Introduction
The golden lion tamarin (GLT), Leontopithecus rosalia, is a small, social and adaptable frugi-faunivore primate of the Callitrichidae family (Kierulff, et al., 2012) (Stolwijk, 2013). They are endemic to the lowland Atlantic Forest region of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil (Kierulff, et al., 2012) (Stolwijk, 2013) (Lapenta, et al., 2007). The golden lion tamarin has a long, silky, and amber pelage as well as a dark bare face surrounded by the mane. The species have elongated arms and fingers, which are used to forage for fruits, small prey, and plant exudes (gums) (Kierulff, et al., 2012) (Snyder, 1974). Wild animals live in matriarchal social groups (called troops) consisting of about four to eight individuals (Stolwijk, 2013) (Lapenta, et al., 2007), typically composed of a breeding pair and their offspring (Lapenta, et al., 2007). The social structure of the species has been shaped by the fact that 78 % of dominant females pregnancies result in twin births, which has led to cooperative breeding in the species (Snyder, 1974) (Stolwijk, 2013) (Lapenta, et al., 2007).
In 1982 GLT was added to IUCN Red List of Threatened Species (Ruiz-Miranda, et al., 2019) due to declining habitat availability and quality (Stolwijk, 2013). They were reclassified as critically endangered from 1996-2000 (Ruiz-Miranda, et al., 2019) (Stolwijk, 2013) (Dietz, 2000). Although the species is now in recovery, there are still complications regarding breeding issues in some captive environments (Snyder, 1974) (Stolwijk, 2013) (Burell & Altman, 2006).
Golden lion tamarins raised in captivity are exposed to other environments and stimuli than their wild-born counterparts (Ruvio & Stevenson, 2017) (Hosey, 2005) (Lutz, et al., 2022). Therefore, they could express different behaviors, new behaviours or possibly lose existing behavioural patterns essential for their social interactions within the troop in addition to their survival in the wild (Hosey, 2005) (Ruvio & Stevenson, 2017). Furthermore, this may result in social instability within a captive-reared troop (Anzenberger & Falk, 2012). These behaviours can include the deficient acquisition of essential survival skills (Sanders & Fernandez, 2020), or detrimental and unnatural stereotypical behaviour, which is often observed in captive animals (Hosey, 2005) (Gottlieb, et al., 2013). Stereotypical behaviours are atypical and repetitive behaviours with no apparent purpose, such as auto-aggression and autoerotism (Gottlieb, et al., 2013) (Lutz, et al., 2022) (Linder, et al., 2020). These behavioural patterns usually indicate that an animals’ psychological welfare is sub-optimal (Lutz, et al., 2022) (Mason & Rushen, 2006) (Linder, et al., 2020). Stereotypical behaviour is often accentuated by insufficient stimuli in the enclosure or may occur as a stress response to the environment or other individuals in the social hierarchy (Lutz, et al., 2022) (Redshaw & Mallinson, 1991) (Carlstead & Shepherdson, 2000). The abnormal stereotypical behaviour may not necessarily be in direct relation to the current welfare state of the animal but could also be a trait from previous triggers (Lutz, et al., 2022) that has become a habitual or automatic response that persists (Lutz, et al., 2022). Auto-aggression and other self-directed behaviours might likewise develop because of restricted physical contact during the first months of life for Callitrichidae (Mallapur & Choudhury, 2003).
Another factor which might induce a stress response in captive GLT in a zoo enclosure, is the presence of larger family troops of other contending Callitrichidae species in proximity (Kierulff, et al., 2012). Studies show that Callitrichidae species should not be housed adjacent to each other, even if they cannot physically interact (Kierulff, et al., 2012) (Snyder, 1974). If there is visual and/or olfactory contact, it could result in severe querulousness (Snyder, 1974). Additionally, GLT will scent mark their territory more frequently when in proximity of contending Callitrichidae (Snyder, 1974). Scent marking activity might increase after sexual encounters, alterations in territory and aggressive intra- and interspecies interactions (Snyder, 1974). The impacts of the zoo environment on the behaviour of captive primates such as GLT have been researched, but concrete conclusions have not been found (Hosey, 2005).
Zoos generally run a higher risk of having an environment that is not optimally suited for the animal (Hosey, 2005) (Sanders & Fernandez, 2020), although modern zoos try to negate this issue to assure the welfare of the animals (Hosey, 2005). Zoos might provide climate control in the enclosures, variation in stimuli and attempt to promote natural behaviours seen in wild GLT, by for instance promoting troop-compositions which could exist in a natural environment (Hosey, 2005) (Snyder, 1974) (Stolwijk, 2013), playing (Snyder, 1974) (Lutz, et al., 2022), self- and allogrooming (Snyder, 1974), resting during the day and bedding down together in dens at lights out (Snyder, 1974), via enrichment programs (Linder, et al., 2020).
While much is known of the social structure and -behaviours of captive Callitrichidae in general, less is known of the influence of an incomplete troop on individual stereotypical behaviour and social interaction. Even less is known of the influence of a sick individual in a captive, small troop.
The aim of this study was to investigate whether the frequency and duration of social- and self-directed behaviours in a captive pair of golden lion tamarins are influenced by a single sick individual. It was anticipated that allogrooming and other positive social interactions would be observed frequently. In lieu of this investigation, potential stereotypical behavioural patterns was examined. This study highlighted the importance of animal welfare in captivity, social- and self-directed behaviours and interactions in social groups.