1. Background
During the transition between prenatal and neonatal life, the autonomic nervous system (ANS) plays a crucial role for cardiovascular and respiratory functioning [
1]. A primary goal of neonatal care for preterm infants is to ensure the stability of ANS [
1]. This is achieved through neuroprotective individualized neonatal care centered on infants’ neurodevelopmental needs. NIDCAP (Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program) focused on a detailed reading of each infant’s behavioral cues helping its’ self-regulation. According NIDCAP, environment and care are adapted to enhance each infant’s strengths and self-regulation collaboratively with parents that play the primary role for infants' developmentn. [
2].
In NIDCAP approach, Kangaroo mother care is one of the multimodal sensory experiences provided in NICU through the early skin-to-skin contact between mothers and preterm infants [
2]. Kangaroo mother care has shown benefits for preterm infants’ self-regulation as well as for maintaining the intimate connection between mother and infant [
3]. This is achieved through the ANS balance having impacts on infants’ physiological stability [
2].
Comparatively to the original vertical positioning, “kangaroo supported flexion diagonal positioning” protocol [
4] replaces the previous vertical position of the preterm infant by a diagonal positioning improving mother-infant gaze contact. Furthermore, it ensures preterm infants’ physiological stability improving the contingent interaction between mother and infant [
5] and so it has been underlined as a promissory method to the understanding of the innate basis of intersubjectivity in the context of very preterm birth [
5]. Also, using this protocol, a significant decrease in maternal stress and a lower risk of postpartum depression were found [
6].
When combined with kangaroo care, maternal singing shows a positive and significant impact in autonomic stability of preterm infants [
7]. Parental “Early Vocal Contact” [
8] using parental singing and parental speech has also been highlighted as a promising approach in NICU to promote preterm infants’ autonomic balance [
8,
9]. Live maternal singing enhances preterm infants’ vagal activity in the short term, thus improving neonatal autonomic stability [
10].
Benefits of live music therapy using humming in a contingent way – synchronizing musical features with infants’ signals - were observed on early preterm infants’ self-regulation in NICU [
11,
12,
13,
14,
15,
16,
17].
Music therapy and sound healing based in vibroacoustic effects of sounds are increasingly utilizing vocal toning including humming in clinical practice to promote a deep relaxation and a change in the state of consciousness [
18]. Vocal toning is a type of vocalizing that utilizes the natural voice to express sounds including humming on the full exhalation of the breath and open vowel sounds [
18] as well as “OM”
chanting [
19]. A study with adults observed that vocal toning can improve attention, awareness, and consciousness [
18]. Also “meditative”, “calm”, and “relaxed” states, which are associated with the parasympathetic nervous system and with a decrease of HR, were the best descriptors of vocal toning. On the contrary, singing evoked emotions that were stronger than the emotions evoked by vocal toning; among those the three most common descriptors included “nostalgia”, “tenderness”, and “joyful activation” [
18].
After preterm birth, maternal emotional state can make it difficult to communicate verbally with the infant and therefore, singing (with words) can evoke very intense emotions while humming can facilitate the connection with the preterm infant in a safer way.
According NIDCAP approach [
2], preterm infants’ vocal responsiveness can be considered as an indicator of behavioral self-regulation if a balance is present in the relationship among the autonomic, the motor, and the states of consciousness subsystems. Otherwise, it may be interpreted as a behavior that may require too much effort and energy expenditure for preterm infants.
Vocal toning, including humming improves cardiovascular function, respiratory flow, and diaphragmatic breathing [
19,
20,
21]. These improvements have been identified as good psychophysiological indicators of decreased anxiety and increased relaxation [
22]. The effectiveness of humming on preterm infants’ physiological parameters is associated with a decrease in HR to adequate values and an increase in O2 saturation [
11,
12,
13].
According to literature [
23], more benefits using live music therapy (singing or humming) when compared with recorded music were found relative to the physiological stabilization of preterm infants (at 30 minutes after music therapy, a lower level of heart rate and a deeper sleep). Also, live lullabies improve the sleep state of preterm infants more than recorded lullabies [
24]. However, both conditions were associated with a decrease in the infants’ heart rate (HR) and no changes were observed in oxygen saturation level [
24]. This way, there were no significant differences in preterm infants’ physiological parameters in response to recorded lullabies compared to mothers’ live lullabies [
25]. Lullabies together with multimodal stimulation induce benefits in weight gain/day and diminishes the number of days of preterm infants’ hospitalization [
26].
Potentially adverse effects of recorded maternal voice through infant directed speech (ID speech) and infant directed singing (ID singing) were found, expressed in decreased O2 saturation [
27]. However, another study did not observe significant differences in HR or O2 saturation when comparing recorded maternal voice against the control condition [
28]. A decrease of HR was found when the recorded maternal voice (speaking or reading) was delivered [
29,
30], while an increase of HR was found when live maternal voice (speaking or singing) was directed to preterm infants [
31].
Inconsistent results were observed about the effects of maternal voice for physiological outcomes such as HR, heart rate variability (HRV), O2 saturation [
7,
11,
26,
27,
30,
32,
33]. These inconsistencies may be due to methodological issues, namely the different conditions in which maternal voice was delivered (recorded, live, speaking, reading, singing, humming, etc.). Most studies used recorded maternal voice during a speech condition (speaking or reading) and few studies used live maternal voice either spoken or sung [
8]. Few studies included singing comparatively with speech and very few studies used humming when compared with singing. Nevertheless, there is a gap about studies of music therapy in NICU regarding the musical characteristics of singing or of humming.
Maternal ID singing is a specific way for mothers to connect infants with a positive affect [
34,
35]. Despite dyads’ individual variations, maternal ID singing presents universal characteristics that discriminate it from singing directed to adults. Most studies on ID singing were conducted with term dyads, and especially with infants older than three months [
36]. Few studies regarding ID singing to preterm infants were reported in literature.
Maternal ID singing is characterized by a slower tempo with a regular beat, a higher and consistent pitch level, and a more expressive vocal tone, when compared with adult-directed singing [
36,
37,
38]. Many of these acoustic features are useful to capture the infants’ attention [
34,
35] and to improve their self-regulation [
39]. Remarkably, parents or mothers naturally change the way they sing based on infants’ needs and their behavioral states [
40].
Lengthening of the final note in a musical phrase is a key feature of parental ID singing. This is a crucial temporal marker that helps infants perceiving units such as vocal phrases and phrase groupings [
41,
42,
43]. It is likely that the lengthening of the final note in humming phrases is longer than in singing phrases, giving infants cues that help them anticipate the end of humming phrases. This way, in a previous study carried on with the present sample, preterm infants’ overlapping vocalizations occurred predominantly during the lengthening of the final note in humming phrases [
44]. This suggests that preterm infants can predict the end of humming phrases. Maternal breathing that follows the lengthening of the final note may be considered as a physiological marker of the offset pauses between humming vocalizations. We think that this marker may induce the infants’ vocal response at the end of the final note.
About the melodic contours of ID singing, Falk [
45] observed six types of melodic contours: linear, rising, falling, bell-shaped, U-shaped, and sinusoidal contours. A microanalytical study with the sample of the present one found a predominance of sinusoidal contours (29.92%) followed by bell-shaped (23.11%), rising (20.72%), falling (15.55%), U-shaped (6.70%), and linear contours (3.97%) in maternal humming directed to preterm infants [
46]. In addition, a predominance of infants’ overlapping vocalizations during sinusoidal and bell-shaped contours was observed [
46]. These melodic contours are predominantly observed in world lullabies characterized by smoothly oscillating sinusoidal contours as well as by descending, bell-shaped, and flat contours with a lengthening of the final note where the pitch range tends to be smaller [
47]. Infant-directed lullabies aim to calm, decreasing the arousal level, ensuring an intimate and warm environment, and inducing a sleep state [
40].
It would be important to understand if maternal humming during kangaroo care is able to improve physiological synchrony in preterm dyads. In short, does the regularity of humming in a lullaby way helps to stabilize mothers’ cardiorespiratory function and to maintain them in a relaxation state during kangaroo care? More than that, does maternal relaxation through humming improve the infants’ self-regulation?
1.1. Purpose of the present study
As a vibroacoustic experience that improves vagal activity [
19], maternal humming during skin-to-skin contact may be even more effective than speech enhancing parasympathetic responses associated with a maternal relaxation state. Compared with speech, humming as associated with improvements in maternal respiratory flow, and in cardiovascular function, with a lower vocal intensity and a smaller tonal variation, may improve preterm infants’ self-regulation.
Our study focused on humming effects upon physiological parameters of preterm dyads during kangaroo care. Among several aspects, we think that humming musical features such as melodic contours and the lengthening of the final note contribute to its effectiveness. There is a gap in research about musical characteristics of humming produced during music therapy sessions. Therefore, it is essential to know how the musical features of humming can help to improve preterm infants’ physiological stability. This will be useful for preterm mothers’ guidance while improving humming addressed to their infants during kangaroo care.
Infants’ physiological stability is ensured when an adequate level of oxygen (O2) saturation is achieved with less effort in heart rate (HR) and breathing [
1]. Thus, a new physiological measure such as proportion of O2 saturation relative to HR (Prop. O2 saturation/HR) should be considered to evaluate the effects of stimulation on preterm infants’ cardiorespiratory stability.
The aims of the present study are to evaluate during kangaroo care: 1) the effect of maternal humming vs. speech on preterm infants’ physiological parameters (infants’ HR and infants’ Prop. O2 saturation/HR); 2) the effect of maternal humming vs. speech on mothers’ physiological parameters (HR and O2 Saturation), and 3) the impact of humming melodic contours (linear, rising, falling, bell-shaped, U-shaped, and sinusoidal) and of the lengthening of the final note on preterm infants’ physiological parameters.
4. Discussion
So far, according to our knowledge, this is probably a pioneering study as it aimed at understanding the effects of melodic contours and of the lengthening of the final note in maternal ID humming on preterm infants’ autonomic system during kangaroo care. Regarding physiological measures used to assess preterm infants’ ANS (HR mean, HR range, HRV and O2 saturation), as highlighted in the state of the art [
7,
11,
17,
26,
28,
30,
32,
33], our study added Prop. Infant O2/HR saturation as a new and promising physiological measure to this research field. This was based on the idea that autonomic stability can be improved if it is achieved while preserving infants’ cardiac resources; this is relevant to ensure preterm infants’ physiological stability [
1].
Regarding the first aim of this study our results show a lower level (group mean) of preterm infants’ HR during maternal humming than during maternal speech. Also, a higher level (group mean) of infants’ Prop. O2 saturation/HR was found during humming when compared with speech. As a consequence, the Prop. O2 saturation/HR shows that the optimal value of O2 saturation can be achieved with a lesser cardiac effort. This suggests that humming is a favorable condition for decreasing preterm infants’ HR while, at the same time, O2 saturation reaches an optimal level of O2 saturation.
About the second aim, a higher maternal Prop. O2 saturation/HR in humming than in speech was found. This suggests that mothers’ humming during kangaroo care may improve maternal cardiorespiratory function, as highlighted in studies about the physiological benefits of vocal toning in adults [
18,
19,
20,
21].
Concerning the third aim of this study, it was observed that the mean duration of humming sinusoidal contours together with the lengthening of the final notes helps to decrease preterm infants’ HR. Furthermore, the stability of O2 saturation is achieved with a lesser cardiac effort. Once that O2 saturation is crucial for human infants’ viability, and that HR is indispensable to achieve its’ regulation [
1], it is important to relate both variables. Because preterm infants are particularly vulnerable to cardiac efforts, O2 saturation should not be regarded in the absence of HR.
Despite mothers were not asked to hum lullabies or in a lullaby style, our results show that maternal humming presents structural and melodic features of that style. Sinusoidal contours and the lengthening of the final notes are usually observed in lullaby style [
47]. As highlighted by music therapy in NICU [
11,
12,
13,
14,
15] our results suggest that maternal humming in a lullaby style can improve preterm infants' physiological stability.
Concerning infants’ vocal responsiveness, in a previous study made with the same sample it was observed that more than a half of the infants’ overlapping vocalizations took place during the final note [
51]. Probably the lengthening of the final note can be perceived by the infants as a temporal marker helping to respond during the end of the humming phrase.
As mentioned in literature, humming has a strong effect on the vagal system [
19] inducing a relaxation state, and wellbeing releasing endorphins, enhancing sleep, lowering heart rate, and blood pressure, decreasing the cortisol and increasing oxytocin [
52]. Therefore, humming in a lullaby style attuned to preterm infants’ behavior is commonly used on music therapy in NICU, [
11,
12,
13,
14,
15].
Our study highlights the important role of the musical characteristics of humming that has been overlooked in studies of music therapy and other neonatal stimulation approaches directed at preterm infants in NICU. When mothers hum to their preterm infants’ during kangaroo care, guidance about humming features should be offered to optimize infants' self-regulation. Maternal humming is likely to be an easy way to improve mother-infant relationship during kangaroo care. If maternal humming in kangaroo care is offered in a lullaby style and keeping the voice calm, slow, simple, predictable, and repetitive, it can optimize the cardiorespiratory functioning of both mother and preterm infant [
12].
When the infant is agitated and presenting tachycardia, maternal humming should be repetitive, having continuous notes with short intervals, predominance of sinusoidal or descending melodic contours and with repetition of the final notes or its’ lengthening. On the other hand, it is important that these guidelines do not induce an excessively relaxed state that could induce severe bradycardia. This reinforces the use of contingent singing including humming in music therapy applied to NICU contexts [
12,
13,
14,
15].
The joint effect of O2 saturation with HR assessed by the Proportion of O2 saturation/HR is a promising physiological variable that should be considered in future studies to clarify the benefits of maternal humming in preterm infants’ cardiorespiratory stabilization.
4.1. Limitations
Our study has several limitations. One of these is that we didn’t analyze the impact of the NICU’s noise, which could have affected the physiological parameters of the dyads. Although infants’ behavioral states and clinical status were controlled as baseline conditions, these aspects were not monitored during the protocol sequence. It is possible that these factors were affected by the protocol conditions and, this way, changing the infants’ physiological parameters. In addition, mothers varied their tactile behavior during the interaction, touching and caressing the infant while they spoke or sang and during silent pauses. The possible effects of maternal touch and rocking directed at the infant during humming were not analyzed. Finally, we didn’t analyze the maternal emotional state during the observation, which is another factor that could have affected mothers’ vocal expressiveness as well as their physiological parameters.
Following studies using multimodal stimulation in music therapy programs with preterm infants [
26,
53] it is important to deepen the joint effect of humming with kangaroo care on infants’ self-regulation. Among multimodal stimulations there are rocking, and touch coordinated with rhythmic features of maternal humming/singing during kangaroo care, i.e., rocking associated with a vestibular experience that can have an impact on the preterm infants’ ANS.
Table 1.
Descriptive statistic of sociodemographic and clinical data of preterm dyads (N = 36).
Table 1.
Descriptive statistic of sociodemographic and clinical data of preterm dyads (N = 36).
Variables |
n |
% |
M |
SD |
min. – max. |
maternal age |
|
|
34.20 |
5.63 |
21 - 48 |
maternal education |
|
|
15.33 |
3.69 |
6 - 24 |
Portuguese nationality |
26 |
72.22 |
|
|
|
African and Brazilian nationality |
10 |
27.78 |
|
|
|
male infants |
20 |
55.55 |
|
|
|
female infants |
16 |
44.44 |
|
|
|
infants’ gestational age at birth* |
|
|
212.78 |
17.11 |
178 - 241 |
infants’ chronological age at observation* |
|
|
26.5 |
19.99 |
4 - 81 |
infants’ weight at birth (g) |
|
|
1265.47 |
308.20 |
590 - 2017 |
infants’ weight at observation (g) |
|
|
1538.05 |
237.72 |
1060 - 2185 |
Table 2.
Descriptive statistics of mothers’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR***) during silent baseline, humming and speech conditions.
Table 2.
Descriptive statistics of mothers’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR***) during silent baseline, humming and speech conditions.
Conditions |
Physiological parameters |
M |
SD |
min. – max. |
Silent baseline |
HR |
79.02 |
10.17 |
47.67 – 97.67 |
O2 saturation |
97.90 |
1.14 |
94.00 – 99.00 |
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
1.26 |
.19 |
.99 – 2.06 |
Humming |
HR |
81.55 |
10.75 |
49.00 – 101.00 |
O2 saturation |
97.92 |
1.10 |
94.33 – 99.00 |
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
1.22 |
.19 |
.95 – 2.00 |
Speech |
HR |
83.47 |
10.39 |
50.67 – 101.00 |
O2 saturation |
97.75 |
1.14 |
94.33 – 99.00 |
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
1.19 |
.18 |
.95 – 1.93 |
Table 3.
Descriptive statistics of infants’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR ***) during silent baseline, humming and speech conditions.
Table 3.
Descriptive statistics of infants’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR ***) during silent baseline, humming and speech conditions.
Conditions |
Physiological parameters |
M |
SD |
min. – max. |
Silent baseline |
HR |
156.24 |
11.37 |
137.00 – 181.00 |
O2 saturation |
97.68 |
3.33 |
86.36 – 100.00 |
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
.63 |
.05 |
.52 – .73 |
Humming |
HR |
153.31 |
13.54 |
119.00 – 185.67 |
O2 saturation |
97.19 |
3.15 |
88.00 – 100.00 |
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
.64 |
.06 |
.50 – .74 |
Speech |
HR |
157.32 |
12.08 |
130.00 – 188.00 |
O2 saturation |
97.11 |
3.02 |
88.00 – 100.00 |
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
.62 |
.06 |
.51 – .77 |
Table 4.
Comparative statistics between mothers’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR ***) during silent baseline (BL), humming (Hum), and speech (Sp) conditions.
Table 4.
Comparative statistics between mothers’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR ***) during silent baseline (BL), humming (Hum), and speech (Sp) conditions.
Physiological parameters |
t |
df |
p |
Cohen’s d |
HR |
BL vs. Hum |
-4.89 |
33 |
< .001 |
-.839 (e)
|
BL vs. Sp |
-5.39 |
33 |
< .001 |
-.926 (e)
|
Hum vs. Sp |
-2.04 |
32 |
.050 |
-.355 (c)
|
O2 saturation |
BL vs. Hum |
-.11 |
33 |
.917 |
-.018 (b)
|
BL vs. Sp |
1.59 |
33 |
.121 |
.273 (b)
|
Hum vs. Sp |
1.75 |
32 |
.089 |
-.355 (c)
|
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
BL vs. Hum |
5.06 |
33 |
< .001 |
.868 (e)
|
BL vs. Sp |
5.51 |
33 |
< .001 |
.945 (e)
|
Hum vs. Sp |
2.29 |
32 |
.029 |
.398 (c)
|
Table 5.
Comparative statistical analyses between infants’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR ***) during silent baseline (BL), humming (Hum), and speech (Sp) conditions.
Table 5.
Comparative statistical analyses between infants’ physiological variables (HR*, O2 saturation**, Prop. O2 saturation/HR ***) during silent baseline (BL), humming (Hum), and speech (Sp) conditions.
Physiological Parameters |
t |
df |
p |
Cohen’s d |
HR |
BL vs. Hum |
1.62 |
35 |
.114 |
.270 (b)
|
BL vs. Sp |
-.95 |
35 |
.349 |
-.158 (b)
|
Hum vs. Sp |
-2.29 |
35 |
.028 |
-.382 (c)
|
O2 saturation |
BL vs. Hum |
1.06 |
35 |
.297 |
.177 (b)
|
BL vs. Sp |
1.32 |
34 |
.195 |
.224(b)
|
Hum vs. Sp |
.00 |
34 |
1.000 |
.000 (a)
|
Prop. O2 saturation/HR |
BL vs. Hum |
-1.26 |
35 |
.218 |
-.209 (b)
|
BL vs. Sp |
1.37 |
34 |
.179 |
.232 (b)
|
Hum vs. Sp |
2.31 |
34 |
.027 |
.390 (c)
|
Table 6.
Multiple linear hierarchical regression for DV infants’ HR during humming and IV’s average duration of sinusoidal contours and of lengthening of the final notes.
Table 6.
Multiple linear hierarchical regression for DV infants’ HR during humming and IV’s average duration of sinusoidal contours and of lengthening of the final notes.
Model |
R |
R2
|
Adjusted R2
|
St. error of estimate |
R2 change |
F change |
df1 |
df2 |
Sig. Of F change |
1 |
.331 |
.109 |
.020 |
13.186 |
.109 |
1.228 |
3 |
30 |
.317 |
2 |
.336 |
.113 |
-.046 |
13.624 |
.003 |
.051 |
2 |
28 |
.951 |
3 |
.575 |
.331 |
.151 |
12.275 |
.218 |
4.246 |
2 |
26 |
.025 |
Table 7.
Multiple linear hierarchical regression for DV infants’ Prop. O2 saturation/HR* during humming and IV’s average duration of sinusoidal contours and of lengthening of the final notes.
Table 7.
Multiple linear hierarchical regression for DV infants’ Prop. O2 saturation/HR* during humming and IV’s average duration of sinusoidal contours and of lengthening of the final notes.
Model |
R |
R2
|
Adjusted R2
|
St. error of the estimate |
R2 change |
F change |
df1 |
df2 |
Sig. of F change |
1 |
.369 |
.136 |
.050 |
.051 |
.136 |
1.574 |
3 |
30 |
.216 |
2 |
.395 |
.156 |
.005 |
.052 |
.020 |
.327 |
2 |
28 |
.724 |
3 |
.587 |
.345 |
.168 |
.048 |
.189 |
3.749 |
2 |
26 |
.037 |