, given that smartphones encourage multitasking behaviours that are intermittently rewarded, individuals often find themselves engrossed in continuous digital activities, leading to heightened levels of inattention (i.e., the allocation of attentive resources across tasks) and potential performance deterioration.
In conclusion, smartphone distraction emerges as a multifaceted phenomenon [21], comprising various cognitive-emotive and behavioural components that act as precursors or predisposing factors, influencing individuals' engagement with smartphones, while also being an outcome of their usage. Indeed, smartphone distraction is perpetuated over time due to the consequences of digital activities, such as online vigilance leading to preoccupation or constant orientation towards online notifications, interrupting attention. Simultaneously, these behaviours are reinforced by positive outcomes, such as the enjoyment of videos or engaging in friendly social exchanges. Aligned with literature that delineates trajectories towards smartphone overuse and addiction [6,15], prolonged smartphone use has enduring implications on concentration, cognitive processes, emotional well-being, and overall performance in the long term [16,35]. Consequently, it becomes imperative to explore how the various dimensions of smartphone distraction [21] contribute to the risk of developing Internet Addiction (IA) among adolescent smartphone users.

2. Aims and hypothesis

To the best of our knowledge, there is a dearth of studies investigating the correlation between Internet Addiction (IA) and the dimensions of distraction, as outlined by the conceptual framework proposed by Throuvala and colleagues (2021), specifically among adolescents. The current study seeks to explore, within a community sample of adolescents, the extent to which the four identified dimensions—namely, Emotion Regulation, Attention Impulsiveness, Online Vigilance, and Multitasking—are associated with IA related to smartphone usage. Secondly, building on existing literature that underscores the elevated incidence of internalizing symptoms [16,24,36] and externalizing symptoms [29,36] in problematic smartphone users, this study delves into the relationships be-tween neuropsychological factors in adolescents and IA.
The scope of this study is to identify the profile of adolescents most susceptible to developing problematic levels of Internet use through smartphones, as well as the primary predictors of their problematic involvement. To achieve the stated objectives, it is anticipated that:
(1) Elevated levels of Internet Addiction (IA) will demonstrate an association with heightened levels of smartphone distraction, encompassing Emotion Regulation, Attention Impulsiveness, Online Vigilance, and Multitasking.
(2) Increased levels of emotional problems and behavioural issues (e.g., hyperactivity/inattention and conduct/interpersonal problems) will exhibit an association with elevated levels of IA.
An additional objective is to replicate previous findings regarding smartphone distraction, as observed in adults [20,37], within an adolescent sample. Accordingly, the study initially seeks to confirm, with Italian adolescent participants, the structure of the Smartphone Distraction Scale. This scale is the original questionnaire developed by Throuvala and colleagues [21], designed to assess the cognitive, emotional, and behavioural processes involved in activities on multimedia devices.

3. Method

3.1. Participants

The study was undertaken in the metropolitan area of Messina, located in Sicily, Southern Italy, with a community sample comprising 676 adolescents aged between 15 and 19 years (mean age = 16.94, SD = 0.92; 42.2% female). Participants were recruited from public high schools, and the inclusion criterion mandated obtaining consent from both parents. Exclusion criteria encompassed the presence of conditions that impede the completion of self-report questionnaires, such as severe intellectual disabilities, neurological disorders, or difficulties in comprehending the Italian language by foreign students.

3.2. Measures

A paper-pencil battery comprising self-report questionnaires was administered to the partecipants.
Descriptive Internet use was assessed through a set of 14 dichotomous questions formulated based on the existing literature to discern the reasons for Internet use. Examples of items include inquiries such as "When you are online, do you use Social Networking Sites?" or "When you are online, do you use streaming services?" The items demonstrate satisfactory internal consistency, with a calculated alpha coefficient of 0.74.
Estimation of the perceived time spent online involved the administration of two ad hoc open-ended questions. Participants were asked to provide information regarding the time spent online during the weekdays and weekends. Respondents indicated the perceived number of hours (in minutes) dedicated to online activities.
Internet Addiction Test (IAT; [38]). Italian version of IAT [39] was utilized to evaluate the presence and severity of Internet and technology addiction. The IAT comprises 20 items, each associated with a 5-point Likert response scale ranging from “Never” to “Always.” Sample items include inquiries such as “Did you stay online longer than you intended?” or “Do you try to hide how much time you spend online?” A total score is computed by summing the rating for each item, with the maximum possible score being 100 points. Higher scores on the IAT indicate more substantial levels of Internet addiction, with a range of 50 to 79 indicating a moderate level of addiction, and scores from 80 to 100 indicating severe addiction. In this study, the IAT demonstrated commendable internal consistency (α = 0.86).
Smartphone Distraction Scale (SDS; [21]) comprises 16 items, each associated with a 5-point Likert response scale ranging from “Very rarely” to “Very often.” The scale is designed to measure the level of distraction related to smartphone use. The items are scored to generate four factors, each consisting of 4 items: Emotion Regulation (ER; e.g., “Using my phone distracts me when I’m under pressure”), Attention Impulsiveness (AI; e.g., “I get distracted by my phone even when my full attention is required on other tasks”), Online Vigilance (OV; e.g., “I get anxious if I don’t check messages immediately on my phone”), and Multitasking (MT; e.g., “I often talk to others while checking what’s on my phone”). To the best of our knowledge, validation in the Italian context has only been conducted on an adult sample [20]; thus, the factor structure was preliminarily verified with an adolescent sample in the present study. The SDS demonstrated an acceptable level of internal consistency for each sub-scale (αER = 0.78; αAI = 0.83; αOV = 0.71; and αMT = 0.69). Higher scores on the scale indicate a greater level of the measured dimensions.
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; [40]) assesses behavioural and emotional difficulties in childhood. Comprising 25 items, the questionnaire employs a 3-point Likert scale (i.e., “Not true,” “Partially true,” and “Absolutely true”) and is structured into five factors: Emotional Symptoms (ES; e.g., “Complaining of headache, stomach pain, or nausea”), Conduct Problems (CP; e.g., “Fights with other children or annoys them on purpose”), Hyperactivity-Inattention (HI; e.g., “Constantly moving or uncomfortable”), Peer Relationship Problems (PRP; e.g., “Have at least one good friend”), and Prosocial Behaviours (PB; e.g., “Respectful of the feelings of others”). The Italian version of the SDQ was developed by Marzocchi et al. (2004). In the present study, acceptable reliability was demonstrated for each sub-scale (αES = 0.76; αCP = 0.50; αHI = 0.64; αPRP = 0.55; and αPB = 0.59). Higher scores on the scale indicate a greater level of the measure dimensions.
Furtheremore, gender and age were gathered as demographic characteristics.

3.3. Procedure

The questionnaires were administered in the classroom following the receipt of authorizations from the headmaster and parents. Students individually completed the questionnaires after being provided information about the research's objectives and the assurance of response anonymity. No incentives were provided.

4. Data analysis

The analysis was conducted in four steps.
  • In the entire sample, a descriptive analysis (frequencies/percentages) was conducted to identify the primary online activities engaged in by adolescents. Additionally, an estimation of the time spent online during weekdays and weekends was performed, providing mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) values.
  • A preliminary data analysis involved the verification of the factorial structure of the SDS scale, based on the Italian validation among adults con-ducted by Mascia and colleagues [20]. Confirmatory analysis was executed using Diagonally Weighted Least Squares (DWLS) estimation with the Robust Method of estimation, applied to compute ordinated categorical variables (i.e., Likert scales) [41]. Fit indices such as Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) were considered for evaluating the structural model. These indices are widely recognized in the literature pertaining to Structural Equation Models (SEM) [42,43,44]. Specifically, an acceptable model was considered if GFI, CFI, and TLI values approached 1, while values close to 0 were expected for RMSEA and SRMR [44]. All analyses were conduct-ed using the Jamovi software with the SEMLJ module [45].
Subsequently, IAT and SDQ scores were obtained following the relative scoring procedures proposed by Italian validations and were incorporated into the analysis as standardized measures.
3.
Pearson's correlations were calculated to examine the associations between SDS and SDQ sub-scale scores with the IAT total score.
4.
Concerning Internet Addiction (IA), a problem group (PG; moderate or severe addiction) and a control group (CG; normal users) were identified. The selection of the two groups adhered to the cut-offs proposed by Young [46], as outlined in Table 3. The CG was equated for gender and age with the PG, which constituted a sub-sample of participants reporting moderate or severe IA levels. Subsequently, a series of Analyses of Variances (ANOVAs) were conducted to examine differences in the mean standard scores on SDS and SDQ. Finally, a binary logistic regression was executed to discern potential predictors of IA.
For step 3 and 4, standardized total scores were inserted in the analysis. The SPSS 26 software was employed for these stages of data analysis.

5. Results

Descriptively, adolescents report engaging in online activities for a variety of purposes (Table 1). They are more frequently involved in the use of Social Networking Sites, streaming and communication services, followed by the search for information. Furthermore, adolescents converged in choosing the smartphone as the main device to being on the web (94.7%). Females report more hours (estimated in minutes) than males both in ferial days (MFfd = 428.45, SDFfd = 252.91, vs. MMfd = 345.00, SDMfd 253.29) and during weekend (MFwe = 458.07, SDFwe 279.24, vs. MMwe = 383.56, SDMwe 324.15). The Mann-Whitney test shows differences between male and female on time spent online (Ufd = 41701.50; p < 0.001; Uwe = 43761.50; p < 0.001).

5.1. Structure of Smartphone Distraction Scale

Building upon the prior validation study of the Italian SDS conducted with adults [20], the factorial structure of the SDS was examined with the adolescents enrolled in this study. The analysis affirms a structure of items organized into 4 factors, consistent with the original questionnaire [21] and the Italian version [20] and demonstrates acceptable fit indices: RMSEA = 0.07 [95% CI = 0.06 / 0.08]; SRMR = 0.06; CFI = 0.98; TLI = 0.98.

5.2. Correlations between IA, SDS and SDQ

Table 2 shows the Pearson’s correlation coefficients between IA and the sub-dimensions of the SDS and SDQ. Specifically, higher levels of IA significantly correlate with SDS (i.e., Emotional regulation, Attentional impulsivity, Online vigilance, Multitasking), and SDQ subscales (i.e., Emotional problems, Conduct problems, Hyperactivity/inattention, Peer relationship problems). Higher levels of IA, higher levels of SDS and SDQ subscales. Conversely, a negative correlation was found with SDQ Prosocial behaviours. Higher levels of IA, lower levels of prosocial behaviours.

5.3. Problematic Internet users versus control

5.3.1. Identification of Problematic and Control Group

Utilizing Internet Addiction (IA) scores (Table 3), a subsample of 169 participants was selected. Those with moderate and severe addiction levels constituted the problematic group (PG), while those with normal levels formed the control group (CG). Table 4 displays frequencies and percentages of the subsamples, balanced for gender and age.
Table 3. IAT cut-off distribution.
Table 3. IAT cut-off distribution.
Score Frequency %
Normal level (≤ 30) 85 12.6
Mild level (31-49) 437 64.6
Moderate level (50-79) 152 22.5
Severe addiction (80-100) 2 0.3
Total 676 100.0
Table 4. Balanced subsamples.
Table 4. Balanced subsamples.
Variable Frequency (%)
PG CG
Gender Male 65 (48.9) 68 (51.1)
Female 19 (52.8) 17 (47.2)
Age 16 35 (50.0) 35 (50.0)
17 27 (51.9) 25 (48.1)
18 18 (47.4) 20 (52.6)
19 4 (44.4) 5 (55.6)

5.4.1. Comparisons between problematic and control groups

The ANOVAs show the comparisons between sub-groups on SDS and SDQ subscales (Table 5). In contrast to CG, the PG was characterized by higher scores on SDS Emotional regulation, Attention impulsiveness, Online vigilance, Multitasking, and SDQ Emotional problems, Conduct problems, Hyperactivity/inattention, whereas lower SDQ Prosocial behaviours.
Table 6 presents the parameters of the Binary Logistic Regression Analysis. A significant effect of hyperactivity/inattention was identified for problematic online users (β = 0.74; p < 0.01). This finding indicates that higher scores on the hyperactivity/inattention dimension elevate the probability of belonging to the problematic group (PG), whereas lower scores on the same dimension characterize the control group (CG). Regarding gen-der, there is a trend toward statistical significance, suggesting that males tend to belong to the problematic group.

6. Discussion

This study confirms that adolescents primarily utilize the Internet for various purposes, including information searching, watching videos, listening to music, communication, and engaging in social net-working, among others. Notably, the smartphone emerges as the most widely adopted device for online connectivity, with a prevalence of 94.7% in the study sample. The re-search endeavours to elucidate the profile of adolescents most susceptible to developing problematic levels of Internet Addiction (IA) through smartphone use. To our knowledge, no previous studies have delved into the correlation between the dimensions of distraction, as conceptualized by Throuvala and colleagues in 2021 [21], and IA within adolescent samples. Past investigations have explored distraction dimensions, operationalized in the SDS (i.e., Emotion regulation, Attention impulsiveness, Online vigilance, and Multitasking), among adults. These studies revealed positive associations with problematic mobile use [20], fear of missing out (FOMO), metacognitions (e.g., advantages of smartphone use as a means to distract oneself from worries), and smartphone addiction [22]. A noteworthy strength of the current study lies in its shift of focus towards adolescents, the generation characterized as both the most avid users of digital devices and the most susceptible to pathological involvement and addiction [6,15]. Consequently, recognizing the cognitive-emotive and behavioural profile of adolescents potentially at risk of developing various forms of technology addiction becomes imperative for effective prevention and intervention strategies.
The findings provide empirical support for the four-factor dimensions of the Italian SDS when applied to adolescents. Throuvala and colleagues [21] previously validated the SDS using a sample of British university students, identifying 16 items organized into four subdimensions: Emotional regulation, Attention impulsiveness, Online vigilance, and Multitasking. In the Italian context, Mascia and colleagues [20] conducted a validation study with adult participants, affirming a four-factor structure identical to that of the original SDS. In our study, utilizing the Italian item translations provided by Mascia and colleagues [20], we implemented a factorial model, which yielded results consistent with the established structure prevalent in the existing literature. To our knowledge, there is currently no validation of the SDS in the Italian context specifically with a sample of adolescents. Consequently, while this result allows us to apply the SDS to the adolescent sample in our study, it also underscores the need for more targeted validation research within the adolescent demographic. This dual perspective emphasizes the significance of both utilizing the SDS in our current investigation and promoting further validation studies tailored to the unique characteristics of adolescent populations in the Italian context.
In accordance with the hypothesis, all SDS dimensions were found to be associated with higher levels of Internet Addiction (IA), thus substantiating the assertion that adolescents with problematic engagement in online activities exhibit elevated attention interruption (i.e., multitasking), cognitive-emotive preoccupation with the smartphone (i.e., online vigilance), and employ smartphone activities as a coping strategy to alleviate negative emotional states (i.e., emotion regulation). These findings align coherently with the developmental literature regarding the psychological functions of smartphones, such as online chatting, connecting with social media networks, and engaging in activities like gambling, among others, within the context of adolescence. Specifically, adolescents consistently access social media platforms to communicate with peers, share experiences, and receive emotional support, thereby expressing a sense of connection and belongingness [47]. This motivation propels them to engage in frequent online activities. Moreover, as elucidated by Griffiths [48], the habit of using social networking sites can be comprehended by considering the impact on users' reward systems, which tend to be unpredictable and random. An illustrative example of a reward is the receipt of "Likes" from other users, contributing to an increased "desire for validation" and prompting repeated visits to check for appreciation on social platforms. Nevertheless, intensive use of social media is correlated with adolescents' smartphone overuse [49], contemplation of social content [50], and apprehension about losing or being excluded from enjoyable online experiences (i.e., FOMO; [51]). Our results are consistent with the studies affirming that the social use of the Internet through smartphones represents a potent source of distraction among adolescents [52].
The utilization of smartphone distraction as a strategy for emotion regulation is also found to be significantly correlated with adolescents' Internet Addiction (IA). In this context, smartphone distraction serves to momentarily alleviate the distress and negative mood perceived by adolescents, yet it functions as a maladaptive coping strategy that perpetuates smartphone overuse. These findings align with prior research indicating that adolescents experiencing stress, anxiety, boredom, or loneliness tend to exhibit excessive online activities and are prone to smartphone overuse [16,27,53,54]. Furthermore, Marino and colleagues [50] observed that emotional regulation predicts the usage of social networking sites among adolescents. It is noted that young individuals are inclined to spend increased time on social networking sites during periods of negative emotional states. The internet is often compulsively employed with the specific aim of "altering" mood, reflecting a pattern consistent with the present study's observations.
It is well-established that pre-existing psychological issues contribute to excessive smartphone use and, concurrently, serve as one of the pathways leading to addiction [15]. Individuals engage with their mobile devices with the aim of enhancing their well-being, yet their emotional challenges, such as social anxiety or depression [16] are also a consequence of the addictive nature of the Internet. In the context of this study, elevated levels of attentional impulsivity – manifested as an inability to focus attention or concentrate on a primary task – were found to be associated with IA. During adolescence, a diminished attentional control can be elucidated as a correlate of incomplete brain development, resulting in observed difficulties in various cognitive tasks, including selective attention, working memory, and inhibitory control [55]. Research affirms that adolescents' immaturity in inhibitory control, self-regulation, and attentional control is linked to smartphone addiction, with higher rates of smartphone addiction observed among children and young people aged 10-20 compared to adults aged 21-30 [56]. Particularly noteworthy, the findings from the current study contribute to existing research on the role of impulsivity [26], in conjunction with disinhibition and susceptibility to boredom (or sensation seeking [57]; as vulnerability factor for problematic smartphone use during adolescence.
In summary, the outcomes of the current study not only endorse the expansion of the application of Throuvala's theoretical framework to a younger demographic of smartphone users but also endeavour to delineate a profile of individuals exhibiting problematic usage patterns. Following the identification of non-problematic adolescent users and those manifesting problematic levels of Internet Addiction as per Young's IAT, a comparative analysis of smartphone distraction dimensions and internalizing/externalizing symptoms was conducted across the two groups. Adolescents categorized within the problematic usage group reported more elevated levels in all dimensions of smartphone distraction, along with heightened emotional issues (e.g., anxiety, negative mood) and disruptive problems (e.g., hyperactivity/inattention and conduct/interpersonal problems). These findings contribute to the existing body of literature elucidating the association between internalizing/externalizing problems and problematic smartphone use during adolescence [36,58].
However, the paramount outcome of the study lies in the identification of heightened levels of hyperactivity/inattention as a precursor to IA. The correlation between symptoms of hyperactivity/inattention associated with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and IA has been extensively documented in the literature ([10] for a comprehensive review). Kim and colleagues [36] uncovered a robust link between ADHD symptoms and smartphone addiction in middle- and high-school students. Likewise, symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity predicted IA among university students [59]. This empirical support is elucidated by studies underscoring the pivotal role of impulsivity, a core symptom of ADHD, and sensitivity to rewards in the development of technology addiction, including IA, smartphone addiction, social media addiction, and gambling [10,60]. Adolescents exhibiting ADHD traits, particularly those with deficiencies in inhibition and cognitive control [10], may find smartphone activities exceptionally appealing, given the ability to seamlessly switch between multiple functions, respond rapidly, and receive immediate rewards. Furthermore, the inclinations towards delay aversion, sensation-seeking, and heightened sensitivity to stimulating activities among individuals with ADHD symptoms contribute to an increased propensity for technology use, owing to the highly stimulating nature of online activities [61,62]. Additional studies emphasize emotional dysregulation, characterized by an inability to effectively regulate aroused emotions, and the stimulation of positive affective states among individuals with ADHD [63]. Balca and colleagues [64] observed a connection between problematic online engagement (as per Young's Internet Addiction Test levels) and challenges in emotion regulation among adolescents diagnosed with ADHD. These findings collectively suggest that emotional dysregulation may contribute to problematic smartphone use among individuals with ADHD.
Collectively, research corroborates the role of ADHD traits – encompassing attention deficits [65] and impulsivity [60] – as predisposing factors for IA within non-clinical populations. The neurodevelopmental nature of ADHD positions these traits at the extreme end of a continuum, where in a subclinical manifestation, they may also be present in the general population. Given the early onset of ADHD symptoms and the decreasing age at which individuals begin using smartphones, it is advisable to conduct further studies aimed at screening children who may be more susceptible to developing technology addiction.

