Preprint
Article

Classical and modern prejudice toward Individuals with Intellectual Disabilities: The role of Experienced Contact, Beliefs in a Just World, and Social Dominance Orientation

Submitted:

01 February 2024

Posted:

02 February 2024

You are already at the latest version

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Abstract
This study investigates the impact of experienced contact on prejudiced attitudes toward individuals with intellectual disabilities (ID), examining beliefs in a just world (BJW) and social dominance orientation (SDO) as potential serial mediators. Data were collected from 224 university students (M = 23.02, SD = 2.48). Path analysis modeling assessed the structural relationships between the study variables. The findings revealed that experienced contact was negatively and significantly associated with BJW and SDO. Additionally, BJW and SDO fully mediated the relationship between experienced contact and overt prejudice. These findings underscore the influence of individual differences on attitudes toward individuals with ID, establishing a crucial foundation for future research and the development of interventions aimed at reducing prejudice and discrimination.
Keywords: 
;  ;  ;  ;  
Subject: 
Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Introduction

According to [1], intellectual disabilities (ID) are characterised by severe limits in cognitive functions and adaptive behavior across a wide variety of everyday tasks evident before age 18. Neurodevelopmental disorders like ID are classified as “Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (IDD)” in the most recent international classifications, along with conditions like Autism Spectrum Disorders, Attention Hyperactivity Disorder, Learning Disabilities, Motor Developmental Disabilities, and Communication Disorders [2]. Prior studies indicate that, despite advancements in understanding and accepting various disabilities, people with ID are frequently the target of stigma and prejudice (e.g., [3,4,5,6]). Furthermore, studies have highlighted that individuals with ID constitute one of the most marginalized and stigmatized groups in society [e.g., [5,7]]. Overcoming prejudice toward individuals with ID represents a significant challenge to achieving a truly inclusive and equitable society [8].
Negative attitudes toward disabled individuals could impede their social participation and integration [9,10,11,12]. Moreover, prejudice toward individuals with ID remains a significant societal issue, hindering their social inclusion and overall personal well-being [13]. This underscores the importance of fostering an inclusive society, aligning with the fundamental principles of the Sustainable Development Goals (SDG). It involves promoting positive contact between individuals with and without ID and challenging negative stereotypes through media campaigns and advocacy strategies.
Prejudice describes a tendency to evaluate members of out-groups as less than in some way than in-group members [14]. It is a viewpoint shaped by personal beliefs and preexisting ideas, lacking a genuine understanding of the relevant facts and individuals. In recent years, scholars have started differentiating between classical (e.g., old-fashioned, blatant, overt) and modern (e.g., subtle, covert) manifestations of prejudice. More specifically, classical forms involve direct or open prejudice, while modern forms are characterized by their covert or subtle nature [e.g., [14,15]]. This twofold conceptualization of prejudice has been adopted in various social and cultural contexts for different types of discrimination toward minority groups [17] as well as toward people affected by ID [18].
This study aims to contribute to a better understanding of the role of variables associated with negative attitudes toward persons with ID. Negative attitudes perpetuate prejudice, such as perceiving individuals with ID as helpless, burdensome, or incapable of making meaningful contributions [19]. Understanding the underlying causes and factors contributing to prejudice against individuals with ID is crucial for developing effective interventions [9].
Research suggests that ignorance, fear, and a lack of experience with individuals affected by ID play significant roles in perpetuating prejudice against members of minority social groups [18,20,21,22]. Additionally, societal factors, such as media portrayal and cultural biases, can influence social and personal attitudes toward individuals with ID. Prejudice toward individuals with ID, in turn, has far-reaching consequences for their well-being and social inclusion. Empirical studies demonstrate that experiences of prejudice contribute to lower self-esteem, increased social exclusion, as well as limited educational and employment opportunities, and compromised mental health among individuals with ID [6,23,24] compared to their typically developed peers. Efforts to combat prejudice toward individuals with ID have focused on several key areas, including education, awareness campaigns, inclusive policies, and fostering positive intergroup contact [25,26]. However, the intergroup contact hypothesis proposed by Allport [14] represents a promising approach to reducing prejudice toward minority groups, such as persons with ID. According to this strategy, increasing personal and positive contact often improves views toward members of negatively stereotyped groups, but casual encounters are more likely to reinforce prejudice than to dispel it, in contrast to personal and meaningful connections [17,25,27]. Allport [14] suggests that to create the best conditions for attitude improvement, interpersonal interaction involving a cooperative and dependent connection supporting status equity and opposing stereotypes is required [28]. Moreover, it seems that the association between intergroup contact and positive attitudes is often influenced by disability type. For instance, [27] discovered a connection between social distance and regular contact: more contact with those with mental illness resulted in fewer feelings of social distance. For sensory deficits, no such association has been discovered. Another study found that the nature of the contact was a significant predictor of students’ attitudes toward people affected by disabilities, suggesting the importance of the quality of contact [25].
Therefore, our goal was to expand existing research on factors systematically associated with negative attitudes toward individuals with ID. Specifically, we examined the combined influence of experienced contact, belief in a just world (BJW), and social dominance orientation (SDO) to explore their effects on prejudice toward people with ID. We anticipated that BJW and SDO would be directly associated with negative attitudes. Individuals who strongly believe that the world systematically compensates for the good and evil one does are also likely to score high in prejudice toward individuals with ID. Additionally, those who envision the social world as a hierarchy of groups rigidly determined by their worthiness are expected to manifest higher prejudice.
Furthermore, we hypothesized that BJW and SDO would mediate the association between experienced contact and prejudice. BJW refers to individuals’ tendency to believe that people generally get what they deserve [29]. Therefore, individuals who socialize in contexts where beliefs in a just world are prominent—the tendency to believe that adverse events befall those who bear responsibility—tend to avoid adopting the social responsibility norm [30]. It’s akin to making a deal with the world, where people believe that if they are good and do the right things, good things will happen to them, and they will achieve their personal and social goals [for a review, see [30]]. These beliefs help people feel safe and secure, giving them hope for the future.
For example, concerning prejudice, individuals who score high in BJW show a positive relationship with negative attitudes toward individuals with mental illness [32]. Additionally, it has been found that BJW is associated with harsh social attitudes and dominance [33]. It appears that a convincing belief in a just world can lead individuals to develop explanatory theories characterized, for instance, as 'conspiratorial attitudes' and 'self-directed' when attempting to make sense of social phenomena. This tendency not only impacts people’s mental well-being, including their subjective comfort and levels of anxiety [34], but also influences their social relationships. For instance, it may affect their interpersonal sensitivity and ability to empathize and understand others’ emotions. This can be particularly evident when considering individuals who belong to minority groups, such as people with disabilities. BJW represents a fundamental component of an individual’s personality that significantly influences their behaviours and experiences. Indeed, people high in BJW may tend discriminatory attitudes and may experience discomfort in interactions with people affected by ID, aligning with the notion that individuals receive outcomes based on what they deserve [for a review, see [34]]. Therefore, individuals who have faith that the world is a just place may be more motivated to attribute blame to marginalized social groups to maintain their worldview beliefs that individuals get what they deserve [36].
Social dominance theory provides a framework for understanding societies’ hierarchical structure and maintaining group-based inequalities [37]. Social Dominance Orientation (SDO; [38]) has been studied to prejudice toward individuals with ID, shedding light on how attitudes toward social hierarchies may influence biases and discriminatory behaviours directed toward this population [15,32,39,40]. Research has shown that individuals with high SDO endorse and support the idea of a fixed social hierarchy, exhibiting more prejudice toward lower-status minorities, such as individuals with ID [41]. They engage in discriminatory behaviours or hold negative stereotypes about ID-affected people’s abilities and worth, increasing social distance. Individuals high in SDO may view persons with ID as less deserving of equal opportunities, resources, and social inclusion, reinforcing discriminatory attitudes and behaviours. Moreover, [42] found that individuals with higher scores in SDO were more predisposed to demonstrate classical and modern prejudice toward individuals perceived to have “impaired development” compared to those with lower scores in this orientation. Similarly, [9] examined the relationship between conservative ideologies (e.g., SDO) and discomfort with disability among preservice educators. They found that SDO and discomfort with disability were stronger predictors of negative attitudes toward people with disabilities and opposition to inclusion. In another study, [10] found that individuals high on SDO and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) were more likely to reject rights for persons with intellectual and physical disabilities than individuals scoring lower on these variables.
[43] found that Belief in a Just World (BJW) and SDO tend to be correlated; people who have a strong belief in a just world endorse and support a rigid hierarchical structure in society, accepting and even promoting inequality among social groups. Moreover, [44] have characterized BJW and SDO as forms of ideologies related to system justification, specifically relevant to social inequality. They argue that these variables play a role in shaping status stereotypes to justify social inequality, suggesting that biases against impoverished individuals are influenced by factors that justify the existing social system.

1.2. The current study

This study aimed to investigate the attitudes of able-bodied students toward individuals affected by ID. We also aimed to identify underlying factors that could either heighten or attenuate negative attitudes toward them. Therefore, the primary objective of the present study was to test a model in which experienced contact serves as the predictor, with BJW and SDO as potential serial mediators and prejudice toward individuals with ID as the outcome. Notably, previous studies have not simultaneously investigated the role of experienced contact and the two individual difference variables (BJW and SDO) and their correlation with classical and modern prejudice against people with ID. This suggests a significant research gap regarding the impact of experienced contact on these relationships.
Therefore, the current study aimed to examine the relationship between experienced contact and the combination of two ideological clusters (BJW and SDO) and the expression of prejudice against people affected by ID. Given that individuals with ID may face beliefs of inadequacy, it is argued that SDO and BJW serve as systems legitimization factors for social inequality. Consequently, a positive correlation is expected between these factors and prejudice towards individuals affected by ID. Specifically, based on the results of previous studies, we predicted that experienced contact should have a buffering role, and should reduce negative attitudes toward people with ID.
Drawing upon [45] theory of ideology and prejudice, we hypothesize that prejudice stems from enduring ideologies rooted in social beliefs, known as worldviews. SDO is identified as a robust predictor of prejudiced attitudes, and we predict that BJW acts as an antecedent to SDO. Individuals with fewer opportunities for positive intergroup contact are expected to exhibit high levels of both BJW and SDO, justifying their prejudicial attitudes toward people with ID [46].
Then, based on the limited findings of previous studies [e.g., [15,32]], we hypothesized that individuals with fewer opportunities for positive intergroup contact might exhibit high levels of BJW (H1) and high levels of SDO (H2) as a strategy to justify their prejudicial attitudes toward people with ID. Moreover, given the results of prior studies [e.g., [9,41]], we expected (H3) that SDO rather than BJW should have a strong impact on prejudice toward people affected by ID. Additionally, we hypothesized (H4) that opportunity for intergroup contact should decrease both BJW and SDO, and these variables may serially mediate the relationship between the experience of contact and prejudice toward people with ID. For example, the interaction between individual differences and diversity (e.g., people affected by ID) is expected to diminish the effects of the mediator variables (BJW and SDO), thereby decreasing the levels of prejudice toward individuals affected by ID. Understanding the mediating role of SDO and the effect of BJW allows for designing targeted intervention programs that promote positive contact experiences and challenge negative beliefs and attitudes toward individuals with ID, which could be helpful.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and procedures

A sample of two hundred and twenty-four participants was recruited for this study. The participants’ ages ranged from 19 and 33 years (M = 23.02, SD = 2.48). Participants were informed that the study aimed to investigate social attitudes toward the person affected by intellectual disabilities. When asked to report their university degree course, participants identified as follows: 158 (70.5%) belonged to a social sciences degree course (i.e., pedagogy, psychology, etc.), and 66 (29.5%) were in the scientific field (i.e., mathematics, engineering, etc.). All participants were recruited from the University of Calabria and the University of Palermo (both located in southern Italy) during break periods and before the beginning of classes. The participants received an informative introduction explaining the aim of the study, after which they were invited to complete an online questionnaire. The researcher emphasised the voluntary nature of participation in the study so the students could stop completing the questionnaire at any time. In addition, the participants were guaranteed complete anonymity and were assured that the data would be used only for research purposes. Participants did not receive any payment or additional university credits. After obtaining the students’ consent, those interested were given a questionnaire, which took them around 20 minutes to complete. The data were collected following the ethical standards of the Italian Psychological Association (AIP) and adhering to the Helsinki Declaration (2013) and its later amendments.

2.2. Measures

When a validated Italian version of the scale was unavailable, a forward and backward translation approach was adopted to ensure the preservation of the original meaning of the items.
3.2.1. Social Dominance Orientation. The Italian version of the Social Dominance Orientation (SDO) 10-item scale [38,47] was used. This scale includes items such as “Some groups of people are simply inferior to other groups”. Participants were required to indicate how much they agreed with each item on a 4-point Likert-type scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Cronbach’s alpha showed acceptable internal consistency of the scale α = 0.63.
3.2.2. Beliefs in a just world. A short version of [48] scale was translated and adapted from English into Italian language and used to measure beliefs in a just world (BJW). The scale includes seven items; an example of an item is “I am confident that justice always triumphs over injustice”. Each participant had to rate their level of agreement on a 7-point Likert-type scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The value of the Cronbach α was good, 0.81.
3.2.3. Modern and Classical Prejudices Scale. Prejudice toward individuals with intellectual disabilities was measured with the Italian Modern and Classical Prejudices Scale (MCPS-IT) [18,20]. The MCPS [original version: [15]] is a 19-item scale for prejudice toward people with ID. It investigates two forms of prejudice: classical or overt/direct (e.g., People with intellectual disabilities often commit crimes) and modern or covert/subtle (e.g., People with intellectual disabilities are getting too demanding in their push for equal rights). Participants responded on a 5-point Likert-type scale, from 1 = ‘Strongly disagree’ to 5 = ‘Strongly agree’. The internal reliability was α = 0.73 for classical prejudice and α = 0.74 for modern prejudice.
3.2.4. Experienced contact as a predictor variable. The predictor variable measured in this study [adapted from [48]] was a composite index, computed as the product of (a) quality of contact with the out-group and (b) quantity of contact with the out-group. Quality of contact was measured with four items preceded by, “When you meet a person affected by intellectual disabilities, in general, do you find the contact …”. The four quality items, assessed on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (never) to 5 (often), were “cooperative”, “superficial”, “voluntary”, and “equal” (Cronbach’s α = 0.66). Quantity of contact was assessed with a single item rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (never) to 5 (very frequently): “How frequently do you have contact with a person affected by intellectual disabilities?” According to [49] to obtain a single index of frequent and positive intergroup contact, we multiplied the scores of quality and quantity of contact. This approach, as utilized by [50], enables us to simultaneously account for both dimensions of contact. Indeed, focusing solely on either the quantity or quality of contact is frequently insufficient in mitigating prejudice; an optimal blend of the two is optimal, as highlighted by Allport [14]. By following [51], before multiplication, quality scores were also re-coded, so that –2 indicated negative contact and +2 positive contact. The quantity scores were re-coded so that 0 corresponded to no contact and 3 to highly frequent contact. Therefore, the composite index ranged from –6, indicating a high amount of negative contact, to +6, indicating a high amount of positive contact. The midpoint 0 may denote either the absence of contact or the presence of a neutrally valanced intergroup contact [49].
3.2.5. Demographic profile. All participants completed a socio-demographic profile collecting information about their gender, age, and degree course.

3. Data analysis

All the statistical analyses were carried out with the support of Jamovi (version 2.3) software [52] and R - lavaan (version 0.6-13) package [53]. Missing data was not an issue, as the online questionnaire required participants to input responses for any missed items. Initially, we inspected the reliability of administered measures using Cronbach’s alpha (α) coefficient. Subsequently, descriptive statistics, univariate normality, and Pearson’s r correlations were computed to explore the variables’ properties and determine their relationships. The scale scores were normally distributed [54]. To assess the hypothesized relationships between variables, we tested a path model with observed variables. Here, experienced contact was the predictor, BJW and SDO were the mediators, and classical and modern prejudice were the outcomes. Direct paths were estimated from the predictor to the outcome variables and correlations between the control, mediators, and outcome variables. Furthermore, a bootstrapping procedure using 5000 resamples was performed to test the significance of indirect effects with a 95% confidence interval (CI) [55]. In estimating all path coefficients, we controlled for gender.
Finally, to assess the adequacy of the proposed research model, we decided to explore a second-trimmed alternative model. The trimmed model included constraining non-significant paths to zero. This approach allows us to determine how well the proposed model aligns with the observed data.

4. Results

4.1. Descriptive and correlations

The results of descriptive statistics among study variables are shown in Table 1.
We found statistically significant correlations in the supposed directions between the predictor variable (experienced contact), SDO, BJW, and attitudes (classical and modern prejudice) (Table 2). Classical prejudice was positively related to SDO, r(224) = 0.53 and BJW, r(224) = 0.37. At the same time, modern prejudice was only associated with BJW, r(224) = 0.40. A positive association between SDO and BJW, r(224) = 0.36 was also found.

4.2. Mediation analysis

The bootstrapped regression-based path analysis displayed direct and indirect effects. Results of the estimated direct effects for the saturated model are shown in Figure 1.
All the associations in the model were statistically significant, except for the following paths (a) from experienced contact to classical prejudice; (b) from experienced contact to modern prejudice; and (c) from SDO to modern prejudice. Experienced contact negatively and significantly predicted BJW and SDO. Both BJW and SDO positively and significantly predicted classical prejudice. BJW was a significant positive predictor of classical and modern prejudice, respectively. The total indirect effect (IE) of experienced contact on classical prejudice was statistically significant, (Β = -0.04, 95% CI [-0.07, -0.02], β = -0.27, p < 0.001). Specifically, we found that BJW and SDO were serial mediators in the relationship between experienced contact and classical prejudice, (Β = -0.04, 95% CI [-0.01, -0.01], β = -0.02, p < 0.01). The current results suggest that experienced contact was indirectly associated with classical prejudice through BJW (Β = -0.01, 95% CI [-0.00, -0.01], β = -0.04, p < 0.05) and SDO, (Β = -0.02, 95% CI [-0.03, -0.01], β = -0.12, p < 0.01), respectively. No other significant results emerged from the current analyses. Regarding the control variables, gender negatively affected only classical prejudice, (Β = -0.13, 95% CI [-0.23, -0.04], β = -0.14, p < 0.05). Classical prejudice accounts for 34% of the variance.
Given the cross-sectional design of the study, we ran a second alternative model in which we trimmed the nonsignificant paths from the first model. Specifically, we tested an additional model by fixing non-significant paths to zero (e.g., dashed lines in Figure 1). The results indicated that the total IE of experienced contact with classical prejudice once again was statistically significant, (Β = -0.03, 95% CI [-0.05, -0.02], β = -0.20, p < 0.001). Experienced contact was a negative and significant predictor of BJW and SDO, respectively. In turn, BJW was associated with classical and modern prejudice as well as with SDO (ps < 0.001). SDO was a significant predictor of classical prejudice (ps < 0.001). The results of the alternative model (Figure 2) suggest that experienced contact was indirectly associated with classical prejudice, through the serial mediation of BJW and SDO as well as by considering the specific effects of BJW and SDO.

5. Discussion

This study sought to investigate the serial mediating effects of BJW and SDO in the relationship between experienced contact and classical and modern prejudice toward individuals affected by ID. Building upon prior research on this phenomenon and drawing from the literature on prejudice and individual differences, the results of the current study provide substantial support for the assumption that experienced contact is an important factor in reducing prejudice, mainly classical prejudice, against people affected by ID. Pearson’s correlations reveal that BJW is correlated with modern prejudice, while SDO is associated with classical prejudice against people with ID.
The key findings of this study revealed that experienced contact was negatively associated with both BJW (H1 supported) and SDO (H2 supported), and these mediating variables significantly influenced classical prejudice (but not modern prejudice) toward individuals with ID. These results indicate that higher-quality intergroup interactions are associated with lower levels of these cluster ideological variables, suggesting that positive intergroup contact can play a pivotal role in reducing prejudice and mitigating the effects of BJW and SDO in discriminating against people with ID. However, the direct association between the experienced contact and prejudice (classical and modern) was not statistically significant.
One of the most notable findings of this study was the relationship between SDO and prejudice against people with ID (H3 supported). In line with previous studies SDO, which reflects a desire for hierarchical intergroup relations and social inequality, was found to have a stronger effect on classical prejudice than modern prejudice toward people affected by ID [9,11,12]. In other words, this result suggests that university students who express discomfort with disability are more likely to perceive society hierarchically and favour social inequality. Therefore, classical prejudice, characterised by overt and explicit forms of bias, may be more closely linked to SDO than modern prejudice, which tends to manifest subtly and less overtly. This finding underscores the complexity of prejudice and the need for nuanced approaches to understanding its underlying mechanisms. People with a high desire for power and control might believe that it is acceptable to mistreat some people (i.e., individuals affected by ID) because it helps them feel important and keeps things the way they want them to be. Therefore, they might use prejudiced ideas to make themselves feel better about treating others unequally. Importantly, the relationship between these variables remained in the revised model; this association is not only robust but likely reflective of how individuals with ID are exposed to negative attitudes. These findings are consistent with previous research linking SDO to prejudice against socially subordinate groups [46] such as people affected by ID [9,12].
Along with SDO, the current results suggest that individuals who scored higher in BJW, characterised by the belief in a fundamentally just and fair world where people generally receive what they deserve, were more likely to express prejudice (classical and modern). This finding highlights the significant role of BJW in explaining the relationship between certain individual factors (e.g., individual characteristics, social influences) and the expression of prejudice. In the context of people affected by ID, the BJW can lead to the assumption that people with ID must have done something to deserve their condition or circumstances. These beliefs can be particularly harmful as they may justify discrimination against people with ID. Unlike overt forms, modern prejudice is often more subtle and can manifest through microaggressions, implicit biases, or veiled negative attitudes toward specific groups, such as individuals affected by ID.
Moreover, our bootstrapped regression-based path analyses suggest that the experienced contact on classical prejudice (but not on modern prejudice) toward people with ID is fully mediated through the serial effects of BJW and SDO (H4 partially supported). Additionally, experienced contact was indirectly related to classical prejudice, through the mediating role of BJW and SDO, respectively. Overall, it appears that reduced experienced contact with individuals affected by ID increases the people the tendency to view the world in terms of social hierarchies, and exhibiting preferences for one’s groups to dominate over others is linked to prejudice against persons with ID. This connection is demonstrated by a diminished motivation to actively strive against prejudicial responses toward those groups and a distinct willingness to resist or limit their rights. The results of this study not only support but also extend previous research [17,25,27], by considering the role of two individual differences such as BJW and SDO, never examined in this field and using as outcome variable the modern prejudice. Including these variables allows for a more comprehensive exploration of the intricate dynamics that underlie intergroup relations and prejudice against people affected by ID.
The current results are partially consistent with the hypotheses of the study since the experienced contact influences BJW, which impacts SDO, and in turn, affects classical prejudice but not modern prejudice. Further, our results indicated a full serial indirect effect of BJW and SDO in the relationship between experienced contact and classical prejudice. Taken together, the current study provides valuable insights into the underlying mechanisms driving prejudice toward individuals with ID and underscores the importance of understanding the complex interplay of beliefs, social hierarchies, and attitudes in intergroup contexts. Individuals with higher levels of BJW and SDO tend to support legitimising myths that justify group-based inequality. Moreover, in line with [45] theory of ideology and prejudice, people who endorse these system-justifying ideologies increase the likelihood of adopting social policy attitudes that reinforce the existing status differences within society [41] for whom they already hold prejudice for members of minority groups (e.g., students with ID) [10]. Notably, the relationship between these variables persisted in the alternative revised model, suggesting how individuals with ID are affected by negative attitudes.
Finally, in this study, the differentiation between modern and classical prejudice offers new insights into research exploring the risk of discrimination toward people affected by ID. Modern prejudice, characterized as a more ‘sophisticated’ form, demands increased cognitive effort and justification. The current results are consistent with the initial study by [15]. Therefore, this study seeks to distinguish between classical and modern forms of prejudice, shedding light on the complexity of discriminatory attitudes.

5.1. Limitations and further studies

The current study has several limitations. First, all the variables were measured using self-report questionnaires. This method is susceptible to social desirability bias, where participants may respond in a way, they believe is socially acceptable, even if it is not their valid response. Future studies should use various methods to measure prejudice, such as implicit measures, to reduce the impact of social desirability bias. Second, the study only examined the serial mediating role of BJW and SDO in the relationship between intergroup contact and prejudice. It is possible that other variables, such as empathy or perceived similarity, may also play a role in this relationship. Future studies should investigate these other variables to gain a more complete understanding of the mediation process. Third, the study was conducted with a sample of university students in southern Italy. This limits the ability to generalise the findings to other populations. Future studies should replicate the findings with a more diverse sample of participants. However, the current sample is relevant to the objective of the study since most of them will work with people who could be affected by ID and other studies interviewed university students in similar research [18]. Finally, the study was cross-sectional, meaning that the data was collected at one point in time. This makes it difficult to conclude the causal relationships between the variables. Future studies should use a longitudinal design to track participants and better understand the causal mechanisms involved. Following this line of discussion, the limitation concerning the issue of causality should also be mentioned. In structural models, causal relationships between factors are often assumed a priori and are not falsified by the data even if the true causal relationship is the reverse of that being suggested [42]. However, it should be noted that in the current study, we integrated into a single model social factors (experienced contact) and personality factors (SDO and BJW) as suggested by scholars [56,57].

6. Conclusion

Overall, the present study suggests that individuals with higher levels of experienced contact show less antisocial behavior and should be more open to interacting with people affected by ID. This study highlights the relevance of individual differences in understanding prejudice against people with ID. The mediating role of BJW and SDO, in the relationship between experienced contact and prejudice, should help those responsible for social and inclusive policies in designing targeted intervention programs to promote the social inclusion of individuals with ID disorders.

Author Contributions

Rocco Servidio: Conceptualization, Project administration, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. Ivan Giuseppe Cammarata: Methodology. Costanza Scaffidi Abate: Writing – review & editing, Supervision. Stefano Boca: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the ethical standards of the Italian Psychological Association (AIP).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the students who participated in our data collection.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Schalock, R.L.; Luckasson, R.; Tassé, M.J.; Verdugo, M.A. A Holistic Theoretical Approach to Intellectual Disability: Going Beyond the Four Current Perspectives. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 2018, 56, 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. , 5th ed.APA, *!!! REPLACE !!!*, Ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, D.C, 2013; ISBN 978-0-89042-554-1.
  3. Hamdani, Y.; Ary, A.; Lunsky, Y. Critical Analysis of a Population Mental Health Strategy: Effects on Stigma for People With Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. Journal of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities 2017, 10, 144–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Mitter, N.; Ali, A.; Scior, K. Stigma Experienced by Family Members of People with Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities: Multidimensional Construct. BJPsych open 2018, 4, 332–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Tenorio, M.; Donoso, J.; Ali, A.; Hassiotis, A. Stigma Toward Persons with Intellectual Disability in South America: A Narrative Review. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities 2020, 17, 346–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Trani, J.-F.; Moodley, J.; Anand, P.; Graham, L.; Thu Maw, M.T. Stigma of Persons with Disabilities in South Africa: Uncovering Pathways from Discrimination to Depression and Low Self-Esteem. Social Science & Medicine 2020, 265, 113449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Walker, J.; Scior, K. Tackling Stigma Associated with Intellectual Disability among the General Public: A Study of Two Indirect Contact Interventions. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2013, 34, 2200–2210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. WHO World Mental Health Report. Transforming Mental Health for All 2022.
  9. Brandes, J.A.; Crowson, H.M. Predicting Dispositions toward Inclusion of Students with Disabilities: The Role of Conservative Ideology and Discomfort with Disability. Soc Psychol Educ 2009, 12, 271–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Crowson, H.M.; Brandes, J.A. Predicting Pre-Service Teachers’ Opposition to Inclusion of Students with Disabilities: A Path Analytic Study. Soc Psychol Educ 2014, 17, 161–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kerr, J.R.; Wilson, M.S. Right-Wing Authoritarianism and Social Dominance Orientation Predict Rejection of Science and Scientists. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 2021, 24, 550–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Crowson, H.M.; Brandes, J.A.; Hurst, R.J. Who Opposes Rights for Persons with Physical and Intellectual Disabilities? J Applied Social Pyschol 2013, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Abdul-Chani, M.M.; Moreno, C.P.; Reeder, J.A.; Zuckerman, K.E.; Lindly, O.J. Perceived Community Disability Stigma in Multicultural, Low-Income Populations: Measure Development and Validation. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2021, 115, 103997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Allport, G.W. The Nature of Prejudice; Unabridged, 25th anniversary ed.; Addison-Wesley Pub. Co: Reading, Mass, 1954; ISBN 978-0-201-00178-5. [Google Scholar]
  15. Akrami, N.; Ekehammar, B.; Claesson, M.; Sonnander, K. Classical and Modern Prejudice: Attitudes toward People with Intellectual Disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2006, 27, 605–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. McConahay, J.B.; Hardee, B.B.; Batts, V. Has Racism Declined in America?: It Depends on Who Is Asking and What Is Asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution 1981, 25, 563–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rocco Servidio; Stefano Boca Pregiudizio classico e moderno nei confronti dei richiedenti asilo: un contributo alla validazione italiana della Prejudice Against Asylum Seekers Scale (PAAS). Psicologia sociale 2021, 99–121. [CrossRef]
  18. Servidio, R.; Marcone, R. Classical and Modern Prejudice towards Individuals with Intellectual Disabilities: A Study on a Sample of Italian University Students. The Social Science Journal 2020, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Beelmann, A.; Heinemann, K.S. Preventing Prejudice and Improving Intergroup Attitudes: A Meta-Analysis of Child and Adolescent Training Programs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 2014, 35, 10–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Marcone, R.; Caputo, A.; Esposito, S.; Senese, V.P. Prejudices towards People with Intellectual Disabilities: Reliability and Validity of the Italian Modern and Classical Prejudices Scale: Prejudices towards People with Intellectual Disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 2019, 63, 911–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Scior, K.; Hamid, A.; Hastings, R.; Werner, S.; Belton, C.; Laniyan, A.; Patel, M.; Kett, M. Intellectual Disability Stigma and Initiatives to Challenge It and Promote Inclusion around the Globe. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities 2020, 17, 165–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ünal, E.; Uyaroğlu, A.K. The Relationship between Beliefs toward Mental Illnesses, Empathic Tendency and Social Distancing in University Students. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 2022, 41, 348–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Gulya, N.; Fehérvári, A. The Impact of Literary Works Containing Characters with Disabilities on Students’ Perception and Attitudes towards People with Disabilities. International Journal of Educational Research 2023, 117, 102132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Sood, S.; Kostizak, K.; Stevens, S.; Cronin, C.; Ramaiya, A.; Paddidam, P. Measurement and Conceptualisation of Attitudes and Social Norms Related to Discrimination against Children with Disabilities: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education 2022, 69, 1489–1504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Alnahdi, G.H.; Elhadi, A.; Schwab, S. The Positive Impact of Knowledge and Quality of Contact on University Students’ Attitudes towards People with Intellectual Disability in the Arab World. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2020, 106, 103765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Lawson, J.E.; Cruz, R.A.; Knollman, G.A. Increasing Positive Attitudes toward Individuals with Disabilities through Community Service Learning. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2017, 69, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Huskin, P.R.; Reiser-Robbins, C.; Kwon, S. Attitudes of Undergraduate Students Toward Persons With Disabilities: Exploring Effects of Contact Experience on Social Distance Across Ten Disability Types. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin 2018, 62, 53–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Orm, S.; Blikstad-Blumenthal, C.; Fjermestad, K. Attitudes toward People with Intellectual Disabilities in Norway. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities 2023, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lerner, M.J. The Belief in a Just World; Springer US: Boston, MA, 1980; ISBN 978-1-4899-0450-8. [Google Scholar]
  30. Dan, B. The Metaphysical Model of Disability: Is This a Just World? Develop Med Child Neuro 2021, 63, 240–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Nudelman, G.; Otto, K. Personal Belief in a Just World and Conscientiousness: A Meta-analysis, Facet-level Examination, and Mediation Model. Br. J. Psychol. 2021, 112, 92–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Bizer, G.Y.; Hart, J.; Jekogian, A.M. Belief in a Just World and Social Dominance Orientation: Evidence for a Mediational Pathway Predicting Negative Attitudes and Discrimination against Individuals with Mental Illness. Personality and Individual Differences 2012, 52, 428–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hafer, C.L.; Sutton, R. Belief in a Just World. In Handbook of Social Justice Theory and Research; Sabbagh, C., Schmitt, M., Eds.; Springer New York: New York, NY, 2016; ISBN 978-1-4939-3215-3. [Google Scholar]
  34. Bertrams, A. Less Illusion of a Just World in People with Formally Diagnosed Autism and Higher Autistic Traits. J Autism Dev Disord 2021, 51, 3733–3743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kong, F.; Lan, N.; Zhang, H.; Sun, X.; Zhang, Y. How Does Social Anxiety Affect Mobile Phone Dependence in Adolescents? The Mediating Role of Self-Concept Clarity and Self-Esteem. Curr Psychol 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Litam, S.D.A.; Balkin, R.; Hendricks, L. Assessing Worry, Racism, and Belief in a Just World. Jour of Counseling & Develop 2022, 100, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Sidanius, J.; Pratto, F. Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, 1999; ISBN 978-1-139-17504-3. [Google Scholar]
  38. Pratto, F.; Sidanius, J.; Stallworth, L.M.; Malle, B.F. Social Dominance Orientation: A Personality Variable Predicting Social and Political Attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1994, 67, 741–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Ekehammar, B.; Akrami, N.; Gylje, M.; Zakrisson, I. What Matters Most to Prejudice: Big Five Personality, Social Dominance Orientation, or Right-Wing Authoritarianism? Eur J Pers 2004, 18, 463–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Phelan, J.E.; Basow, S.A. College Students’ Attitudes Toward Mental Illness: An Examination of the Stigma Process. J Appl Social Pyschol 2007, 37, 2877–2902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Sibley, C.G.; Duckitt, J. The Ideological Legitimation of the Status Quo: Longitudinal Tests of a Social Dominance Model. Political Psychology 2010, 31, 109–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bäckström, M.; Björklund, F. Structural Modeling of Generalized Prejudice: The Role of Social Dominance, Authoritarianism, and Empathy. Journal of Individual Differences 2007, 28, 10–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. De Keersmaecker, J.; Roets, A. All Victims Are Equally Innocent, but Some Are More Innocent than Others: The Role of Group Membership on Victim Blaming. Curr Psychol 2020, 39, 254–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Oldmeadow, J.; Fiske, S.T. System-Justifying Ideologies Moderate Status = Competence Stereotypes: Roles for Belief in a Just World and Social Dominance Orientation. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 37, 1135–1148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Duckitt, J. A Dual-Process Cognitive-Motivational Theory of Ideology and Prejudice. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Elsevier, 2001; Vol. 33, pp. 41–113 ISBN 978-0-12-015233-9.
  46. Duckitt, J. Differential Effects of Right Wing Authoritarianism and Social Dominance Orientation on Outgroup Attitudes and Their Mediation by Threat From and Competitiveness to Outgroups. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2006, 32, 684–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Di Stefano, G.; Roccato, M. Una Banca Di Item per Misurare l’orientamento Alia Dominanza Sociale in Italia. [An Item Bank for Measuring Social Dominance Orientation in Italy.]. Testing Psicometria Metodologia 2005, 12, 5–20. [Google Scholar]
  48. Lipkus, I. The Construction and Preliminary Validation of a Global Belief in a Just World Scale and the Exploratory Analysis of the Multidimensional Belief in a Just World Scale. Personality and Individual Differences 1991, 12, 1171–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Voci, A.; Hewstone, M. Intergroup Contact and Prejudice Toward Immigrants in Italy: The Mediational Role of Anxiety and the Moderational Role of Group Salience. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 2003, 6, 37–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Brown, R.; Maras, P.; Masser, B.; Vivian, J.; Hewstone, M. Life on the Ocean Wave: Testing Some Intergroup Hypotheses in a Naturalistic Setting. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 2001, 4, 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Rozich, B.C.; Kenworthy, J.B.; Voci, A.; Hewstone, M. What’s Past Is Prologue: Intergroup Emotions and Trust as Mediating the Links between Prior Intergroup Contact and Future Behavioral Tendencies. TPM - Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology. [CrossRef]
  52. Jamovi The Jamovi Project 2022. Retrieved from https://www.jamovi.org.
  53. Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. Journal of Statistical Software 2012, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling; Methodology in the social sciences; Fourth edition.; The Guilford Press: New York, 2016; ISBN 978-1-4625-2335-1. [Google Scholar]
  55. Preacher, K.J.; Hayes, A.F. Asymptotic and Resampling Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indirect Effects in Multiple Mediator Models. Behavior Research Methods 2008, 40, 879–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Altemeyer, B. Right-Wing Authoritarianism; University of Manitoba Press: Winnipeg, 1981; ISBN 978-0-88755-124-6. [Google Scholar]
  57. Whitley, B.E.; Kite, M.E. Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination; Thrid Edition.; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: New York ; London, 2016; ISBN 978-1-138-94752-8.
Figure 1. Standardised path coefficients for the whole model. Dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Note. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Standardised path coefficients for the whole model. Dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. Note. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Preprints 97980 g001
Figure 2. Standardised path coefficients. Note. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Standardised path coefficients. Note. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Preprints 97980 g002
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, skewness, and kurtosis for the main study variables.
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, skewness, and kurtosis for the main study variables.
M SD Skewness Kurtosis
Experienced contact 2.40 2.57 0.12 -1.19
Social Dominance Orientation 1.53 0.32 0.93 1.48
Beliefs in a Just World 2.64 0.87 0.20 -0.34
Classical prejudice 1.58 0.43 0.95 0.72
Modern prejudice 2.10 0.43 -0.34 -0.48
Age 23.02 2.48 1.20 2.34
Note. Experienced contact ranges from –6 to +6.
Table 2. Pearson’s bivariate correlations.
Table 2. Pearson’s bivariate correlations.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Experienced contact 1.00
2. Social Dominance Orientation -0.35*** 1.00
3. Beliefs in a Just World -0.18** 0.36*** 1.00
4. Classical prejudice -0.28*** 0.53*** 0.37*** 1.00
5. Modern prejudice 0.00 0.12 0.40*** 0.42*** 1.00
6. Age -0.02 0.07 -0.06 0.07 0.02 1.00
7. Gender 0.05 -0.07 0.03 -0.17* -0.01 -0.06 1.00
Note. Gender is a point bi-serial correlation (1 = male, 2 = female). Experienced contact ranges from –6 to +6. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.

Downloads

115

Views

57

Comments

0

Subscription

Notify me about updates to this article or when a peer-reviewed version is published.

Email

Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2025 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated