Introduction
Zebrafish (
Danio rerio) is a model organism widely used in different research areas including developmental biology, pharmacology, neurobiology and environmental toxicology [
1,
2,
3]. This organism exhibits a remarkable resemblance at genetic, molecular and physiological levels with mammals, thus allowing the use of zebrafish as a vertebrate model in biomedicine studies [
4,
5]. In the neurotoxicology field, the high degree of similarity observed between zebrafish and mammalian nervous systems has led to the increasing use of zebrafish early larvae to predict neurotoxic effects of environmental pollutants and drugs on vertebrates [
6,
7,
8]. Since zebrafish larvae between 5-8 days post-fertilisation (dpf) have an inflated swim bladder, well-developed sensorimotor systems and a size of about 3.0-3.5 m [
4], they are widely used for high-throughput analysis of neurobehavioural effects induced by chemical exposure. [
9]. Basal locomotor activity (BLA) and visual motor response (VMR), also known as light/dark transition test (LDT), are the two more commonly used behavioral assays on zebrafish larvae using commercially available video-tracking methodologies, such as DanioVision (Noldus, Wageningen, The Netherland) or ZebraBox (Viewpoint, Lyon, France) [
8]. Locomotor activity in zebrafish early larvae is an essential behavior for starting exogenous feeding and is the result of coordinated brain, motor, and sensory pathways [
10]. VMR is used to assess the locomotive and visual skills of zebrafish larvae by exposing them to alternating periods of light and darkness [
11].
Research employing zebrafish behavioral methods encounters a significant obstacle, namely, the inconsistency of findings within individual labs and across multiple research facilities. The absence of universally accepted testing procedures for assessing zebrafish behavior activities likely contributes to these variations in results among different labs [
9]. To ensure the reliability and replicability of the results, it is important to recognize and define the intrinsic and extrinsic factors that influence the larval behavior and its assessment [
12]. Among the intrinsic factors, the age of the larvae and the time of day are known to strongly influence on the outcome of the assays. The state of maturity of the neurotransmitter systems, the motor system and the retinal OFF channels, the sensory circuit responsible for gathering visual stimuli, will increase with the age of the larvae, whereby age could affect the results of these behavioral assays [
13,
14,
15]. In addition, the diurnal locomotor activity of larvae appears to be modulated by circadian rhythms, so the outcomes of the assays may be profoundly affected by the timing of the test [
3,
16]. Finally, different experiments could lead to some differences in the outcome of each assay due to the different batches of the larvae used.
In this study, our primary aim was to investigate how age, time of day, experiment, and their interactions modulate larvae behavior. To achieve this goal, BLA and VMR have been assessed daily, from 5-8 dpf, in 143 larvae, from three independent experiments, 7 times a day (08:00, 10:00, 12:00, 14:00, 16:00, 18:00, and 20:00 h). The results presented here provide new information on the effects of these three intrinsic factors and their interactions, on the results of BLA and VMR. In addition, the most convenient periods for conducting each test, based on stability criteria, are proposed for each age.
Materials and Methods
Fish husbandry and larvae production
Wild-type short-fin adult zebrafish were purchased from Pisciber (Barcelona, Spain) and maintained at the Research and Development Centre of the Spanish Research Council (CID-CSIC) facilities. Zebrafish embryos were obtained through natural mating, maintaining a female-to-male ratio of 3:2 in each mating tank. After collection, fertilized embryos were transferred to 6-well plates, with 20 embryos per well in 10 mL of fish water [MilliQ-water containing 90 mg/L of Instant Ocean (Aquarium Systems, Sarrebourg, France), 0.58 mM CaSO
4·2H2O; pH 6.5-7.0 and 750–900 μS/cm of conductivity], and kept at 28°± 1°C within a temperature-controlled chamber (POL-EKO APARATURA Climatic chamber KK350, Poland), following a 12L -12D cycle (light period: 08:00-20:00 h). Fish water was renewed after the first 48 hours. During the 5-8 dpf, zebrafish larvae undergo an endoexotrophic nutritional period in which they begin external feeding while continuing to consume residual yolk [
17]. Since external feeding may be an additional source of variability, larvae were not fed throughout the experiment. All processes received approval from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee
(IACUC) at CID-CSIC and complied with the institution's guidelines, authorized by the local government under agreement N
o 11336.
Experimental procedure
Following the criteria commonly used in Developmental Biology [
18], the term “larva” will be used across the manuscript for any zebrafish, reared at 28°C and older than 3 dpf (hatching period). According to Directive 2010/63/EU on animal experimentation, however, zebrafish reared at 28°C is considered an “embryo” until the 5 dpf, when this organism starts independent feeding. Therefore, the 5 dpf larvae tested in this study are considered eleutheroembryos from a regulatory point of view.
For this study, 4 dpf zebrafish larvae were randomly transferred into a 48-well plate for the behavioral assessment. Each well contained one embryo in 1 mL of fish water. The plate was then covered with aluminum foil to prevent light disturbances and moved back into the temperature-controlled unit, ready to be used the next day. While handling the plate on the days of the experiment, the room was kept in darkness. After the last cycle, the plate was once again covered in aluminum foil and placed back inside the incubator. Throughout the experiment, 5, 6, 7, and 8 dpf, the larvae remained in the same well. Medium was introduced into the wells as needed to counteract potential evaporation and uphold water quality. This study took place from the 5th to the 8th dpf, starting at 8 a.m. and ending at 8 p.m. each day. The behavior of each larva was assessed every 2 hours. Behavioral testing was conducted in three different experiments, considering an experiment a microplate with 48 zebrafish larvae collected on the same day from the same spawn.
Behavioral analysis
Behavioral assessments in zebrafish larvae
Behavioral assessments in fish were carried out using the DanioVision platform (Noldus, Wageningen, The Netherland), consisting of a DanioVision Observation Chamber (DVOC) equipped with near-infrared illumination and temperature control unit to maintain the system at 28°C during all trials. Video tracking was conducted using the EthoVision XT13 and behavioral analysis with the Noldus EthoVision XT 16 software (Wageningen, The Netherland). Recordings were made at a frame rate of 25 frames per second (fps). Larvae were allowed to acclimate in the DVOC for 60 minutes in dark conditions before the start of the recording. The routine started recording with a period of 15 minutes in dark conditions, and then, suddenly, a light phase (intensity: 100%, duration: 10 minutes) began. The light period was followed by a dark phase (intensity: 0%, duration: 95 minutes). 7 repetitions of this routine were performed. Then, a new trial of the assessment began. This resulted in obtaining data every 2 hours, from 8 a.m. to 8 p.m. The EthoVision software's multi-tracking module was employed to analyze the visual motor response (VMR) and basal locomotor activity (BLA) for each individual larva. The VMR and BLA were evaluated according to methods previously described by Bedrossiantz et al. [
19]. Briefly, VMR was calculated by subtracting the distance covered during the first 2 minutes of darkness from the distance covered during the final 2 minutes of the preceding light period in centimeters. BLA was defined by calculating the total distance (cm) that larvae travelled during a 10-minute period in darkness. Following the tracking process, a quality control step was implemented, where wells displaying incorrect tracking were excluded from the final dataset.
Data analysis
A total of 3 independent experiments (Experiment 1: 48 larvae/ Experiment 2: 48 larvae/ Experiment 3: 47 larvae; each experiment was conducted on a different day) were analyzed in this study, giving a total number of 143 larvae. BLA and VMR of each larva was measured during 4 consecutive days, from 5 to 8 dpf. At each age, these behaviors were determined in each larva every 2 hours during the whole daytime period (from 8 a.m. to 8 p.m.), giving a total of 7 measurements per larva and age. Therefore, each behavior was determined 4,032 times in total.
Statistical analysis
The data structure was longitudinal with a series of 28 measurements per larva. Graphical and numerical descriptive results were calculated. Standard deviation (SD) between larvae (over larvae) and within larvae (over days/hours) were calculated together with the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (Rho). Normality tests and graphs were used to check the assumption of normal distribution of BLA and VMR variables before adjusting a Random-Effects (RE) linear regression model [
20], using generalized least squares (GLS), to take into account the clustering (correlation) of the measures of each larvae over time (over days/hours). The model included the main effect of variables day (age), time (hours), experiment (replicate), the second-way interactions between them and the three-way interaction, as showed in Eq. (1):
and for each
i,
, of which
periods were actually observed
being = Age (dpf), = Time (hours) and = Replicate our categorical factors.
Intraclass Correlation of the error of the model (ρ) and within, between and global R-squared (squared correlation between original and fitted values of dependent variable) were calculated [
20]. Goodness of fit and post-estimation fitted model predictions (predictive margins) were assessed and compared. Pairwise comparisons were adjusted using the Bonferroni method.
P-values ≤ 0.05 were considered statistically significant and
P-values between the interval (0.05;0.10) were considered borderline for statistical significance [
21]. STATA 17.0 statistical software was used to perform all statistical analysis [
22].
Results
Basal locomotor activity (BLA)
Table 1 summarizes the main descriptive results of 3,814 analyses of BLA performed on a total of 143 zebrafish larvae in this study. Due to the tracking errors identified during the analysis of a few larvae, there was a small percentage of missing data (5.41 %). The overall BLA of the larvae during the 28 reads was 133.14 ± 28.99 cm (mean ± SD), with 83.57% of the observed variability explained by the intra-individual differences. When the effect of the age of the larvae, the time of the day, and the experiment was considered (
Table 1 and
Figure 1), the differences within each larva also explained most of the observed variability, with percentages (
1-Rho) ranging from 57.81% (5 dpf larvae) to 85.36% (larvae from the 3
rd replicate).
BLA results were adjusted to the age of the larvae (5-8 dpf), the time of the day (08:00-20:00 h), the experiment (1-3) and the full interactions, using a RE linear regression model, being this model able to explain the 16.8% of the total variation (
Supplementary Table S1).
Intra-individual differences (within) accounted for 18.84% of the variation, while the inter-individual differences (between) accounted for only 6.15%. The variable experiment had a big effect on the model, being the marginal effect of experiment 1 statistically lower than experiments 2 (P= 0.004) and 3 (P= 0.018). Adjusting for this variable, marginal effects of the age were statistically different between all pairwise comparisons except for the comparison between 5 and 7 dpf (
P= 1.000), with the biggest difference (-13.25 cm/10 min) found between 6 and 8 dpf (95%CI: -16.22 to -10.28) (
Table S2). Finally, the marginal effect of the time of the day on BLA levels of the larvae at 08:00 h was significantly lower than at 12:00 h (P= 0.001) and 16:00 h (P= 0.000), but significantly higher than at 18:00 (P= 0.000) and 20:00 h (P= 0.000) (
Table S3).
Next, the marginal effect of the time of the day on the BLA levels of the larvae, adjusted by the replicate effect, was determined. No differences were found in the BLA predictive marginal effects between 10:00 and 16:00 h (P= 0.672). Whereas the marginal predictive effects of BLA between 16:00 and 18:00 hours showed a significant (P= 0.000) decrease of -15.76 cm/10 min (95% CI: -20.19 to -11.34), the greatest decrease, -24.60 cm/10 min (95%CI: -29.86 to -19.34), was found between 16:00 and 20:00 hours (P= 0.000).
Finally, the continuous periods of time in which BLA is stable were analyzed for each age (
Figure 2 and Figure 5,
Table S4). For 5 dpf larvae, no differences were found between the BLA predicted values during the time 8:00 and period 12:00 to 16:00 h. For 6 dpf larvae, BLA predicted values were similar during the 08:00 to 10:00 h and from 14:00 to 16:00 h. BLA predicted values at 12:00 h were only 7% higher than those at 14:00 h (141.29 cm and minimum value of the period), but this difference was statistically significant (P= 0.012). For 7 dpf larvae, BLA predicted values were similar during the period from 10:00 to 16:00 h. Finally, for the 8 dpf larvae, the predicted BLA values were similar between 08:00 and 14:00 h.
Visual motor response (VMR)
Table 2 summarizes the main descriptive results of 3699 analyses of VMR performed on a total of 143 zebrafish larvae in this study. As for BLA, due to the tracking errors identified in a few wells during some reads, there is a small percentage of missing data (8.26%). The average VMR was 12.79 ± 8.56 cm, with 81.86% of the observed variability explained by the differences within each larva. When the effect of the age of the larvae, the time of the day, and the experiment was considered (
Table 2 and
Figure 3), the differences within each larva also explained most of the observed variability, with percentages ranging from 50.91% (8 dpf larvae) to 86.43% (larvae from the 3
rd replicate).
When VMR results were adjusted to the age, time of the day, experiment, and their full interactions using the RE linear regression model, the model was able to explain 8.4% of the total variation (
Supplementary Table S1). Intra-individual differences accounted for 8.17% of the variation, while inter-individual differences accounted for 8.95%. The variable experiment had a big effect on the model, being the marginal effect of replicate 1 statistically lower than replicate 2 (P= 0.000) and 3 (P= 0.001). Adjusting for this variable, the predictive marginal effect of VMR in 8 dpf was statistically lower when pairwise comparisons with 6 dpf (P= 0.000) and 7 dpf (P= 0.024), with the biggest difference (-1.29 cm) found between 6 and 8 dpf (95%CI: -2.14 to -0.44) (
Table S5). Finally, the marginal effect of the time of the day on the VMR levels of the larvae, adjusted by the replicate effect, was determined. Whereas no differences in the marginal predictive values were found between 10:00, 12:00, 14:00, and 16:00 h, and between 8:00, 18:00, and 20:00 h (P=0.423), an increase of 3.52 cm was found between 10:00 and 20:00 h (95%CI: 2.04 to 4.99) (
Table S6).
Finally, the continuous periods of time where VMR is stable were determined for each age (
Figure 4 and
Figure 5,
Table S7). For 5 dpf larvae, no differences were found between the VMR values predicted during the period of time from 10:00 to 16:00 h. For 6 dpf larvae, VMR predicted values were similar during 10:00-18:00 h. For 7 dpf and 8 dpf larvae, VMR predicted values were similar in both ages between 10:00-16:00 h.
Discussion
In this work, we have shown that several intrinsic variables significantly affect the locomotor behavior of zebrafish larvae. When locomotor behavior is used to assess the neurotoxicity of one chemical or a chemical mixture, it is assumed that any changes in behavior are due to the chemical treatment. However, we have seen here that the locomotor behavior of 5-8 dpf zebrafish larvae is extremely sensitive to different factors other than chemical treatment, and if these variables are not conveniently controlled, they can lead to type 1 and 2 errors. Whereas different studies have already reported a significant effect of the age of the larvae on BLA, this effect seems to be modulated by the interactions with other intrinsic and extrinsic factors. Celine de Esch et al. found that the effect of the age on BLA in larvae from 5-7 dpf was strongly dependent on the strain (5 dpf> 6 dpf > 7 dpf for AB strain and 7 dpf > 5 dpf ≈ 6 dpf for TL strain) [
23]. In our study, however, the population of wild-type adult zebrafish (blue-fin-short phenotype) maintained at CID-CSIC is genetically heterogeneous, and therefore more representative of the variability of natural populations than wild-type laboratory strains [
24,
25]. Moreover, there are differences in the size of the experimental arena between de Esch’s study (96-wells microplates) and ours (48-wells microplates), and this extrinsic factor has been reported to modify the outcome of both BLA and VMR [
12]. Therefore, differences in the zebrafish strain and experimental arena size could lead to the differences observed between de Esch’s study and the results presented in this manuscript (6 dpf > 5 dpf ≈ 7 dpf > 8 dpf). Moreover, the low BLA observed in 7 and 8 dpf larvae compared to 6 dpf larvae in our study could be related to a decrease in energy supply in the older larvae, as they did not receive external feeding and by 7-8 dpf the yolk sac is almost depleted of nutrients [
26]. Ingebretson et al. found that 4 dpf larvae exhibited significantly higher BLA levels than 7 dpf larvae [
27]. Again, the differences in the experimental conditions used by Ingebretson and ours (different diameter of the well fron 6-wells vs 48 wells microplates, light vs dark conditions, 4 dpf vs 5 dpf larvae, 1 measure vs 7 measures per day) make very difficult, if possible, a direct comparison of the results.
About the effect of the time of day on BLA performance, MacPhail et al. reported, in 6 dpf larvae, an initial decrease in BLA levels from 10:00 to 13:00 h and then, a stability period between 13:00 and 15:30 h. [
28]. In MacPhail’s study the 6 dpf larvae were raised at 26° C, and considering the algorithm proposed by Kimmel to compare the developmental stage of embryos raised at different temperatures, a 144 hpf (6 dpf) larvae reared at 26°C is equivalent to 124 hpf (about 5 dpf ) larvae reared to the standard 28.5°C [
18]. Therefore, when we compare results by MacPhail with those presented in this manuscript for 5 dpf larvae, despite of the differences in photoperiod (14:10 vs 12:12) and the arena size (98 wells vs 48 wells), there is a period of stability between 13:00 to 16:00 h. Finally, Kristofco et al. found that for 5-8 dpf larvae, BLA at 09:00 was significantly lower than at other times of the day, increasing thereafter until 12:00 h and reaching the highest stability during the period 14:00-18:00 h [
29]. Again, the different experimental conditions used in both studies could be behind the observed differences in the modulatory effect of the time of the day on BLA, which emphasizes the urgent need to harmonize the conditions of the assay (strain, embryo rearing conditions, experimental arena size, time of day, lighting conditions…) in order to make it reproducible among laboratories.
The sudden decrease in light intensity triggers an acute locomotor response in zebrafish larvae [
13], also known as the light-off VMR. The sensory circuit involved in this response is the OFF channel of the retina, which, in response to a sudden decrease in illumination, transmits signals from photoreceptors to specific OFF ganglion and bipolar cells, and ultimately to higher visual centers [
14]. Activation of this circuit evokes the O-turns, followed by an hyperactivity period [
30]. The light-off VMR is the most relevant component of the light/dark transition test (LDT), one of the most frequently used behavioral assays with zebrafish larvae [
31]. This assay is generally based on measuring the locomotor response induced with a series of alternating light and dark periods [
32]. There is a great variability in the number and duration of light and dark periods of the LDT between laboratories [
23,
28,
33,
34,
35,
36,
37]. In addition to these extrinsic factors, also some intrinsic factors, such as age and time of day, have been shown to modulate the outcome of LDT [
12,
29,
32]. While in our study 6 dpf larvae exhibited higher light-off VMR than 5 dpf larvae, Stephanie Padilla et al. found that the light-off VMR was higher for 5 dpf than for 6 dpf larvae [
12]. The differences in the rearing (26°C vs 28°C, 14L:10D vs 12L:12D photoperiod) and the size of the experimental arena (96 wells vs 48 wells) could explain the differences observed between both studies. In contrast to the differences observed with the age in the VMR in our study, no differences were found by Jennifer A. Fitzgerald et al. [
35]. However, the differences in the rearing conditions (26°C vs 28°C, 14L:10D vs 12L:12D photoperiod) and the effect of the time of day (09:00 vs 14:00 h) make difficult a direct comparison. As already discussed for BLA, there is an urgent need to harmonize the LDT protocol in order to allow the comparison of results across laboratories.
A possible objection to the experimental design used in the present study is that BLA and MRV were analyzed in each larva 28 times in total, so in most of the trials the larvae were not experimentally naïve. However, neither motor activity, as measured by the BLA, nor the escape reflex evoked by a visual stimulus, measured by the VMR, is dependent on novelty. Larvae were placed in a multi-well plates at 4 dpf and from day 5 to 8 the plate was placed 12 hours per day inside the DVOC. During these 12 h, the larvae were unaware on when they are being recorded. Habituation is a progressive decrease in the responses when a series of stimuli are presented to the same animal[
38]. However, with our design the possibility of habituation with the visual stimuli can be ruled out, as the interstimulus interval is of 2 h and habituation only happens when the interstimulus interval is very short, of the order of 1 second [
38].
Finally, there is a lack of studies analyzing the effect of the experiment on the outcome of behavioral analysis in zebrafish larvae. The results presented in this study show, however, that the experiment is an important intrinsic factor of variability for BLA and VMR, both alone or in combination with age or time of day. Further efforts should be made to better the effect of this factor on behavior.
Conclusions
The first conclusion of this study is that researchers using behavioral analysis in neurotoxicity studies should be aware of all the intrinsic and extrinsic factors that potentially modulate zebrafish behavior. Second, the stability periods for BLA and light-off VMR identified in this study will be only useful for studies using experimental conditions similar to those described here. Third, more efforts should be invested in the study of the effect of the different experiments on the outcome of the assays. Finally, the data presented in this manuscript emphasize urgent need to harmonize the conditions for conducting behavioral assays with zebrafish larvae.
Supplementary Materials
The following are available online at the website of this paper posted on Preprints.org. Supplementary Figures: Figure S1. Understanding the 3-way interaction between age, hours and replicate for BLA and VMR in the RE linear regression model; Supplementary Tables: Table S1. Random-effects (RE) linear regression model estimation using generalized least squares (GLS) for BLA and VMR variables. Main effects, full two-way interactions, and the three-way interaction included; Table S2. Pairwise comparisons of predictive margins of BLA for age. Differences and P-values corrected using the Bonferroni method to account for multiple comparisons; Table S3. Pairwise comparisons of predictive margins of BLA for time of the day. Differences and P-values corrected using the Bonferroni method to account for multiple comparisons; Table S4. Pairwise comparisons of predictive margins of BLA for time of the day. Differences and P-values corrected using the Bonferroni method to account for multiple comparisons; Table S5. Pairwise comparisons of predictive margins of VMR for age. Differences and P-values corrected using the Bonferroni method to account for multiple comparisons; Table S6. Pairwise comparisons of predictive margins of VMR for time of the day. Differences and P-values corrected using the Bonferroni method to account for multiple comparisons; Table S7. Pairwise comparisons of predictive margins of VMR for time of the day. Differences and P-values corrected using the Bonferroni method to account for multiple comparisons.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, D.R.; Methodology, J.B.; Validation, N.T. and C.R.M.; Formal Analysis, M.J.B. and C.B.; Investigation, N.T., C.R.M., and E.P.; Resources, D.R., C.F.; Writing – Original Draft Preparation, D.R., N.T., C.R.M., J.B., E.P., C.B., and C.F.; Writing – Review & Editing, D.R., N.T., C.R.M., J.N., E.P., C.B., and C.F.; Visualization, D.R., M.J.B., J.B.; Supervision, D.R., C.F.; Project Administration, D.R., and C.B.; Funding Acquisition, D.R., C.B, and C.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by “Agencia Estatal de Investigación” from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (projects PID2020-113371RB-C21 and TED2021-130845B-C3), and IDAEA-CSIC, Severo Ochoa Centre of Excellence (CEX2018-000794-S). Juliette Bedrossiantz was supported by a PhD grant (PRE2018-083513) co-financed by the Spanish Government and the European Social Fund (ESF).
Institutional Review Board Statement
All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees at the CID-CSIC and conducted in accordance with the institutional guidelines under a license from the local government (agreement number 9027).
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable
Data Availability Statement
Data supporting the findings of this study are available within the manuscript, and its Supplementary Material file, or will be available from the corresponding author upon request.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- Ilie, O.D.; Duta, R.; Jijie, R.; Nita, I.B.; Nicoara, M.; Faggio, C.; Dobrin, R.; Mavroudis, I.; Ciobica, A.; Doroftei, B. Assessing Anti-Social and Aggressive Behavior in a Zebrafish (Danio rerio) Model of Parkinson’s Disease Chronically Exposed to Rotenone. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rashidian, G.; Mohammadi-Aloucheh, R.; Hosseinzadeh-Otaghvari, F.; Chupani, L.; Stejskal, V.; Samadikhah, H.; Zamanlui, S.; Multisanti, C.R.; Faggio, C. Long-term exposure to small-sized silica nanoparticles (SiO2-NPs) induces oxidative stress and impairs reproductive performance in adult zebrafish (Danio rerio). Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part C Toxicol. Pharmacol. 2023, 273, 109715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ricarte, M.; Prats, E.; Montemurro, N.; Bedrossiantz, J.; Bellot, M.; Gómez-Canela, C.; Raldúa, D. Environmental concentrations of tire rubber-derived 6PPD-quinone alter CNS function in zebrafish larvae. Sci. Total Environ. 2023, 896, 165240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Basnet, R.M.; Zizioli, D.; Taweedet, S.; Finazzi, D.; Memo, M. Zebrafish larvae as a behavioral model in neuropharmacology. Biomedicines 2019, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strähle, U.; Scholz, S.; Geisler, R.; Greiner, P.; Hollert, H.; Rastegar, S.; Schumacher, A.; Selderslaghs, I.; Weiss, C.; Witters, H.; et al. Zebrafish embryos as an alternative to animal experiments-A commentary on the definition of the onset of protected life stages in animal welfare regulations. Reprod. Toxicol. 2012, 33, 128–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hill, B.N.; Coldsnow, K.D.; Hunter, D.L.; Hedge, J.M.; Korest, D.; Jarema, K.A.; Padilla, S. Assessment of Larval Zebrafish Locomotor Activity for Developmental Neurotoxicity Screening. Neuromethods 2021, 172, 327–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faria, M.; Bellot, M.; Bedrossiantz, J.; Ramírez, J.R.R.; Prats, E.; Garcia-Reyero, N.; Gomez-Canela, C.; Mestres, J.; Rovira, X.; Barata, C.; et al. Environmental levels of carbaryl impair zebrafish larvae behaviour: The potential role of ADRA2B and HTR2B. J. Hazard. Mater. 2022, 431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faria, M.; Prats, E.; Rosas Ramírez, J.R.; Bellot, M.; Bedrossiantz, J.; Pagano, M.; Valls, A.; Gomez-Canela, C.; Porta, J.M.; Mestres, J.; et al. Androgenic activation, impairment of the monoaminergic system and altered behavior in zebrafish larvae exposed to environmental concentrations of fenitrothion. Sci. Total Environ. 2021, 775, 145671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hill, B.N.; Britton, K.N.; Hunter, D.L.; Olin, J.K.; Lowery, M.; Hedge, J.M.; Knapp, B.R.; Jarema, K.A.; Rowson, Z.; Padilla, S. Inconsistencies in variable reporting and methods in larval zebrafish behavioral assays. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2023, 96, 107163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cassar, S.; Adatto, I.; Freeman, J.L.; Gamse, J.T.; Iturria, I.; Lawrence, C.; Muriana, A.; Peterson, R.T.; Van Cruchten, S.; Zon, L.I. Use of Zebrafish in Drug Discovery Toxicology. Chem. Res. Toxicol. 2020, 33, 95–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosa, J.G.S.; Lima, C.; Lopes-Ferreira, M. Zebrafish Larvae Behavior Models as a Tool for Drug Screenings and Pre-Clinical Trials: A Review. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Padilla, S.; Hunter, D.L.; Padnos, B.; Frady, S.; MacPhail, R.C. Assessing locomotor activity in larval zebrafish: Influence of extrinsic and intrinsic variables. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2011, 33, 624–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fero, K.; Yokogawa, T.; Burgess, H.A. The behavioral repertoire of larval zebrafish. In Zebrafish models in neurobehavioral research; Springer, 2011; pp. 249–291. [Google Scholar]
- Tian, N.; Copenhagen, D.R. Visual stimulation is required for refinement on ON and OFF pathways in postnatal retina. Neuron 2003, 39, 85–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tufi, S.; Leonards, P.; Lamoree, M.; De Boer, J.; Legler, J.; Legradi, J. Changes in Neurotransmitter Profiles during Early Zebrafish (Danio rerio) Development and after Pesticide Exposure. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2016, 50, 3222–3230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolter, M.E.; Svoboda, K.R. Doing the locomotion: Insights and potential pitfalls associated with using locomotor activity as a readout of the circadian rhythm in larval zebrafish. J. Neurosci. Methods 2020, 330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poupard, G.; André, M.; Durliat, M.; Ballagny, C.; Boeuf, G.; Babin, P.J. Apolipoprotein E gene expression correlates with endogenous lipid nutrition and yolk syncytial layer lipoprotein synthesis during fish development. Cell Tissue Res. 2000, 300, 251–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kimmel, C.B.; Ballard, W.W.; Kimmel, S.R.; Ullmann, B.; Schilling, T.F. Stages of embryonic development of the zebrafish. Dev. Dyn. 1995, 203, 253–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bedrossiantz, J.; Faria, M.; Prats, E.; Barata, C.; Cachot, J.; Raldúa, D. Heart rate and behavioral responses in three phylogenetically distant aquatic model organisms exposed to environmental concentrations of carbaryl and fenitrothion. Sci. Total Environ. 2023, 865, 161268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cameron, A.; Trivedi, P.K. Microeconometrics Using Stata, 2nd ed.; Stata Press: Texas, 2022; ISBN 978-1-59718-359-8. [Google Scholar]
- Armitage, P. Statistical methods in medical research; John Wiley and Sons: New York, NY, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp, L. Stata statistical software: Release 17. Coll. Stn. StataCorp LP 2021.
- de Esch, C.; van der Linde, H.; Slieker, R.; Willemsen, R.; Wolterbeek, A.; Woutersen, R.; De Groot, D. Locomotor activity assay in zebrafish larvae: Influence of age, strain and ethanol. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2012, 34, 425–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maximino, C.; Meinerz, D.L.; Fontana, B.D.; Mezzomo, N.J.; Stefanello, F. V.; de, S. Prestes, A.; Batista, C.B.; Rubin, M.A.; Barbosa, N. V.; Rocha, J.B.T.; et al. Extending the analysis of zebrafish behavioral endophenotypes for modeling psychiatric disorders: Fear conditioning to conspecific alarm response. Behav. Processes 2018, 149, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lima, M.G.; Silva, R.X.d.C.; Silva, S. de N. dos S.; Rodrigues, L. do S. dos S.; Oliveira, K.R.H.M.; Batista, E. de J.O.; Maximino, C.; Herculano, A.M. Time-dependent sensitization of stress responses in zebrafish: A putative model for post-traumatic stress disorder. Behav. Processes 2016, 128, 70–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Selderslaghs, I.W.T.; Hooyberghs, J.; De Coen, W.; Witters, H.E. Locomotor activity in zebrafish embryos: A new method to assess developmental neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2010, 32, 460–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ingebretson, J.J.; Masino, M.A. Quantification of locomotor activity in larval Zebrafish: Considerations for the design of high-throughput behavioral studies. Front. Neural Circuits 2013, 7, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacPhail, R.C.; Brooks, J.; Hunter, D.L.; Padnos, B.; Irons, T.D.; Padilla, S. Locomotion in larval zebrafish: Influence of time of day, lighting and ethanol. Neurotoxicology 2009, 30, 52–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kristofco, L.A.; Cruz, L.C.; Haddad, S.P.; Behra, M.L.; Chambliss, C.K.; Brooks, B.W. Age matters: Developmental stage of Danio rerio larvae influences photomotor response thresholds to diazinion or diphenhydramine. Aquat. Toxicol. 2016, 170, 344–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fernandes, A.M.; Fero, K.; Arrenberg, A.B.; Bergeron, S.A.; Driever, W.; Burgess, H.A. Deep brain photoreceptors control light-seeking behavior in zebrafish larvae. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, 2042–2047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haigis, A.C.; Ottermanns, R.; Schiwy, A.; Hollert, H.; Legradi, J. Getting more out of the zebrafish light dark transition test. Chemosphere 2022, 295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ogungbemi, A.; Leuthold, D.; Scholz, S.; Küster, E. Hypo- or hyperactivity of zebrafish embryos provoked by neuroactive substances: a review on how experimental parameters impact the predictability of behavior changes. Environ. Sci. Eur. 2019, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rock, S.; Rodenburg, F.; Schaaf, M.J.M.; Tudorache, C. Detailed Analysis of Zebrafish Larval Behaviour in the Light Dark Challenge Assay Shows That Diel Hatching Time Determines Individual Variation. Front. Physiol. 2022, 13, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fraser, T.W.K.; Khezri, A.; Jusdado, J.G.H.; Lewandowska-Sabat, A.M.; Henry, T.; Ropstad, E. Toxicant induced behavioural aberrations in larval zebrafish are dependent on minor methodological alterations. Toxicol. Lett. 2017, 276, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitzgerald, J.A.; Kirla, K.T.; Zinner, C.P.; vom Berg, C.M. Emergence of consistent intra-individual locomotor patterns during zebrafish development. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nüßer, L.K.; Skulovich, O.; Hartmann, S.; Seiler, T.B.; Cofalla, C.; Schuettrumpf, H.; Hollert, H.; Salomons, E.; Ostfeld, A. A sensitive biomarker for the detection of aquatic contamination based on behavioral assays using zebrafish larvae. Ecotoxicol. Environ. Saf. 2016, 133, 271–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Maeda, H.; Fukushima, N.; Hasumi, A. Standardized method for the assessment of behavioral responses of zebrafish larvae. Biomedicines 2021, 9, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faria, M.; Prats, E.; Novoa-Luna, K.A.; Bedrossiantz, J.; Gómez-Canela, C.; Gómez-Oliván, L.M.; Raldúa, D. Development of a vibrational startle response assay for screening environmental pollutants and drugs impairing predator avoidance. Sci. Total Environ. 2019, 650, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).