Limitations

The findings of this study are subject to certain limitations that warrant consideration. Firstly, the results are derived from a convenience sample of high school students in a city in southern Italy; therefore, generalizability across the broader Italian youth population is constrained. Secondly, due to the self-reported nature of the data, the possibility of bias in reported online activities cannot be entirely dismissed. Future investigations incorporating objective assessment methods (as exemplified by Coyne and colleagues [66]) could offer a more comprehensive exploration of the associations between smartphone distraction and various online activities (e.g., watching movies, browsing social networking sites) engaged in by adolescents. Similarly, it would be worthwhile to delve into which specific online activities (e.g., shopping, social media networking) may induce higher levels of smartphone distraction. Thirdly, internalizing and externalizing symptoms were self-reported by participants using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; [40]). Although the SDQ is a widely recognized screening scale and it was employed in studies on behavioural problems associated with adolescents' smartphone overuse (e.g., [58], its selection aligns with the exploratory nature of the present study. However, future investigations with clinical samples could further explore the reciprocal influences between internalizing factors (e.g., depression, anxiety, loneliness) or externalizing disorders (e.g., ADHD) and the cognitive-emotive aspects of smartphone distraction. Specifically, the results of this study underscore the significance of high levels of hyperactivity/inattention symptoms compared to other individual characteristics (i.e., conduct problems, emotional issues, poor prosocial behaviours) in predicting Internet Addiction (IA) among problematic smartphone users. Subsequent studies with clinical samples may elucidate how core symptoms of ADHD, such as impulsivity [15,67] or hyperactivity/inattention [10,59,65] contribute to distraction and smartphone overuse.

7. Conclusion

The study carries significant implications for clinical research, as well as for preventive and public health measures. A considerable proportion of adolescents exhibit a pronounced degree of Internet usage through their smartphones. Moreover, cognitive-emotive distraction processes that impede everyday functioning may come into play. As posited by Throuvala and colleagues [21], it is plausible that multiple dimensions simultaneusly contribute to problematic Internet use, particularly through smartphones. For instance, a bored student (engaging in emotion regulation) may continually check their smartphone (manifesting online vigilance) while awaiting notifications, thereby interrupting the ongoing homework (engaging in multitasking). Nevertheless, individuals with problematic Internet use demonstrate elevated levels of smartphone distraction, and in the presence of heightened levels of hyperactivity/inattention, a vulnerability towards overusing the Internet via smartphones appears more probable. This holds particular relevance for clinical research objectives.
Moreover, the present study underscores the necessity of conducting screenings using comprehensive instruments (such as SDS and SDQ) to obtain increasingly precise information for subsequent interventions. While more thorough and structured validation studies with adolescents are imperative, the SDS could prove beneficial for screening and interventions within school populations [68]. It can be employed to instruct individuals on effective management of negative emotions, identification of external factors (e.g., notification cues) or internal conditions (i.e., thoughts or emotions) preceding Internet access, and the promotion of self-regulation in smartphone use. As empirically-based evidence, a cognitive-behavioral intervention focusing on mindful attention, self-monitoring, and mood self-awareness demonstrated effectiveness in reducing smartphone distraction and daily smartphone usage among university students [69].
Hence, the present study contributes to comprehending Internet Addiction (IA) as a complex, multidimensional phenomenon linked to various individual (such as impulsivity and emotional states) and contextual factors (including parenting or school learning activities) that play a role in the discomfort experienced by adolescents.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.B. and E.G.; methodology, L.B. and E.G.; software, S.R.; formal analysis, S.R. and M.I.; resources, G.D.R..; data curation, A.C. and R.P.; writing—original draft preparation, L.B and S.R.; writing—review and editing, L.B., A.G. and M.I.; supervision, L.B. and M.I. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethical Committee of the CeRIP – Center for Psychological Research and Intervention of the University of Messina (protocol code 146435, 16.11.2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the schools that allowed this study to be carried out and the students for their willingness to compile questionnaire on Internet Addiction.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Anderson, M., & Jiang, J. (2018). Teens, social media & technology 2018. Pew Research Center, 31(2018), 1673-1689.
  2. Han, G., Zhang, J., Ma, S., Lu, R., Duan, J., Song, Y., & Lau, P. W. (2021). Prevalence of internet addiction and its relationship with combinations of physical activity and screen-based sedentary behavior among adolescents in China. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 18(10), 1245-1252. [CrossRef]
  3. Smahel, D., Machackova, H., Mascheroni, G., Dedkova, L., Staksrud, E., Ólafsson, K., Livingstone, S., Hasebrink, U. (2020). EU Kids Online 2020: Survey results from 19 countries. EU Kids Online 2020. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12162/5299.
  4. Spina, G., Bozzola, E., Ferrara, P., Zamperini, N., Marino, F., Caruso, C., Antilici, L., & Villani, A. (2021). Children and Adolescent’s perception of media device Use consequences. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(6), 1-8. [CrossRef]
  5. Ingrassia, M., Cedro, G., Puccio, S., Benedetto, L. (2021). Adolescents Suspended in the Space-Time: Problematic Use of Smartphone between Dissociative Symptoms and Flow Experiences. In M. Ingrassia & L. Benedetto (eds.), Adolescences. IntechOpen. [CrossRef]
  6. Lozano-Blasco, R., Latorre-Martínez, M. P., Cortés-Pascual, A. (2022). Screen addicts: A meta-analysis of internet addiction in adolescence. Children and Youths Services Review,135(2022), 1-11. [CrossRef]
  7. Montag, C., & Reuter, M. (2017). Internet addiction. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.
  8. Machimbarrena Garagorri, J. M., Gonzalez Cabrera, J., Ortega Baron, J., Beranuy Fargues, M., Alvarez Bardon, A., & Tejero, B. (2019). Profiles of Problematic Internet Use and Its Impact on Adolescents' Health-Related Quality of Life. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(20), 1-17. [CrossRef]
  9. Kliesener, T., Meigen, C., Kiess, W., Poulain, T. (2022). Associations between problematic smartphone use and behavioural difficulties, quality of life, and school performance among children and adolescents. BMC Psychiatry, 22(1),195-207. [CrossRef]
  10. Wang, P., Zhao, M., Wang, X., Xie, X., Wang, Y., & Lei, L. (2017). Peer relationship and adolescent smartphone addiction: The mediating role of self-esteem and the moderating role of the need to belong. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 708-717. [CrossRef]
  11. Serra, G., Lo Scalzo, L., Giuffrè, Ferrara, P., Corsello, G. (2021). Smartphone use and addiction during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic: cohort study on 184 Italian children and adolescents. Italian Journal of Pediatrics, 47, 150-160. [CrossRef]
  12. Burkauskas, J., Gecaite-Stonciene, J., Demetrovics, Z., Griffiths, M. D., & Király, O. (2022). Prevalence of problematic Internet use during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 46, 1-6. [CrossRef]
  13. Rollo, S., Venuleo, C., Ferrante, L., & De Luca Picione, R. (2023). What adolescents have to say about problematic internet use: a qualitative study based on focus groups. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(21), 1-19. [CrossRef]
  14. Montag, C., Wegmann, E., Sariyska, R., Demetrovics, Z., & Brand, M. (2021). How to overcome taxonomical problems in the study of Internet use disorders and what to do with “smartphone addiction”?. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 9(4), 908-914. [CrossRef]
  15. Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Current Addiction Reports, 2(2), 156-162. [CrossRef]
  16. Wacks, Y., & Weinstein, A. M. (2021). Excessive smartphone use is associated with health problems in adolescents and young adults. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 762-774. [CrossRef]
  17. Firth, J., Torous, J., Stubbs, B., Firth, J. A., Steiner, G. Z., Smith, L., Alvarez-Jimenez, M., Gleeson, J., Vancampfort, D., Christopher, J., & Sarris, J. (2019). The “online brain”: how the Internet may be changing our cognition. World Psychiatry, 18(2), 119-129. [CrossRef]
  18. Liebherr, M., Schubert, P., Antons, S., Montag, C., & Brand, M. (2020). Smartphones and attention, curse or blessing?A review on the effects of smartphone usage on attention, inhibition, and working memory. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 1, 1-8. [CrossRef]
  19. Skowronek, J., Seifert, A., & Lindberg, S. (2023). The mere presence of a smartphone reduces basal attentional performance. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 1-9. [CrossRef]
  20. Mascia, M. L., Agus, M., Tomczyk, Ł., Bonfiglio, N. S., Bellini, D., & Penna, M. P. (2023). Smartphone Distraction: Italian Validation of the Smartphone Distraction Scale (SDS). International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(15), 1-12. [CrossRef]
  21. Throuvala, M. A., Pontes, H. M., Tsaousis, I., Griffiths, M. D., Rennoldson, M., & Kuss, D. J. (2021). Exploring the dimensions of smartphone distraction: Development, validation, measurement invariance, and latent mean differences of the smartphone distraction scale (SDS). Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 1-21. [CrossRef]
  22. Zhao, X., Hu, T., Qiao, G., Li, C., Wu, M., Yang, F., & Zhou, J. (2022). Psychometric properties of the smartphone distraction scale in Chinese college students: validity, reliability and influencing factors. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 1-11. [CrossRef]
  23. Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251-259. [CrossRef]
  24. Elhai, J. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., Levine, J. C., Picard, K. J., & Hall, B. J. (2018). Depression and emotion regulation predict objective smartphone use measured over one week. Personality and Individual Differences, 133, 21-28. [CrossRef]
  25. Zhitomirsky-Geffet, M., & Blau, M. (2016). Cross-generational analysis of predictive factors of addictive behavior in smartphone usage. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 682-693. [CrossRef]
  26. Diotaiuti, P., Girelli, L., Mancone, S., Corrado, S., Valente, G., & Cavicchiolo, E. (2022). Impulsivity and depressive brooding in internet addiction: a study with a sample of Italian adolescents during COVID-19 lockdown. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 1-12. [CrossRef]
  27. Yildiz, M. A. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies as predictors of internet addiction and smartphone addiction in adolescents. Journal of Educational Sciences and Psychology, 7(1), 66-78.
  28. Extremera, N., Quintana-Orts, C., Sánchez-Álvarez, N., & Rey, L. (2019). The role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies on problematic smartphone use: Comparison between problematic and non-problematic adolescent users. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(17), 1-13. [CrossRef]
  29. Guo, Z., Liang, S., Ren, L., Yang, T., Qiu, R., He, Y., & Zhu, X. (2022). Applying network analysis to understand the relationships between impulsivity and social media addiction and between impulsivity and problematic smartphone use. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 1-11. [CrossRef]
  30. Gul, H., Yurumez Solmaz, E., Gul, A., & Oner, O. (2018). Facebook overuse and addiction among Turkish adolescents: Are ADHD and ADHD-related problems risk factors? Psychiatry and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 28(1), 80–90. [CrossRef]
  31. Bloemen, N., & De Coninck, D. (2020). Social Media and Fear of Missing Out in Adolescents: The Role of Family Characteristics. Social Media + Society, 6(4), 1-11. [CrossRef]
  32. Tomczyk, L., Selmanagic-Lizde, E. (2018). Fear of missing out (FOMO) among youth in Bosnia and Herzegovina—Scale and selected mechanisms. Children and Youth Services Review, 88, 541–549. [CrossRef]
  33. Mrazek, A. J., Mrazek, M. D., Ortega, J. R., Ji, R. R., Karimi, S. S., Brown, C. S., Alexander, C. A., Khan, M., Panahi, R., Sadoff, M., Scott, A., Tyszka, J. R. E., & Schooler, J. W. (2021). Teenagers’ smartphone use during homework: an analysis of beliefs and behaviors around digital multitasking. Education Sciences, 11(11), 713-723. [CrossRef]
  34. Courage, M. L., Bakhtiar, A., Fitzpatrick, C., Kenny, S., & Brandeau, K. (2015). Growing up multitasking: The costs and benefits for cognitive development. Developmental Review, 35, 5–41. [CrossRef]
  35. Chu, J., Qaisar, S., Shah, Z., & Jalil, A. (2021). Attention or distraction? The impact of mobile phone on users' psychological well-being. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 1-12. [CrossRef]
  36. Kim, S. G., Park, J., Kim, H. T., Pan, Z., Lee, Y., & McIntyre, R. S. (2019). The relationship between smartphone addiction and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity in South Korean adolescents. Annals of General Psychiatry, 18(1), 1-8. [CrossRef]
  37. Zhao, X., Hu, T., Qiao, G., Li, C., Wu, M., Yang, F., & Zhou, J. (2022). Psychometric properties of the smartphone distraction scale in Chinese college students: validity, reliability and influencing factors. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 1-11. [CrossRef]
  38. Young, K.S. (1998). Caught in the net: How to recognize the signs of internet addiction--and a winning strategy for recovery. John Wiley & Sons.
  39. Ferraro, G., Caci, B., D'Amico, A., & Blasi, M. D. (2006). Internet addiction disorder: an Italian study. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10(2), 170-175. [CrossRef]
  40. Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337-1345. [CrossRef]
  41. Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. É., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 354–373. [CrossRef]
  42. Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238–246. [CrossRef]
  43. Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: a Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1-55. [CrossRef]
  44. Pastore, M. (2012). Modelli di equazioni strutturali per variabili osservate: una nota sugli indici di adattamento. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 39(2), 435-440. [CrossRef]
  45. Gallucci, M., Jentschke, S. (2021). SEMLj: jamovi SEM Analysis. [jamovi module]. Retrieved from https://semlj.github.io/.
  46. Young, K. (2009). Internet addiction: Diagnosis and treatment considerations. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 39, 241-246. [CrossRef]
  47. Allen, K. A., Ryan, T., Gray, D. L., McInerney, D. M., & Waters, L. (2014). Social media use and social connectedness in adolescents: The positives and the potential pitfalls. The Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 31(1), 18-31. [CrossRef]
  48. Griffiths, M. D. (2018). Adolescent social networking: How do social media operators facilitate habitual use?. Education and Health, 36(3), 66-69.
  49. Fischer-Grote, L., Kothgassner, O.D., & Felnhofer, A. (2019). Risk factors for problematic smartphone use in children and adolescents: a review of existing literature. Neuropsychiatry, 33, 179–190. [CrossRef]
  50. Marino, C., Canale, N., Melodia, F., Spada, M. M., & Vieno, A. (2021). The overlap between problematic smartphone use and problematic social media use: a systematic review. Current Addiction Reports, 8, 1-12. [CrossRef]
  51. Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). “I don’t want to miss a thing”: Adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’ social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 1-8. [CrossRef]
  52. Siebers, T., Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2022). Explaining variation in adolescents’ social media-related distraction: The role of social connectivity and disconnectivity factors. Current Psychology, 1-14. [CrossRef]
  53. Gioia, F., Fioravanti, G., Casale, S., & Boursier, V. (2021). The effects of the fear of missing out on people's social networking sites use during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of online relational closeness and individuals' online communication attitude. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 1-11. [CrossRef]
  54. Karaer, Y., & Akdemir, D. (2019). Parenting styles, perceived social support and emotion regulation in adolescents with internet addiction. Comprehensive psychiatry, 92, 22-27. [CrossRef]
  55. Willhelm, A. R., Fortes, P. M., Czermainski, F. R., Rates, A. S. A., & Almeida, R. M. M. D. (2016). Neuropsychological and behavioral assessment of impulsivity in adolescents: a systematic review. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 38, 128-135. [CrossRef]
  56. Randler, C., Wolfgang, L., Matt, K., Demirhan, E., Horzum, M. B., & Besoluk, S. (2016). Smartphone addiction proneness in relation to sleep and morningness-eveningness in German adolescents. Journal of Behavior Addiction, 5(3), 465–473. [CrossRef]
  57. Pérez de Albéniz Garrote, G., Rubio, L., Medina Gómez, B., & Buedo-Guirado, C. (2021). Smartphone abuse amongst adolescents: the role of impulsivity and sensation seeking. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 746626. [CrossRef]
  58. Poulain, T., Vogel, M., Kliesener, T., & Kiess, W. (2021). Associations between changes in behavioral difficulties and levels of problematic smartphone use in adolescents over a 1-year period. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1-4. [CrossRef]
  59. Zakaria, H., Hussain, I., Zulkifli, N. S. A., Ibrahim, N., Noriza, N. J., Wong, M., Abd Latif, M. H. (2023). Internet addiction and its relationship with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms, anxiety and stress among university students in Malaysia. PloS one, 18(7), e0283862. [CrossRef]
  60. Rømer Thomsen, K., Callesen, M. B., Hesse, M., Kvamme, T. L., Pedersen, M. M., Pedersen, M. U., & Voon, V. (2018). Impulsivity traits and addiction-related behaviors in youth. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 317-330. [CrossRef]
  61. Chou, W. J., Chang, Y. P., & Yen, C. F. (2018). Boredom proneness and its correlation with Internet addiction and Internet activities in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The Kaohsiung Journal of Medical Sciences, 34(8), 467-474. [CrossRef]
  62. Benedetto L., Gullotta R., Ingrassia M. (2015). ADHD, sensation seeking and problematic internet use in adolescence. Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, 19(3), 511-521. [CrossRef]
  63. Bunford, N., Evans, S. W., & Wymbs, F. (2015). ADHD and emotion dysregulation among children and adolescents. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 18, 185-217. [CrossRef]
  64. Balca, Y., Kardaş, B., & Kardaş, Ö. (2023). Relationship of Problematic Internet Use with Emotion Regulation in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Kocaeli Üniversitesi Sağlık Bilimleri Dergisi, 9(3), 177-183. [CrossRef]
  65. Aydin, Ü., Cañigueral, R., Tye, C., & McLoughlin, G. (2023). Face processing in young adults with autism and ADHD: An event related potentials study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 14, 1080681. [CrossRef]
  66. Coyne, P., Voth, J., & Woodruff, S. J. (2023). A comparison of self-report and objective measurements of smartphone and social media usage. Telematics and Informatics Reports, 10, 1-9. [CrossRef]
  67. Wallace, J., Boers, E., Ouellet, J., Afzali, M. H., & Conrod, P. (2023). Screen time, impulsivity, neuropsychological functions and their relationship to growth in adolescent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 1-14. [CrossRef]
  68. Mac Cárthaigh, S. (2020). The effectiveness of interventions to reduce excessive mobile device usage among adolescents: A systematic review. Neurology, Psychiatry and Brain Research, 35, 29-37. [CrossRef]
  69. Throuvala, M. A., Griffiths, M. D., Rennoldson, M., & Kuss, D. J. (2020). Mind over matter: testing the efficacy of an online randomized controlled trial to reduce distraction from smartphone use. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(13), 1-30. [CrossRef]
Table 1. Frequencies and percentage of adolescents’ online activities.
Table 1. Frequencies and percentage of adolescents’ online activities.
Online activity Frequency
(n/676)
Percentage
(%)
Finding information 446 66.0
Gaming 286 42.3
Social Networking Site 508 75.1
Streaming service 508 75.1
Communication 508 75.1
Shopping 314 46.4
Blog/Forum 49 7.2
Other 19 2.8
Table 2. Pearson's r correlation coefficients between SDS, SDQ and IAT measures (N = 676).
Table 2. Pearson's r correlation coefficients between SDS, SDQ and IAT measures (N = 676).
Variable Sub-dimension Correlation (r) with IAT total score
SDS Emotion Regulation 0.249**
Attention Impulsiveness 0.190**
Online Vigilance 0.204**
Multitasking 0.183**
SDQ Emotional Problems 0.327**
Conduct problems 0.356**
Hyperactivity / Inattention 0.400**
Peer Relationship problems 0.158**
Prosocial Behaviour -0.130**
Note: SDS = Smartphone Addiction Scale; SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; IAT = Internet Addiction Test (total score).
** The correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed).
Table 5. Comparisons of PG versus CG (ANOVAs; means and SDs are expressed in z points).
Table 5. Comparisons of PG versus CG (ANOVAs; means and SDs are expressed in z points).
Variable Mean (SD) F Sign.
CG PG
SDS Emotion regulation -0.196 (0.574) 0.158 (0.636) 13.982 All ps < .01
Attention impulsiveness -0.201 (0.740) 0.160 (0.808) 8.863
Online vigilance -0.153 (0.652) 0.160 (0.697) 8.772
Multitasking -0.100 (0.535) 0.152 (0.584) 8.278
SDQ Emotional problems -0.594 (0.923) 0.101 (0.919) 24.120
Conduct problems -0.542 (0.834) 0.467 (1.239) 38.712
Hyperactivity/Inattention -0.696 (0.848) 0.310 (0.987) 50.534
Peer relationship problems -0.222 (0.995) 0.338 (1,044) 12.730
Prosocial behaviour 0.271 (0.954) -0.313 (1,060) 14.193
Note: SDS = Smartphone Addiction Scale; SDQ: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; CG = Control Group; PG = Problematic Group.
Table 6. Regression coefficients (β) and relative statistics for IAT criterion variable.
Table 6. Regression coefficients (β) and relative statistics for IAT criterion variable.
Indipendent variable β SE Wald df p value Exp (β)
SDS Emotion regulation 0.831 0.462 3.242 1 0.072 2.296
Attention impulsiveness 0.001 0.620 0.000 1 0.998 1.001
Online vigilance -0.090 0.600 0.023 1 0.880 0.914
Multitasking -0.013 0.621 0.000 1 0.983 0.987
SDQ Emotional problems 0.208 0.248 0.703 1 0.402 1.231
Conduct problems 0.420 0.242 3.017 1 0.082 1.523
Hyperactivity/Inattention 0.745 0.242 9.490 1 0.002 2.107
Peer relationship problems 0.179 0.216 0.685 1 0.408 1.196
Prosocial behaviours -0.187 0.221 0.720 1 0.396 0.829
Gender -1.198 0.620 3.730 1 0.053 0.302
Constant 0.245 0.210 1.363 1 0.243 1.277
Note: SDS = Smartphone Addiction Scale; SDQ: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire;
Gender (dummy; 0 = Male, 1 = Female).
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated