Preprint
Article

Perceptions of Animal Welfare on Livestock: Evidence from College Agronomy Students in Costa Rica

Altmetrics

Downloads

109

Views

56

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

02 April 2024

Posted:

03 April 2024

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Ethical considerations regarding our treatment of animals have gained strength, leading to legislation and a societal focus across various disciplines. This is a subject of study within curricula related to agri-food sciences. The aim was to determine the perceptions of agronomy university students concerning animal welfare in livestock production systems. A survey was conducted to encompass various aspects, from participants' sociodemographic attributes to their attitudes and behaviors regarding animal welfare and the consumption of animal products. Statistical analysis, performed using R software, delved into the associations between participants' characteristics and their perspectives on the ethical, bioethical, and legal dimensions of animal welfare. Associations between demographic factors and ethical viewpoints among students were identified. Gender differences emerged in animal treatment perceptions, while rural and urban environments impacted perspectives on various animals. Bioethical considerations revealed distinctive disparities based on gender and education in concerns for animal welfare, value perceptions, evaluations of animal behaviors, and opinions on animal research. It is crucial to distinguish between animal welfare and ethical considerations arising from coexisting with sentient beings capable of experiencing suffering. Ethical theories provide a lens through which we perceive our obligations toward animals. The responsibility to ensure animal welfare is firmly rooted in recognizing that animals, like humans, experience pain and physical suffering. Consequently, actions causing unjustified suffering or mistreatment, particularly for entertainment purposes, are considered morally unacceptable.
Keywords: 
Subject: Biology and Life Sciences  -   Animal Science, Veterinary Science and Zoology

1. Introduction

Since the 1970s, the ethical concern regarding our relationship with animals has shifted from relatively limitated [1] to progressively growing, almost exponentially, in recent times [2,3,4]. This surge has directly impacted legislation, making it increasingly protective of animal welfare [5]. Human ethics are crucial as they can justify obligations towards animals [6,7,8] and are rooted in Ethics or Moral Philosophy [9] by addressing imperatives that compel humans to act in specific ways ([10]. Yet, understanding animal biology is necessary when delineating these obligations [11,12].
A significant ethical consideration is firmly rooted in society's agreement that animals shouldn’t suffer [13,14]. However, the extent to which animals can be utilized as models in clinical trials for human pain treatment raises questions [15,16]. While not delving into animal suffering and consciousness, no harm should be caused to animals, including that which may arise from their participation in human entertainment activities [17,18,19]. Legal considerations, or alternatively, deontological aspects, further add to these debates [20]. The Five-Domains Model for animal welfare assessment en compasses scientific issues relate to this discipline [21], however, it can also be used in conjunction with ethical theories to clarify our relationship with animals in various production or entertainment contexts [22,23,24].
It is also necessary to differentiate between animal welfare and the ethical issue arising from our coexistence with nonhuman sentient beings capable of experiencing physical suffering. Animal welfare involves an organism whose neuroendocrine system, akin to humans, should not exhibit physiological alterations. Emotional manifestations (e.g., fear or stress) should not deviate from what occurs in normal conditions [25,26,27]. Practically, welfare is assessed through animal behavior observation [28]. Positive human-animal relationships have been highlighted by several authors to contribute intrinsically to animal welfare [29,30,31,32].
The Five Domains Model (since 1994) for animal welfare assessment provides specific guidance on how to evaluate the negative and/or positive impacts of human behavior on animals [21]. This model facilitates systematic and structured assessment of positive as well as negative welfare-related affects, the circumstances that give rise to them and potential interactions between both types of affects, all of which extend the utility of the model [33]. The Five Domains are clearly of use to animal behavior and welfare scientists because they can embrace new knowledge and understanding and provide pointers for new study. They can also be used for in-depth analysis of the impact of specific management practices (human actions) on animal welfare [34].
Numerous ethical theories focus on the human-animal bond, with most centered-on animal suffering [4,35]. The theory of reciprocal recognition suggests avoiding animal cruelty by recognizing the harm caused, considering both animal and human interests [36]. This ideology aligns with a derivative of utilitarianism [37,38], which advocates maximizing pleasure and minimizing suffering for sentient beings—forming the basis of ethics and obligations towards animals [18,39,40]. Such utilitarianism is common among professionals working with animals, aiming to prevent suffering and enhance animal welfare in livestock production [28,41].
The responsibility to ensure animal welfare is rooted in the established fact that animals experience physical pain and suffering [16,42]. Therefore, causing unjustified suffering, such as in entertainment involving cruelty, mistreatment, or unnatural conditions contrary to their physiological and behavioral needs, is not morally acceptable [6].
University education in agronomy promotes the human-animal bond in a practical academic learning environment, sensitizing students to animal welfare as they progress through their study programs [43], considering socio-demographic attributes of the student population. This study aimed to determine the perceptions of agronomy university students concerning animal welfare in livestock production systems.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Data Source

The data used was collected from the Animal Welfare survey administered to students majoring in Agronomy Engineering at the Costa Rica Institute of Technology (ITCR) as part of an exploratory study on attitudes, behaviors, and perceptions toward animal welfare and meat consumption among Agronomy Engineering students at ITCR between May and July 2023. The survey gathered a wide range of information, including the participants' sociodemographic aspects, interaction with pet animals, general information about animal welfare, knowledge on the ethical dimension of animal welfare, consumption of animal products, consumption of meat and other animal-derived meat products, as well as attitudes towards meat products within animal welfare considerations. The data is available on the Open Science Framework. All interviewees were 18 years or older.

2.2. Data Collection

Data were collected in person through a survey. Prior to this, each participant was sent a consent form for participation, use of collected information, and data treatment within the framework of the research study. From the total respondents (n = 150), students who did not consent to participate in the research or were under 18 years old (n = 6) were excluded from the statistical analysis. A total of 144 surveys were analyzed.
The questionnaire comprised four parts distributed as follows: 1) Participant sociodemographic aspects, 2) Generalities on animal welfare, 3) Ethical and bioethics dimension of animal welfare, and 4) Animal welfare within the framework of bio-law.
To develop the questionnaire, recommendations were followed to maintain the wording as simple and direct as possible, using symmetric response scales and framing elements both positively and negatively. Moreover, an expert consultation phase was included before the questionnaire's administration. Regarding differences in response scale usage, the recommendations were to employ symmetric bipolar response scales with a clear midpoint.

2.3. Measures

In the present paper, the sociodemographic variables are gender, age, career level, upbringing environment, and income. The remaining questions were selected and categorized into three thematic groups: 1) ethical, consisting of 7 questions; 2) bioethical, 5 questions, and 3) legal, 5 questions. All responses are categorical.
The survey comprised questions related to the ethical, bioethical, and legal dimensions of animal welfare that were utilized in this study. Participants were asked various questions regarding animal use. Additionally, inquiries were made about suffering, sentience, and cruelty toward nonhuman animals, addressing the ethical, bioethical, and legal dimensions of animal welfare that extend beyond livestock production. Participants responded to items on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very good) to 5 (very bad). Subsequently, responses were recoded into good or bad and yes or no categories.

2.4. Perceived Animal Ethics, Bioethics and Legal Issues

Agronomy engineering students were surveyed on topics related to animal ethics to understand perceptions based on sociodemographic variables such as gender, age, education level, geographic origin, and income. The questions are as follows:
  • "In general, how do you believe farm animals are treated?"
  • "Provide an assessment of how you believe the following farm animals are generally treated."
  • "Would you be willing to pay more for a product if it helped improve animal welfare?"
  • "Do you believe humans should be respectful towards animals?"
  • "Do you consider animal cruelty to be a condemnable behavior?"
  • "Do you consider it ethical and responsible for social movements to aim at using 'humane' methods to exterminate crop pests?"
  • "How do you assess the act of humans killing other animals for food?"
The questions related to life sciences and the interaction animal sciences and environment that could form the bioethical foundations of agronomy students were:
  • “How would you rate your level of concern for animal welfare?”
  • “Regarding animal suffering, do you believe some animals are more valuable than others?”
  • “How do you assess nonhuman animals preying on each other?”
  • “How do you assess animal research, specifically using them as models in trials to test new drugs for humans?”
  • “How do you feel about the idea of witnessing an animal suffer?”
The questions related to legal and regulatory aspects that could contribute to developing the argumentation on animal welfare within the framework of bio-law among agronomy students were as follows:
  • “Do you find it ethical to practice "hunting" sports while complying with established regulations for it? “
  • “Do you consider that animals subjected to hunting (even when each country's regulations are followed) limit their well-being?”
  • “If you admit that animals should not suffer, would you be willing to use animals in scientific experimentation as models for the study of human diseases?”
  • "If you acknowledge that animals are not mere objects, do you believe that animals should be granted 'rights'?"
  • “Do you consider granting rights as the only way to protect the interests of animals?”

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed in R version 4.3.1 [44] and RStudio version 2022.7.1.554 [45], using packages ggplot2 [46] for figures and gtsummary [47] for tables. The Chi-square test of independence was used to assess the association between the participants' characteristics and each question within the thematic groups. In cases where the expected frequency was less than 5, the Fisher's exact test was employed, and statistical significance was determined as a p value below 0.05. When skewness was present, response categories were collapsed to the subsequent level.

2.4.1. Quantitative Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using R software (v4.2.2, R Core Team, 2021), and summary statistics were computed for all variables of interest. Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were used to analyze all Likert scale questions to determine if students' responses differed among them. For each question, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were conducted with the five Likert scale categories included (i.e., 1 = High; 2 = Medium or Low) or (1 = Yes, 2 = No, 3 = I am indifferent).
Based on the results of the Wilcoxon signed-rank test, only questions that were significantly different from each other (p < 0.05) were further analyzed. Ordinal logistic regression assuming proportional odds was employed to assess the relationship between Likert responses and respondents' demographic factors. Demographic factors considered for regression analysis were gender (male or female), age in years (18-20; 21-23; 24-26; over 26), progression level in the agronomy curriculum (less than 25%; between 25-50%; between 51-75%; over 75%), hometown type (rural or urban), agricultural background (originating from a farm or not), and experience with animals (yes or no). Model selection was performed through backward elimination based on parameter significance (P ≤ 0.05), retaining only significant parameters in the final models.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

A total of N = 150 individuals were contacted, of which only 144 met the inclusion criteria (above 18 years old) and were entered in the analysis, including n = 89 men and n = 55 women (Table 1). The age distribution for each gender is shown in plot in Figure 1. As can be observed data is well balanced for gender, there's a notable difference of around 10% between the 18 to 20 age group and a 38% difference among those over 26 years old, comprising 13 men and 2 women. The age distribution within college level is shown in the plot in Figure 2. As expected, concerning the education level, there's a clear trend: as age increases, there's a higher percentage of individuals with advanced levels in their education.

3.2. Heat Maps Multivariate Data

3.2.1. Perceived Animal Ethics

The agronomy engineering students responded to questions regarding animal ethics to understand their perceptions based on sociodemographic variables such as gender, age, education level, upbringing environment, and income. The following heat map summarizes the association between variables and questions, displaying the p-values of the hypothesis test. It can be concluded that gender and willingness to pay more for a product to improve animal welfare ("Would you be willing to pay more for a product if it helped improve animal welfare?") are not independent, similarly, gender and considering it ethical and responsible for social movements to use "humane" methods to exterminate pests ("Do you consider it ethical and responsible for social movements to aim at using 'humane' methods to exterminate crop pests?"), as well as the assessment of whether it is ethical for humans to kill animals for food ("How do you assess the act of humans’ animals killing other nonhuman animals for food?"), are not independent. Likewise, the level of education and considering it ethical and responsible for social movements to use "humane" methods to exterminate crop pests and the assessment of whether it is ethical for humans’ animals to kill others nonhuman animals for food are not independent. In other relationships, there was not enough statistical evidence found to consider them non-independent (Figure 3).
The statistical associations between animal ethics perception and demographic factors (gender and upbringing environment) among agronomy students about the question "Provide an assessment of how you believe the following farm animals are generally treated" are shown in Table 2. For turkeys, there's a statistically significant difference in perception between males and females (p = 0.016), indicating varied viewpoints based on gender. The perception of beef cattle also significantly differs between genders (p = 0.050), showing distinct opinions among male and female students.
When the upbringing environment was analyzed, perceptions of sheep significantly vary between rural and urban areas (p = 0.050), suggesting differing viewpoints based on location. Likewise, perceptions of rabbits, beef cattle, and dairy cattle also showcase notable differences between rural and urban settings. These findings indicate that both gender and upbringing environment influence the way agronomy students perceive various animals, with certain species' perceptions being more significantly impacted by these demographic factors (Table 2). Other species were also included in the questionnaire, but the answers did not provide significant data, namely: buffalo, goat, horse, sheep, rabbit, chicken, duck and fish.
Concerning to the question "Would you be willing to pay more for a product if it helped improve animal welfare?" There exist among students the willingness to pay more for a product to improve animal welfare based on gender differences. The data demonstrates a substantial gender-based discrepancy (p < 0.001). Among men, 65% are inclined to consider paying more, with 49% expressing a definitive 'Yes' response. In contrast, women show a different trend, with 51% showing readiness to pay more, significantly higher than the 4.3% who oppose the idea. This gender-driven disparity highlights contrasting attitudes toward investing in animal welfare through higher product prices.
The ethical issues regarding the use of 'humane' methods in crop pest extermination across different demographics, demonstrates a statistically significant difference (p = 0.004) where 82% of males oppose 'humane' pest control methods compared to 18% of females who oppose it. Moreover, there were a variation (p < 0.001) across education brackets. Participants with higher education levels (> 75%) exhibit less opposition (39%) compared to those with lower education levels (< 25%) at 25%. Regarding the perceptions of humans killing animals for food, there was gender-based contrast (p < 0.001), where 71% of females view it negatively ('Bad') compared to 29% of males. There was minor disparity (p = 0.070) across age groups. Younger participants (18-20) demonstrate higher negativity (62%) towards this act compared to older groups. Those students with lower education levels (< 25%) are more likely (57%) to perceive it negatively compared to those with higher education (> 75%) at 4.8%. These findings indicate significant disparities in ethical considerations regarding crop pest control methods and perceptions of animal humans killing nonhuman animals for food across different demographic segments (Table 3).

3.2.2. Perceived Bioethics about Animal Welfare

In the bioethics component, it may be concluded that each of the questions made in the bioethics component, are not independent regarding gender. Similarly, there exists a relationship between age and the assessment of animal research, specifically their use as models in trials for testing new drugs for humans (“How do you assess animal research, specifically using them as models in trials to test new drugs for humans?”); and the education level concerning the consideration that some animals are worth more than others (“Regarding animal suffering, do you believe some animals are more valuable than others?”), the assessment of nonhuman animals preying on each other (“How do you assess nonhuman animals preying on each other?”), and the evaluation of animal research, specifically their use as models in trials for testing new drugs for humans (“How do you assess animal research, specifically using them as models in trials to test new drugs for humans?”). In other relationships, there wasn't sufficient statistical evidence to consider them as non-independent (Figure 4).
When analyzed varying perspectives on animal welfare, bioethical considerations, and assessments regarding animal behaviors and research, highlighting gender and education level influences. regarding animal welfare levels, there's a significant gender discrepancy (p < 0.001), with 83% of males rating their concern as low compared to 47% of females. Similarly, views on animal value perception reveal gender disparities (p < 0.001) and educational differences (p = 0.008). Assessments of animal behaviors show gender distinctions (p = 0.034) and educational variations (p = 0.044). Evaluating animal research as models for drug trials displays both gender (p = 0.002) and educational (p = 0.002) disparities. Additionally, upbringing environment differences were noted, albeit marginally significant (p = 0.070), (Table 4).
When analyzing the responses related to the question “How do you feel about the idea of witnessing an animal suffer?”, all indifferent respondents were men, while most females found it unbearable or unpleasant yet tolerable. This disparity reveals distinct gender-related perceptions regarding witnessing animal suffering, emphasizing a marked contrast in emotional reactions between male and female respondents (Table 5). No participant responded with "I enjoy it."

3.3.2. Perceived Legal and Normative Criteria Regarding Animal Welfare

The gender and the bioethical perception of practicing "hunting" sports within established regulations (“Do you find it ethical to practice "hunting" sports while complying with established regulations for it? “), the willingness to use animals in scientific experimentation as models for the study of human diseases (“If you admit that animals should not suffer, would you be willing to use animals in scientific experimentation as models for the study of human diseases?”), considering that animals should be granted "rights" ("If you acknowledge that animals are not mere objects, do you believe that animals should be granted 'rights'?"), and believing that granting rights is the only way to protect animal interests (“Do you consider granting rights as the only way to protect the interests of animals?”) are not independent (p < 0.05). Similarly, age and considering that hunted animals (even when each country's regulations are followed) limit their well-being (“Do you consider that animals subjected to hunting (even when each country's regulations are followed) limit their well-being?”) are not independent (p < 0.05). The education level was also not independent (p < 0.05) from the willingness to use animals in scientific experimentation as models for the study of human diseases (“If you admit that animals should not suffer, would you be willing to use animals in scientific experimentation as models for the study of human diseases?”) and considering that animals should be granted "rights" ("If you acknowledge that animals are not mere objects, do you believe that animals should be granted 'rights'?"); likewise, the upbringing environment with considering that animals should be granted "rights" ("If you acknowledge that animals are not mere objects, do you believe that animals should be granted 'rights'?"). In other relationships, there wasn't sufficient statistical evidence to consider them non-independent (Figure 5).
Concerning to the question about the ethical perception regarding practicing "hunting" sports in adherence to established regulations, the analysis highlights a statistically significant relationship (p < 0.05) between gender and responses (yes, no, indifferent to me), showing distinctions in viewpoints between men and women. Specifically, 65% of indifferent respondents were men, 52% of those who were against hunting were male, and 82% of respondents in favor of such sports were also men.
When analyzing activities regulated by legislation such as hunting, recreational fishing, or crop pest control, differences were observed based on gender for each activity (p < 0.05). In general, there's a noticeable discrepancy between males and females in their assessments. For instance, in hunting a higher percentage of males rated it as "Good" compared to females. Similarly, in recreational fishing and pest control in crops, males tended to rate these activities higher compared to females (Table 6).
Regarding pest control in crops based on different education levels at college, there were significant differences observed among the groups (p < 0.001). The majority rated it positively across all categories. However, within the groups that assessed it as "Regular," a notable discrepancy was noticed, with a higher percentage of respondents in the "51-75%" range completion of the curriculum in agronomy degree compared to the other groups.
In examining perceptions about animal well-being in hunting scenarios, there was an effect (p < 0.05) of gender and age revealed. Men tended to perceive hunting as less limiting to animal well-being compared to women. Younger age groups were more likely to consider hunting as affecting animal well-being. Regarding the use of animals in scientific research, there was an effect (p < 0.05) of gender and education level exhibited. Men were more open about the idea of using animals in research compared to women, and those with lower education levels were more inclined to support animal experimentation. Moreover, there was an effect of gender, education level, and upbringing environment on belief in granting animals 'rights' displayed. Men were less likely than women to support granting animal’s rights, while higher education levels and urban settings showed more favorable inclinations toward animal rights. The necessity of granting rights for animal protection indicated gender disparities, with men less inclined to view rights as the sole safeguard for animal interests compared to women (Table 7).

4. Discussion

Animal welfare is currently a fundamental component of animal production systems [3,35,48] because it directly impacts the quality of meat, milk, eggs, wool, leather, and all derivatives thereof industries. Typically, agronomical engineering schools in Costa Rica do not offer courses on animal welfare in their curricula; instead, they focus on livestock production in theoretical and practical courses on production systems. The results of this study indicate that most undergraduate agronomy students stated that animal welfare was important within livestock production systems. This assessment aligns with the widespread social perception of how animals should be treated, where animal welfare involves reducing suffering in different livestock production systems [49]. The notion that causing animals to suffer is not acceptable is an ethical judgment that the science of animal welfare can utilize to promote the necessity for improving welfare to benefit the groups associated with the livestock industry and all recognized forms of human-animal relationships [50,51].
The study results highlight differences in animal welfare perceptions among animal categories. The variability in students' perceptions regarding the treatment of different animal species may be explained by the level of interaction individuals have with specific species due to their upbringing or environment. Various studies have demonstrated that experience in an agricultural or rural context and being male diminish perceptions of the importance of animal welfare [52,53,54]. However, several socio-demographic factors can influence individual perceptions of animal welfare. This underscores a somewhat variable sense of responsibility toward certain animal species, especially regarding wildlife or domestic species used in food production [55,56,57].
Understanding what perspectives could underlie perceptions and assessments on livestock non-human animals is relevant aimed at proposing new teaching activities, materials, and research [58]. We find two general positions currently, with different variants: the first one claims that human beings are different from the other species (we may call it anthropologism instead of anthropocentrism); there are some variants of this position [59,60]. Here we adopt, without any further analysis, that there exist biological species, as claimed, for example, by biological taxonomists [61].
The second position considers that human being is part of the order of nature [62]. It is claimed that we share with other species most aspects including biological mechanisms, ingredients of life, and even cognitive structures [63]. It does not claim that capabilities are the same along the natural order, but that different species share them with different degrees and differences [64].
When our results about perceptions were analyzed, most respondents held positive attitudes toward improving animal welfare [65,66]. These perceptions were linked to key concepts: animal suffering, animal research, willingness to pay for products with guaranteed animal welfare, using 'humane' methods for crop pest control, and the moral conception of harvesting animals for meat consumption and animal-origin products. It is ethically desirable to address moral arbitrariness where they were not previously found, due to considering them as natural variation [67]. If we acknowledge that animal suffering poses an ethical issue, we must investigate when and how animals suffer to address the problem based on evidence [14,16]. Here, we must employ the scientific method to conduct experiments associating the results with our current understanding in fields such as zoology, animal husbandry, genetics, evolutionary biology, and all sciences focused on living organisms. Livestock production is often criticized for certain animal management practices, such as confinement systems and procedures like castration or dehorning are controversial [68,69]. This awareness is evident among students and reflects in their ethical and bioethical perceptions regarding welfare and nonhuman animal suffering. However, this tends to increase as they progress through the agronomy curriculum. Regarding nonhuman animal suffering, it cannot be claimed that avoiding suffering makes a good person due to the recognition of their right “not to suffer”, because these are different categories [13], not extended on this article.
Attitudes toward animal welfare differed based on sociodemographic characteristics and the level of concern. Women, younger participants, and those with more education showed higher levels of concern and held more negative attitudes toward animal abuse and suffering. They were also more likely to pay for animal-origin products with welfare seals (WTP - willing-to-pay) and welfare-friendly products (WFP), indicating awareness of responsibly consuming animal-origin products and a potential market conscious of the value animals provide as protein sources in human diets [41,70]. Women held more negative attitudes regarding animal research involving their suffering and expressed greater concerns for animal welfare. Regarding gender, some authors highlight the role of women and their solidarity-driven roles in society [71], which could explain their more empathetic perceptions about welfare concerns compared to men. Students entering agronomy programs might have increased access to information about production systems through social media, influencing their level of concern for animal treatment. However, this study demonstrates that perceptions of agricultural production systems tend to change more positively as students’ progress through the curriculum. This change could be associated with advanced-level courses on animal production, sensibilizing their perception objectively towards the treatment of animals in different livestock species, as well as caring more for the wellbeing of the species they will continue to interact with in livestock production. Further supporting the role of social media in agricultural education, a notable example is a study where Instagram was used effectively to educate and engage the public about dairy cow nutrition and welfare. This initiative successfully communicated scientific knowledge to a wider audience, creating a community engaged in the welfare of dairy cows. Such innovative approaches in digital outreach highlight the evolving landscape of agricultural education and the growing importance of accessible and interactive platforms for public engagement [72].
Analyzing perceptions of animal welfare based on sociodemographic variables could be explained through the human-animal bond. An interdisciplinary approach to addressing this bond could generate new knowledge related to the bioethics of animal welfare, where ethical issues about our relationship with animals [6,10] interact with knowledge of animal biology [73], the latter being something that agronomy students learn in the early years of their program. The bioethical principle of autonomy recognizes the telos, i.e., the fundamental biological and psychological essence of any animal based on Aristotle’s principles [9]. According to this doctrine, the purposes of humans and nonhuman animals do not need coincide, the decision to ethically treat animals is a moral stance specific to humans, therefore, it is not a biological purpose but an ethical decision [74]. In accordance with the ethics of animal husbandry, nonhuman animals currently serve human rational purposes through breeding and good livestock practices.
Sociodemographic variables such as gender, students’ upbringing environment, and their progress within the curriculum directly influenced ethical and bioethical perceptions regarding the human-animal bond and animal welfare. In this study, most students (85%) came from rural backgrounds compared to 15% from urban areas. This differs from data reported by some authors that underscore a current trend regarding the backgrounds of students studying agronomical sciences who are not from rural environments [75,76]. The correlation between sociodemographic variables and the degree of ethical concern for animal welfare might stem from the animal interest movement that gained momentum after the publication of the book ‘Animal Liberation’[77], marking a turning point in civil society, increasingly concerned about how animals were treated [78,79,80,81] or treated on farms or livestock production facilities [23,82].
The equal consideration of interests introduced by Peter Singer in 1975 (Animal Liberation) is an interesting qualification of non-human animals. He introduces it as an ethical principle and not as a description of facts. What this does is to prescribe the correct way of understanding and of behavior that we should adapt in connection with other living organisms [83].
The educational level was linked to greater concern for welfare, with those who had studied longer expressing more affinity and concern for it. Those with more technical knowledge also reported higher concerns, similar to those more familiar, for instance, those who previously lived in rural areas, owned farms, or worked in livestock activities. This explains how prior knowledge of activities can sensitize attitudes and perceptions, along with knowledge transmitted through formal education, to avoid misconceptions about animal production that could potentially affect attitudes.
Some studies have analyzed sociodemographic characteristics and their relation to perceptions and attitudes about animal welfare and haven’t reported significant differences attributable to socioeconomic factors, such as in the present study with family income level [84,85]. Student preferences indicate they would pay a differentiated price to improve welfare. Although the effect was significant when analyzing gender, this could provide market niches and should be analyzed in producer associations to assess the costs associated with animal welfare and the benefits of investments made following ethical criteria. Some studies suggest the need for interdisciplinary work because it’s challenging to generate a global concept of welfare that satisfies all involved parties [86]. The best way to build a just multispecies society may not be known in the present moment, but accepting this responsibility will impact positively to find it out [87].
Negative perceptions and concerns associated with animal welfare were confined to a zoocentric or anthropocentric view. Perceptions about welfare from an animal perspective are based on animals’ ability to express emotions and feel pain [10], ergo the observable behavior that would elicit an uncomfortability response in humans. In this regard, the perception that animals may have a value greater than utilitarian value for humans becomes relevant. However, the utilitarian argument for animal welfare lies in the fact that subsequent animal products will be better for the quality and safety of humans consuming them. Conversely, some people's concerns about animal welfare were motivated by their own well-being, seemingly avoiding anthropomorphism. This was reflected in educational levels; in the initial years of the program, there was a negative perception of meat consumption, and as students progressed in their university careers, this perception significantly shifted positively toward meat consumption. Understanding livestock production systems might sensitize this perception change. However, the criterion that animals are treated inadequately or even cruelly remained constant. Some authors have described consuming animal meat as a human right [88]. The centric view can also offer another form of dissonance, seeing animals as objects rather than sentient beings [60,89]. This constitutes a scientific problem and should be addressed as such, without our opinions on animal rights or obligations intervening or interacting. Welfare legislation will consider these scientific issues and will likely affect production systems because consumers are concerned about the welfare of farm animals, leading to social pressure that will affect new legislation. Certain production systems have numbered days, such as foie gras, where force-feeding induces fatty liver in ducks and geese to obtain the "delicacy" product, an act that could be labeled barbaric [90].
Although most students didn't have negative perceptions of regulated activities like hunting to control species overpopulation, recreational fishing, and pest control, this could be explained by their interaction within agronomy programs involving courses related to agricultural crops, where they engage in supervised practices for biological and/or chemical pest control in crops [91,92]. Animals related to fishing and hunting, not being livestock species, might denote a gradient in the value attributed to species closer to humans due to the human-animal bond [13], for example, mammals, and within these, the establishment of scales for assessing suffering, with primates at the forefront, followed by pets and then livestock animals.
Concerning animal rights, experimentation with them, and ways to protect animal interests, these considerations were not independent of gender, educational level, or students' upbringing environments. Students' assessments regarding bio-rights-related questions suggest a positive stance regarding granting rights to animals, with 70% of respondents indicating that rights should be granted. Granting rights to animals is a complex issue studied in various works [60,87,93,94,95]. However, granting rights should go hand in hand with acquiring duties [96,97], and the predominant question is, can rights be granted without assuming duties? From a broader perspective, production animals exist under the biopower exerted upon them by the human species, an act defined by Foucault [98,99], as the subjugation and disposal of their bodies and population control. Therefore, discussions about animal welfare will be directed toward recognizing this power relationship and seeking to reduce mistreatment linked to domination rather than aiming for a 'liberation' from the breeding and production process for harvesting and generating animal-origin foods. Biopower also functions as a technology of the humanist ideological system to differentiate people from the rest of the animals, as our rationality both dominates and excludes them from the social contract. This implies that animals aren't accountable for their behaviors within the system that surveils and punishes, yet they also don't possess rights or sovereignty over their bodies [100]. It's incongruous to grant rights to production animals because they exist to be 'utilized' by the human species, and they could never emancipate themselves from this power relationship. Even attempting to grant them rights is another act of domination through rationality and social agreement systems. Instead, it's pertinent of the dominant species, to ensure their welfare from the inherent responsibility and the moral duty to prevent harm and, furthermore, from a zootechnical commitment to responsible animal production that respects animals and ensures productive quality.

5. Conclusions

Animal welfare is a topic of increasing relevance in society, as there is growing concern about responsible consumption habits regarding animal-origin products. This concern even impacts legislation regarding nonhuman animal use. Consequently, public perceptions about animal welfare are complex and often understudied within various professional groups associated with animals. Overall, the findings of this study suggest that most respondents hold positive ethical assessments toward animal welfare and negative perceptions toward animal suffering. However, differences emerged when consulting about pest control among species. It has been demonstrated that perceptions of animal welfare vary among species, genders, levels of university education, and places of origin. This highlights the importance of understanding agronomy students' perceptions of animal welfare as input for designing, improving, and updating educational programs with formal components on animal welfare. An integrated education on animal welfare could equip students with multidisciplinary knowledge supporting the science of animal welfare to address current challenges and dilemmas concerning animal welfare today.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at the website of this paper posted on Preprints.org. Figure S1: title; Table S1: title; Video S1: title.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.V., J.A.G., E.R.S.R. and F.S.; methodology, J.A.G, F.S. and A.V.; software, A.V.; validation, J.A.G., F.S., and A.V.; formal analysis, A.V.; investigation, J.A.G., F.S., and A.V.; resources, A.V.; data curation, A.V.; writing—original draft preparation, J.A.G. and A.V.; writing—review and editing, S.M., J.A.G., E.R.S.R., C.V., F.S., R.G., and A.V.; visualization, S.M., E.R.S.R., C.V., R.G., and A.V.; supervision, A.V.; project administration, A.V.; funding acquisition, A.V. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Costa Rica Institute of Technology Fellowships Department); and Vice-Chancellor’s Office of Research and Extension Project 5402-2151-1019). The funders had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the approval of the ethical principles of the Committee of “Centro de Investigación y Desarrollo de la Agricultura Sostenible para el Trópico Húmedo” at the Costa Rica Institute of Technology (CIDASTH-ITCR) according to Section 010/2023, article 7.0, DAGSC-093-2023, approval date is 5 May 2023.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available within the article.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the Costa Rica Institute of Technology for financing this study. AV are grateful to the master’s program in Bioethics of Philosophy and Letters School at National University of Costa Rica.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no competing interest.

References

  1. Harrison, R. Animal Machines; CABI, 1964.
  2. Fraser, D. Understanding Animal Welfare. Acta Veterinaria Scandinavica 2008, 50, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Hewson, C.J. What Is Animal Welfare? Common Definitions and Their Practical Consequences. The Canadian Veterinary Journal 2003, 44, 496. [Google Scholar]
  4. Schmidt, K. Concepts of Animal Welfare in Relation to Positions in Animal Ethics. Acta Biotheor 2011, 59, 153–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Horgan, R.; Gavinelli, A. The Expanding Role of Animal Welfare within EU Legislation and Beyond. Livest Sci 2006, 103, 303–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ferrin, A. Nonhuman Animals Are Morally Responsible. Am Philos Q 2019, 56, 135–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Fraser, D. Animal Ethics and Animal Welfare Science: Bridging the Two Cultures. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1999, 65, 171–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Fraser, D. A “Practical” Ethic for Animals. J Agric Environ Ethics 2012, 25, 721–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Grumett, D. Aristotle’s Ethics and Farm Animal Welfare. J Agric Environ Ethics 2019, 32, 321–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Leyton, F. Bioética Frente a Los Derechos Animales: Tensión En Las Fronteras de La Filosofía Moral. Tesis Doctoral, Universitat de Barcelona: Barcelona, España, 2014.
  11. Garner, R. Toward a Theory of Justice for Animals. Journal of Animal Ethics 2012, 2, 98–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Scanes, C.G.; Toukhsati, S.R. Animals and Human Society-. Animals and Human Society 2017, 1–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Blasco, A. Ética y Bienestar Animal; 1er ed.; Ediciones Akal, S.A., 2011; ISBN 978-84-460-2321-0.
  14. Dawkins, M.S. The Science of Animal Suffering. Ethology 2008, 114, 937–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Hickman, D.L.; Johnson, J.; Vemulapalli, T.H.; Crisler, J.R.; Shepherd, R. Commonly Used Animal Models. Principles of Animal Research for Graduate and Undergraduate Students 2017, 117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Olsson, I.A.S.; J Nicol, C.; Niemi, S.M.; Sandøe, P. From Unpleasant to Unbearable—Why and How to Implement an Upper Limit to Pain and Other Forms of Suffering in Research with Animals. ILAR J 2019, 60, 404–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. McManus, P. Animal-Based Entertainment Industries, Animal Death and Social Licence to Operate (SLO): An Analysis of ‘The Final Race’ and the 2019 Melbourne Cup. [CrossRef]
  18. Sapontzis, S.F. Morals, Reason, and Animals.; Temple Univ. Press: Philadelphia, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  19. Stoddart, H. Animals in Sport and Entertainment. Ref Libr 2004, 41, 25–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Rodriguez Ferrere, M.B. Animal Welfare Underenforcement as a Rule of Law Problem. Animals (Basel) 2022, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N.J.; Littlewood, K.E.; McLean, A.N.; McGreevy, P.D.; Jones, B.; Wilkins, C. The 2020 Five Domains Model: Including Human–Animal Interactions in Assessments of Animal Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 1870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Kiley-Worthington, M. Ecological, Ethological, and Ethically Sound Environments for Animals: Toward Symbiosis. J Agric Ethics 1989, 2, 323–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Mejdell, C.M. The Role of Councils on Animal Ethics in Assessing Acceptable Welfare Standards in Agriculture. Livest Sci 2006, 103, 292–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. von Essen, E.; Lindsjö, J.; Berg, C. Instagranimal: Animal Welfare and Animal Ethics Challenges of Animal-based Tourism. Animals 2020, 10, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Broom, D.M. The Scientific Assessment of Animal Welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci 1988, 20, 5–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Broom, D.M. Animal Welfare: Concepts and Measurement. J Anim Sci 1991, 69, 4167–4175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Broom, D.M.; Johnson, K.G. Stress and Animal Welfare; Chapman and Hall: London, UK, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  28. Broom, D.M.; Fraser, A.F. Domestic Animal Behaviour and Welfare; Fourth.; CABI: Wallingford, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lazzaroni, M.; Range, F.; Backes, J.; Portele, K.; Scheck, K.; Marshall-Pescini, S. The Effect of Domestication and Experience on the Social Interaction of Dogs and Wolves With a Human Companion. Front Psychol 2020, 11, 508171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Rault, J.L.; Waiblinger, S.; Boivin, X.; Hemsworth, P. The Power of a Positive Human–Animal Relationship for Animal Welfare. Front Vet Sci 2020, 7, 590867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Tallet, C.; Veissier, I.; Boivin, X. Human Contact and Feeding as Rewards for the Lamb’s Affinity to Their Stockperson. Appl Anim Behav Sci 2005, 94, 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Waiblinger, S. Agricultural Animals. In Anthrozoology: Human–Animal Interactions in Domesticated and Wild Animals.; Hosey, G., Melfi, V., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, 2019; pp. 32–58. [Google Scholar]
  33. Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N.J. Extending the ‘Five Domains’ Model for Animal Welfare Assessment to Incorporate Positive Welfare States. Animal Welfare 2015, 24, 241–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Webster, J. Animal Welfare: Freedoms, Dominions and “A Life Worth Living. ” Animals 2016, Vol. 6, Page 35 2016, 6, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Fraser, D. Assessing Animal Welfare at the Farm and Group Level: The Interplay of Science and Values. Animal Welfare 2003, 12, 433–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Martos, E.C. El Derecho de Los Animales En España: Regulación Normativa, Problemática y Solución Constitucional. CEFLegal. Revista práctica de derecho 2020, 239, 123–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bentham, J. Introduction to the Principles of Moral and Legislation; Oxford University PresS: Oxford, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  38. Killoren, D.; Streiffer, R. Utilitarianism about Animals and the Moral Significance of Use. Philos Stud 2020, 177, 1043–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lindsay, R.A. ANIMALS, MORAL STATUS, AND THE OBJECTIVES OF MORALITY. Think 2017, 16, 33–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mill, J.S. The Project Gutenberg EBook of Utilitarianism Available online:. Available online: https://www.gutenberg.org/files/11224/11224-h/11224-h.htm (accessed on 26 October 2021).
  41. Clark, B.; Stewart, G.B.; Panzone, L.A.; Kyriazakis, I.; Frewer, L.J. A Systematic Review of Public Attitudes, Perceptions and Behaviours Towards Production Diseases Associated with Farm Animal Welfare. J Agric Environ Ethics 2016, 29, 455–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Molony, V.; Kent, J.E. Assessment of Acute Pain in Farm Animals Using Behavioral and Physiological Measurements. J Anim Sci 1997, 75, 266–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Bundy, J.M.; Sterle, J.A.; Johnson, A.K.; Krahn, G.T. The Impact of an Introductory Animal Handling Course on Undergraduate Students Who Lack Previous Livestock Handling Experience. J Anim Sci 2019, 97, 3588–3595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. R Core Team Available online:. Available online: https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed on 12 December 2023).
  45. RStudio Team Available online:. Available online: https://posit.co/download/rstudio-desktop/ (accessed on 12 December 2023).
  46. Wickham, H. Ggpolt2 Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Use R! series 2016, 211. [Google Scholar]
  47. Sjoberg, D.D.; Whiting, K.; Curry, M.; Lavery, J.A.; Larmarange, J. Reproducible Summary Tables with the Gtsummary Package. R Journal 2021, 13, 570–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Verbeke, W. Stakeholder, Citizen and Consumer Interests in Farm Animal Welfare. Animal Welfare 2009, 18, 325–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Norwood, F.B.; Lusk, J.L. The Farm Animal Welfare Debate. Choices 2009, 24. [Google Scholar]
  50. Alonso, M.E.; González-Montaña, J.R.; Lomillos, J.M. Consumers’ Concerns and Perceptions of Farm Animal Welfare. Animals 2020, 10, 385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Nurse, A. Beyond the Property Debate: Animal Welfare as a Public Good. Contemporary Justice Review 2016, 19, 174–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Bradley, A.; Mennie, N.; Bibby, P.A.; Cassaday, H.J. Some Animals Are More Equal than Others: Validation of a New Scale to Measure How Attitudes to Animals Depend on Species and Human Purpose of Use. PLoS One 2020, 15, e0227948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hazel, S.J.; Signal, T.D.; Taylor, N. Can Teaching Veterinary and Animal-Science Students about Animal Welfare Affect Their Attitude toward Animals and Human-Related Empathy? J Vet Med Educ 2011, 38, 74–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Mariti, C.; Pirrone, F.; Albertini, M.; Gazzano, A.; Diverio, S. Familiarity and Interest in Working with Livestock Decreases the Odds of Having Positive Attitudes towards Non-Human Animals and Their Welfare among Veterinary Students in Italy. Animals 2018, 8, 150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Green, T.C.; Mellor, D.J. Extending Ideas about Animal Welfare Assessment to Include ‘Quality of Life’ and Related Concepts. N Z Vet J 2011, 59, 263–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Mellor, D.J. Operational Details of the Five Domains Model and Its Key Applications to the Assessment and Management of Animal Welfare. Animals 2017, 7, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Mellor, D.J.; Stafford, K.J. Integrating Practical, Regulatory and Ethical Strategies for Enhancing Farm Animal Welfare. Aust Vet J 2001, 79, 762–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Sinclair, M.; Lee, N.Y.P.; Hötzel, M.J.; de Luna, M.C.T.; Sharma, A.; Idris, M.; Derkley, T.; Li, C.; Islam, M.A.; Iyasere, O.S.; et al. International Perceptions of Animals and the Importance of Their Welfare. Frontiers in Animal Science 2022, 3, 960379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Bradshaw, J.W.S.; Casey, R.A. Anthropomorphism and Anthropocentrism as Influences in the Quality of Life of Companion Animals. Animal Welfare 2007, 16, 149–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Pulina, G. Anthropocentrism, Natural Harmony, Sentience and Animal Rights: Are We Allowed to Use Animals for Our Own Purposes? Animals 2023, 13, 1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Aldhebiani, A.Y. Species Concept and Speciation. Saudi J Biol Sci 2018, 25, 437–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Ramsey, G. Human Nature. Elements in the Philosophy of Biology 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hernández-Avilez, E.G.; Ruiz-Gutiérrez, R. From One Darwin to Another: Charles Darwin’s Annotations to Erasmus Darwin’s ‘The Temple of Nature. ’ Humanities and Social Sciences Communications 2023, 10, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Plotkin, H. Human Nature, Cultural Diversity and Evolutionary Theory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 2011, 366, 454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Mijares, S.; Sullivan, P.; Cramer, C.; Román-Muñiz, N.; Edwards-Callaway, L. Perceptions of Animal Welfare and Animal Welfare Curricula Offered for Undergraduate and Graduate Students in Animal Science Departments in the United States. Transl Anim Sci 2021, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Sullivan, P.; Mijares, S.; Davis, M.; Oselinsky, K.; Cramer, C.; Román-Muñiz, N.; Stallones, L.; Edwards-Callaway, L. A Nationwide Survey of Animal Science Students’ Perceptions of Animal Welfare across Different Animal Categories at Institutions in the United States. Animals 2022, 12, 2294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Williston, B. The Anthropocene Project; Oxford University Press: Oxford, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  68. Weary, D.M.; Ventura, B.A.; Von Keyserlingk, M.A.G. Societal Views and Animal Welfare Science: Understanding Why the Modified Cage May Fail and Other Stories. Animal 2016, 10, 309–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Widmar, N.O.; Morgan, C.J.; Wolf, C.A.; Yeager, E.A.; Dominick, S.R.; Croney, C.C.; Widmar, N.O.; Morgan, C.J.; Wolf, C.A.; Yeager, E.A.; et al. US Resident Perceptions of Dairy Cattle Management Practices. Agricultural Sciences 2017, 8, 645–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Clark, B.; Stewart, G.B.; Panzone, L.A.; Frewer, L.J.; Frewer, L. A Protocol for a Systematic Review into Consumers’ Attitudes, Beliefs and Perceived Ethical Obligations towards Farm Animal Welfare. 2014. [CrossRef]
  71. Kendall, H.A.; Lobao, L.M.; Sharp, J.S. Public Concern with Animal Well-Being: Place, Social Structural Location, and Individual Experience*. Rural Sociol 2006, 71, 399–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Muca, E.; Buonaiuto, G.; Lamanna, M.; Silvestrelli, S.; Ghiaccio, F.; Federiconi, A.; De Matos Vettori, J.; Colleluori, R.; Fusaro, I.; Raspa, F.; et al. Reaching a Wider Audience: Instagram’s Role in Dairy Cow Nutrition Education and Engagement. Animals 2023, 13, 3503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Carenzi, C.; Verga, M. Animal Welfare: Review of the Scientific Concept and Definition. 2009, 8, 21–30. [CrossRef]
  74. Hauskeller, M. Telos: The Revival of an Aristotelian Concept in Present Day Ethics. 2006, 48, 62–75. [CrossRef]
  75. Parrish, J.J.; Smith, M.F.; Geisert, R.D.; Davis, D.L.; Wilson, M.E.; Flowers, W.L. How to Communicate with Undergraduate Students That Lack an Animal Science or Agricultural Background. Animal Frontiers 2015, 5, 54–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Reiling, B.A.; Marshall, T.T.; Brendemuhl, J.H.; McQuagge, J.A.; Umphrey, J.E. Experiential Learning in the Animal Sciences: Development of a Multispecies Large-Animal Management and Production Practicum. J Anim Sci 2003, 81, 3202–3210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Singer, P. Animal Liberation; Avon Books: New York, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  78. Akhtar, A. The Flaws and Human Harms of Animal Experimentation. Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics 2015, 24, 407–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Recuerda, P.; Moyano, R.; Castro, F. Bienestar Animal: Experimentación, Producción, Compañía y Zoológicos. In Proceedings of the Libro de resúmenes II curso sobre Bienestar Animal.; Córdoba, España; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  80. Webster, J. Ethical and Animal Welfare Considerations in Relation to Species Selection for Animal Experimentation. Animals (Basel) 2014, 4, 729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Webster, A. Farm Animal Welfare: The Five Freedoms and the Free Market. Vet J 2001, 161, 229–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Brambell, F. Report of the Technical Committee to Enquire into the Welfare of Animals Kept under Intensive Livestock Husbandry Systems; Her Majesty’s Stationery Office: London, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  83. Russell, S.; Nicoll, C. A Dissection of the Chapter “Tools for Research” in Peter Singer’s Animal Liberation. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 1996, 211, 109–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Colonius, T.; Swoboda, J. Student Perspectives on Animal-Welfare Education in American Veterinary Medical Curricula. 2010, 37, 56–60. [CrossRef]
  85. Abood, S.K.; Siegford, J.M. Student Perceptions of an Animal-Welfare and Ethics Course Taught Early in the Veterinary Curriculum. J Vet Med Educ 2012, 39, 136–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Tuyttens, F.A.M.; Vanhonacker, F.; Van Poucke, E.; Verbeke, W. Quantitative Verification of the Correspondence between the Welfare Quality® Operational Definition of Farm Animal Welfare and the Opinion of Flemish Farmers, Citizens and Vegetarians. Livest Sci 2010, 131, 108–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Sebo, J. Saving Animals, Saving Ourselves; Oxford University Press: New York, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  88. Harper, G.; Henson, S.; Miele, M.; Parisi, V.; Degli Studi di Pisa, U.; Reimar von Alvensleben, P.; Köhler, F. Consumer Concerns about Animal Welfare and the Impact on Food Choice 1 EU FAIR CT98-3678; 2001.
  89. Yeates, J.W. Sentience, Harmony and the Value of Nature. Animals (Basel) 2023, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Rochlitz, I.; Broom, D.M. The Welfare of Ducks during Foie Gras Production. Animal Welfare 2017, 26, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Kalkura, P.; B, P.R.; N, S.K.; Surya; Ramyashree, Ms. Pest Control Management System Using Organic Pesticides. Global Transitions Proceedings . 2021, 2, 175–180. [CrossRef]
  92. Mankad, A.; Kennedy, U.; Carter, L. Biological Control of Pests and a Social Model of Animal Welfare. J Environ Manage 2019, 247, 313–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Feinberg, J. The Rights of Animals and Unborn Generations. In Philosophy and Environmental Crisis.; Blackstone, W.T., Ed.; University of Georgia Press: Athens, USA, 1974; pp. 43–6. [Google Scholar]
  94. Frey, R.G. Interests and Rights: The Case Against Animals; Press, Clarendon: Oxford, UK, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  95. Zuolo, F. A Theory of Justice for Animals. Animal Rights in a Nonideal World. By Robert Garner (Oxford: Oxford Universit Press, 2013). Constellations 2015, 22, 473–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Guzmán Dalbora, J.L. El Delito de Maltrato de Animales. In La ciencia del derecho penal ante el nuevo siglo: libro homenaje al profesor doctor don José Cerezo Mir; Díez Ripollés, J.L., Cerezo Mir, J., Eds.; Tecnos, 2002; pp. 1319–1350 ISBN 84-309-3921-0.
  97. Saez, J.A.G. ¿Pueden Los Animales Ser Titulares de Derechos? Algunos Argumentos Desde Una Teoría Garantista Del Derecho. Revista Catalana de Dret Ambiental 2011, 3. [Google Scholar]
  98. Foucault, M. The History of Sexuality; Pantheon Books: New York, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  99. Foucault, M. Right of Death and Power over Life. The History of Sexuality 1984, 258–272. [Google Scholar]
  100. Derrida, J. The Animal That Therefore I Am; Malle, M.-L., Ed.; Fordham University Press.: New York, 2008.
Figure 1. Samples ages distribution for gender.
Figure 1. Samples ages distribution for gender.
Preprints 102870 g001
Figure 2. Age distribution within college level.
Figure 2. Age distribution within college level.
Preprints 102870 g002
Figure 3. Heat map of ethical perspectives and sociodemographic factors among agronomy engineering students. p < 0.05.
Figure 3. Heat map of ethical perspectives and sociodemographic factors among agronomy engineering students. p < 0.05.
Preprints 102870 g003
Figure 4. Heat map of interdependencies between sociodemographic variables and bioethical considerations in animal welfare by agronomy students. p < 0.05.
Figure 4. Heat map of interdependencies between sociodemographic variables and bioethical considerations in animal welfare by agronomy students. p < 0.05.
Preprints 102870 g004
Figure 5. Heat map of interrelated sociodemographic factors influencing legal and normative criteria knowledge and perceptions on animal welfare by agronomy students. p < 0.05.
Figure 5. Heat map of interrelated sociodemographic factors influencing legal and normative criteria knowledge and perceptions on animal welfare by agronomy students. p < 0.05.
Preprints 102870 g005
Table 1. Number of respondents (percentage variation in brackets) to the questions about animal ethics, bioethics, and legal issues in animal welfare according to sociodemographic variables (N = 144).
Table 1. Number of respondents (percentage variation in brackets) to the questions about animal ethics, bioethics, and legal issues in animal welfare according to sociodemographic variables (N = 144).
Gender Age breakdown College level Upbringing environment
Male Female 18-20 21-23 24-26 >26 <50% >50% Rural Urban
89 (62) 55 (38) 52 (36) 43 (30) 34 (24) 15 (10) 82 (57) 62 (43) 123 (85) 21 (15)
Table 2. Ethical perception on animal welfare among agronomy students by the question "Provide an assessment of how you believe the following farm animals are generally treated".
Table 2. Ethical perception on animal welfare among agronomy students by the question "Provide an assessment of how you believe the following farm animals are generally treated".
Animal p-value2
Gender
Male, N = 891 Female, N = 551
Turkey 0.016
Bad 10 (11%) 0 (0%)
Regular 36 (40%) 29 (53%)
Good 43 (48%) 26 (47%)
Beef cattle 0.050
Bad 4 (4.5%) 9 (16%)
Regular 35 (39%) 21 (38%)
Good 50 (56%) 25 (45%)
Dairy cattle 0.10
Bad 6 (6.7%) 9 (16%)
Regular 23 (26%) 17 (31%)
Good 60 (67%) 29 (53%)
Upbringing environment
Rural, N = 1231 Urban, N = 211
Sheep 0.050
Bad 3 (2.4%) 2 (9.5%)
Regular 51 (41%) 12 (57%)
Good 69 (56%) 7 (33%)
Rabbit 0.026
Bad 6 (4.9%) 4 (19%)
Regular 34 (28%) 8 (38%)
Good 83 (67%) 9 (43%)
Beef cattle 0.039
Bad 8 (6.5%) 5 (24%)
Regular 48 (39%) 8 (38%)
Good 67 (54%) 8 (38%)
Dairy cattle 0.050
Bad 10 (8.1%) 5 (24%)
Regular 33 (27%) 7 (33%)
Good 80 (65%) 9 (43%)
1 n (%), 2 Fisher’s exact test; Pearson’s Chi-squared test.
Table 3. Ethical perspectives on 'Humane' pest control and human-animal interactions food practices: gender and education level influences.
Table 3. Ethical perspectives on 'Humane' pest control and human-animal interactions food practices: gender and education level influences.
"Do you consider it ethical and responsible for social movements to aim at using 'humane' methods to exterminate crop pests?"
Variable No, N = 441 Yes, N = 351 p-value2
Gender 0.004
Male 36 (82%) 18 (51%)
Female 8 (18%) 17 (49%)
Education level
 < 25 % 11 (25%) 17 (49%)
 25-50 % 10 (23%) 10 (29%)
 51-75 % 6 (14%) 1 (2.9%)
 > 75% 17 (39%) 7 (20%)
"How do you assess the act of humans killing other animals for food?"
Variable Bad, N = 211 Good, N = 1231 p-value2
Gender <0.001
Male 6 (29%) 83 (67%)
Female 15 (71%) 40 (33%)
Age 0.070
 18 a 20 13 (62%) 39 (32%)
 21 a 23 5 (24%) 38 (31%)
 24 a 26 2 (9.5%) 32 (26%)
 Más de 26 1 (4.8%) 14 (11%)
Education level 0.004
 < 25 % 12 (57%) 41 (33%)
 25-50 % 3 (14%) 26 (21%)
 51-75 % 5 (24%) 12 (9.8%)
 > 75% 1 (4.8%) 44 (36%)
1 n (%), 2 Fisher’s exact test; Pearson’s Chi-squared test. Percentages within each variable are shown for column.
Table 4. Influential Factors in Attitudes Toward Animal Welfare, bioethical Perceptions, and Assessments in Agronomy Students.
Table 4. Influential Factors in Attitudes Toward Animal Welfare, bioethical Perceptions, and Assessments in Agronomy Students.
How would you rate your level of concern for animal welfare?
Variable Low, N = 441 High, N = 1021 p-value2
Gender <0.001
Male 35 (83%) 54 (53%)
Female 7 (17%) 48 (47%)
Regarding animal suffering, do you believe some animals are more valuable than others?
Variable No, N = 63 1 Yes, N = 81 1 p-value 2
Gender <0.001
Male 26 (41%) 63 (78%)
Female 37 (59%) 18 (22%)
Education level 0.008
 < 25 % 32 (51%) 21 (26%)
 25-50 % 11 (17%) 18 (22%)
 51-75 % 8 (13%) 9 (11%)
 > 75% 12 (19%) 33 (41%)
How do you assess non-human animals preying on each other?
Variable Bad, N = 161 Good, N = 1281 p-value2
Gender 0.034
Male 6 (38%) 83 (65%)
Female 10 (63%) 45 (35%)
Education level 0.044
 < 25 % 7 (44%) 46 (36%)
 25-50 % 2 (13%) 27 (21%)
 51-75 % 5 (31%) 12 (9.4%)
 > 75% 2 (13%) 43 (34%)
How do you assess animal research, specifically using them as models in trials to test new drugs for humans?
Variable Bad, N = 841 Good, N = 601 p-value2
Gender 0.002
Male 43 (51%) 46 (77%)
Female 41 (49%) 14 (23%)
Education level 0.002
 < 25 % 37 (44%) 16 (27%)
 25-50 % 22 (26%) 7 (12%)
 51-75 % 7 (8.3%) 10 (17%)
 > 75% 18 (21%) 27 (45%)
Upbringing environment 0.070
Rural 68 (81%) 55 (92%)
Urban 16 (19%) 5 (8.3%)
1 n (%), 2 Fisher’s exact test; Pearson’s Chi-squared test.
Table 5. Gendered perceptions and emotional disparities of agronomy students about animal suffering.
Table 5. Gendered perceptions and emotional disparities of agronomy students about animal suffering.
Variable Indifferent N = 81 Unbereable N = 681 Unpleasant yet tolerable N = 681 p-value2
Gender <0.001
Male 8 (100%) 29 (43%) 52 (76%)
Female 0 (0%) 39 (57%) 16 (24%)
1 n (%), 2 Fisher’s exact test; Pearson’s Chi-squared test.
Table 6. Perceptions of Gender-Associated regulated activities: hunting, recreational fishing, and pest control in crops by agronomy engineering students.
Table 6. Perceptions of Gender-Associated regulated activities: hunting, recreational fishing, and pest control in crops by agronomy engineering students.
Activity p-value2
Gender
Male,N = 891 Female,N = 551
Hunting to regulate overpopulation of species 0.005
Bad 16 (18%) 19 (35%)
Regular 21 (24%) 19 (35%)
Good 52 (58%) 17 (31%)
Recreational fishing <0.001
Bad 7 (7.9%) 17 (31%)
Regular 29 (33%) 24 (44%)
Good 53 (60%) 14 (25%)
Pest control in crops <0.001
Bad 1 (1.1%) 1 (1.8%)
Regular 8 (9.0%) 18 (33%)
Good 80 (90%) 36 (65%)
1 n (%), 2 Fisher’s exact test; Pearson’s Chi-squared test.
Table 7. Gender disparities in perceptions of animal well-being associated with insights from hunting, research, and animal rights advocacy.
Table 7. Gender disparities in perceptions of animal well-being associated with insights from hunting, research, and animal rights advocacy.
“Do you consider that animals subjected to hunting (even when each country's regulations are followed) limit their well-being?”
Variable No, N = 191 Yes, N = 801 p-value2
Gender 0.2
Male 14 (74%) 47 (59%)
Female 5 (26%) 33 (41%)
Age (years) 0.026
 18 a 20 4 (21%) 31 (39%)
 21 a 23 4 (21%) 28 (35%)
 24 a 26 10 (53%) 14 (18%)
 > 26 1 (5.3%) 7 (8.8%)
“If you admit that animals should not suffer, would you be willing to use animals in scientific experimentation as models for the study of human diseases?”
Variable No, N = 861 Yes, N = 581 p-value2
Gender <0.001
Male 41 (48%) 48 (83%)
Female 45 (52%) 10 (17%)
Education level 0.017
 < 25 % 39 (45%) 14 (24%)
 25-50 % 17 (20%) 12 (21%)
 51-75 % 11 (13%) 6 (10%)
 > 75% 19 (22%) 26 (45%)
"If you acknowledge that animals are not mere objects, do you believe that animals should be granted 'rights'?"
Variable No, N = 431 Yes, N = 1001 p-value2
Gender <0.001
Male 36 (84%) 52 (52%)
Female 7 (16%) 48 (48%)
Education level 0.014
 < 25 % 11 (26%) 42 (42%)
 25-50 % 5 (12%) 24 (24%)
 51-75 % 6 (14%) 10 (10%)
 > 75% 21 (49%) 24 (24%)
Upbringing environment 0.035
Rural 41 (95%) 82 (82%)
Urban 2 (4.7%) 18 (18%)
“Do you consider granting rights as the only way to protect the interests of animals?”
Variable No, N = 971 Yes, N = 471 p-value2
Gender 0.003
Male 68 (70%) 21 (45%)
Female 29 (30%) 26 (55%)
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated