1. Introduction
The tendency to adopt a particular pattern of causal inferences has been referred to as one’s attributional style or explanatory style [
1]. Attributional style is conceptualised along three dimensions: internal–external, global–specific, and stable–unstable [
2]. The internal–external dimension refers to whether the explanation for an event is grounded in the physical, behavioural, or cognitive characteristics of the self (i.e., internal) or arises as a consequence of someone or something outside the self (i.e., external). The stable–unstable dimension refers to whether the explanation for an event has enduring (i.e., stable) or ephemeral effects (i.e., unstable). Lastly, the global–specific dimension refers to whether the event is viewed as having ubiquitous effects and generalised to much of the individual’s life (i.e., global) or has more limited effects and influences only particular facets of the individual’s life (i.e., specific).
Considerable evidence underscores the importance of attributional style in human functioning and wellbeing. Research conducted in adult populations demonstrated a significant association between attributional style and health outcomes [
3], occupational outcomes [
4], athletic performance [
5], and many other relevant measures of functioning [
6]. Notably, these findings extend to adolescents. For instance, Dweck and colleagues have persuasively argued that the attributions made by youth after academic and social task failures constitute a critical determinant of their subsequent responses to those tasks [
7,
8]. That is, how youth explain or attribute these events serves as a predictive factor for their future explanations or attributions of similar occurrences. Along the same lines, research conducted on children identified a connection between attributional style and both social and academic performance [
9,
10,
11,
12,
13]. Moreover, children attributing their social failures to internal and stable factors (i.e., a lack of ability) are more likely to withdraw from social interactions and are less liked by peers [
14].
In a psychopathological framework, internal, stable, and global attributions for the causes of negative events have demonstrated significant implications for depressive symptomatology in children and adolescents [
15,
16,
17,
18]. Moreover, existing literature suggests that children with anxiety disorders tend to exhibit higher negative attributions (internal, global, stable) for adverse events compared to normal controls (for a review see [
19,
20,
21]). A perceived lack of control over life events is described as a key risk factor for both anxiety and depression, directly related to hopeless attributional style (i.e., attributional style characterised by a sense of uncontrollability, uncertainty, and unpredictability regarding future occurrences of events).
If the association between attributional style and internalizing symptoms has been ascertained by several meta-analyses [
22,
23], the direction of this link is still to be clarified. Most of the existing longitudinal studies considered attributional style as the predictor, but this does not exclude the possibility that internalizing symptoms predict a specific attributional style. In support of this claim, an experimental study conducted by Lo and colleagues [
24] showed that depressive symptoms were able to predict negative attributional style among college students. More recently, another study demonstrated that, after controlling for depressive symptoms, positive attributions for good events were predicted by low levels of social anxiety in 337 Italian children [
25].
Another possibility, that has not been tested yet, is that a bidirectional link exists between attributional style and internalizing symptoms.
Finally, it is possible that attributional style acts as a mediator between specific individual features and internalizing symptoms. Support to this latter possibility comes from a recent study conducted by Luo and Wu [
26], who showed that a depressogenic attributional style significantly mediated the relationship between self-discrepancies and anxiety.
Overall, beyond the nature and the direction of the link between attributional style and adaptive/maladaptive functioning, early identification of dysfunctional attributional style seems to be particularly important, as potentially lifelong consequences can be prevented or mitigated through timely interventions.
The main measure of attributional style in children is the Children’s Attributional Style Questionnaire [
2] (CASQ), which aligns with the reformulation of the learned helplessness model of depression in children [
27], and is constituted by 48 items. Several studies have consistently demonstrated that children making more internal-stable-global attributions for negative events and more external-unstable-specific attributions for positive events, as measured by CASQ, tend to report higher levels of depressive symptoms compared to their peers with the reverse attributional style [
28].
In the last years, a revised version of the CASQ has been developed, constituted by 24 items. The fact that children have a limited attention span, and that the CASQ is often used in larger test batteries, have led to the development of a shortened version [
29]. The 24 items of the CASQ revised [
30] (CASQ-R) were selected based on psychometric analyses (i.e., positive corrected item-total correlations and internal consistency reliability) conducted on the answers of 449 elementary school children who completed the original CASQ [
31,
32]. The psychometric properties of CASQ-R were examined on 1,086 children aged 9 to 12 years old, with equal representation of boys and girls, and of African American and Caucasian children. Approximately one half (n = 475) of the youths also completed the CASQ-R six months later. Results revealed that although the CASQ-R was somewhat less reliable than the original CASQ, with moderate internal consistency reliability and fair test-retest reliability, it demonstrated equivalent criterion-related validity with self-reported depressive symptoms. A following study [
33] using a sample of 621 adolescents (184 males and 437 females), aged 11–18 years, indicated an adequate model fit for a one-factor solution for negative event items and a two-factor solution for positive event items. Reliability estimates for these factors were low but acceptable in the former, and lower than recommended guidelines in the latter.
Nonetheless, to date, the CASQ-R is the most widely used measure of youth attributions. Since the CASQ-R has never been studied in Italian children, this study aims at assessing preliminary psychometric properties of the Italian CASQ-R, to determine whether this shortened scale is a reliable and valid substitute for the original CASQ with Italian-speaking children. Furthermore, to delve deeper into the investigation of attributional styles within the general population of Italian-speaking children, we conducted a clustering analysis using the CASQ-R scores. This approach allowed us to explore the presence of any subgroups of children who might exhibit internally homogeneous and externally heterogeneous attributional styles. Finally, we tested the mean differences in the target sociodemographic and clinical variables across the identified clusters.
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
The sample is composed of 337 children (11.29 ± 1.76 years old, 169 females and 168 males) attending the third (N = 87) and fifth (N = 73) year of primary school, and the second year (N = 177) of middle school.
3.2. Between-Group Comparisons
T-test for independent samples revealed that no gender differences exist in the assessed sample with regard to positive attributional style (t(323) = 1.35, p = .09, d = 1.92), negative attributional style (t(323) = .517, p = .30, d = 1.91), and overall attributional style (t(323) = .534, p = .29, d = 2.99), whereas significant gender differences emerged on depressive symptomatology (t(335) = -2.33, p = .01, d = 6.24) with higher depressive symptoms displayed by females (11.63 ± 6.22) compared to males (10.04 ± 6.25).
Similarly, t-test for independent samples among the younger (˂11 years old) and older (˃12 years old) cohort revealed no age differences regarding all the study variables (positive attributional style: t(309) = .126, p = .450, d = 1.90; negative attributional style: t(309) = .277, p = .39, d = 1.92; overall attributional style; t(309) = -.098, p = .461, d = 2.97; and depressive symptomatology: t(321) = 1.27, p = .10, d = 6.32).
3.3. Psychometric Properties of CASQ-R
Internal consistency of CASQ-R. The internal consistency of CASQ-R was calculated on a sample of 325 subjects using the Kuder-Richardson reliability coefficient (KR20), which is the statistical equivalent of Cronbach’s Alpha in case of dichotomous items (usually scored as 0 or 1), for the positive (α = .370) and negative (α = .352) composite scores, as well as for the single CASQ-R subscales, namely positive internality (α = .200), positive stability (α = .207), positive globality (α = .173), negative internality (α = .260), negative stability (α = .272), and negative globality (α = .099).
Criterion-related validity of the CASQ-R. Pearson’s bivariate correlations run on the entire sample outlined that CASQ-R positive (r = -.328,
p < .001), negative (r =.249,
p < .001), and overall (r = -.371,
p < .001) composite scores correlated significantly and in the predicted directions with children’s score on CDI. Specifically, lower scores on the positive (i.e., a more depressive attributional style for positive events) and overall (i.e., more internal-stable-global attributions for negative events and more external-unstable-specific attributions for positive events) composite scores were associated with higher levels of depressive symptoms in children; whereas higher scores on the negative composite score (i.e., a depressive attributional style for negative events) were related to heightened self-reported symptoms of depression. Criterion-related validity of CASQ-R was also performed separately by gender and age categories and, as
Table 1 and
Table 2 show, the same correlational patterns emerged with no differences noted among males and females, as well as among younger and older children.
Cluster estimation. The six CASQ-R subscales (i.e., Negative Globality, Negative Internality, Negative Stability, Positive Globality, Positive Internality, and Positive Stability) were considered jointly in a multivariate FMM. The BIC value (-5284.253) suggested the presence of four clusters with diagonal distribution, variable volume, and equal shape. The clusters’ estimated results are summarized in
Figure 1 and
Table 3.
Cluster characteristics. Non-parametric between-clusters comparisons outlined that the six CASQ-R subscales were significantly different among the four clusters. Results are shown in
Figure 2,
Figure 3,
Figure 4,
Figure 5,
Figure 6 and
Figure 7.
The first cluster is characterized by low levels of positive globality, positive internality, and positive stability attributional style, medium levels of negative globality attributional style, and high levels of negative internality attributional style.
The second cluster is characterized by low levels of negative globality attributional style and medium levels of negative internality, negative stability, positive globality, positive internality, and positive stability attributional style.
The third cluster is characterized by medium levels of positive globality, positive internality, and positive stability attributional style, and high levels of negative globality, negative internality and negative stability attributional style.
The fourth cluster is characterized by low levels of negative internality and negative stability attributional style, medium levels of negative globality attributional style, and high levels of positive globality, positive internality, and positive stability attributional style.
Likewise, between-clusters comparisons highlighted that depressive symptoms are significantly different among the four clusters. Specifically, low levels of depressive symptoms were encountered in the second and fourth clusters, medium levels of depressive symptoms in the first cluster and high levels in the third cluster (See
Figure 8).
Lastly, no significant between-cluster differences emerged considering age and gender.
4. Discussion
The present research aimed at evaluating the psychometric properties of the Italian CASQ-R and at exploring the presence of any subgroups of children and early adolescents of the general population who might exhibit internally homogeneous and externally heterogeneous attributional styles. Moreover, we aimed at investigating potential sociodemographic and clinical differences among the identified clusters of attributional styles.
Overall, the internal consistency of the CASQ-R composite scores was very low in the present study. Nonetheless, existing literature concerning both CASQ and CASQ-R (both original version and translations) most of times reported mediocre values [
29,
33,
39,
40,
41,
42,
43,
44].
Criterion-related validity of the CASQ-R yielded good results, in that positive/negative/overall composite scores of the CASQ-R significantly correlated with depressive symptoms scores in a coherent fashion (i.e., depressive symptoms were positively associated with negative CASQ-R and negatively associated with positive/overall CASQ-R). This result is in line with past literature [
2,
29] and further extends the significant link between attributional style and depressive symptoms to Italian children of the general populations.
Moreover, in this study, neither age nor gender differences emerged for CASQ-R composite scores, coherently with the results obtained in previous studies conducted on subjects of a similar age [
29]. Nonetheless, we believe that gender should still be investigated in future research, as it could be an important factor especially when considering attributions interacting with depressive symptoms and/or anxiety [
45,
46,
47,
48,
49,
50,
51]. Similarly, age is to be taken into account when the comparison is between very different age groups, since people become increasingly selective in engaging cognitive resources and they can develop expert social knowledge across time [
52,
53].
The overall low internal consistency found in the present research warranted further statistical analyses based on clustering. Specifically, four distinct clusters emerged using the six CASQ-R subscales in a multivariate finite mixture model. Such clusters presented peculiar levels of positive/negative, internal/external, stable/unstable attributions. More interestingly, the four clusters revealed different associations with depressive symptoms scores, so that high levels of depressive symptoms were reported by children showing high scores in all the negative attributional subscales (globality, internality and stability) and medium scores in all the positive attributional subscales. Conversely, low levels of depressive symptoms were reported by children showing medium-to-high scores in the positive attributional subscales and low-to-medium scores in the negative attributional subscales. Our results are not directly comparable with those of other research, as no previous study used cluster analysis to look at attributional styles. Nonetheless, the features of the distinct clusters are definitely in line with the large amount of literature that has accumulated on the attributional styles that typically characterize depressed subjects [
22,
23,
54,
55]. Notably, neither gender nor age differences were found comparing the four clusters emerged in our study.
Overall, the interesting groupings of subjects that emerged in our study could guide interventions aimed at reducing depressive symptoms starting from the school context by acting on attributional styles in children and adolescents. Such interventions could build on Dweck’s work conducted within the framework of the cognitive theories of depression, which focuses on the cognitive restructuring of attributions[
56]. More recently, children's beliefs/mindsets about the stability of self-concepts such as intelligence and personality received growing attention. Therefore, promising interventions could also target children’s mindset, with potential beneficial effects on wellbeing [
57,
58].
Despite our study offers valuable insights, it is essential to acknowledge certain limitations. First, the generalizability of our findings to a broader population may be constrained due to the regional focus on Northern Italy. Secondly, the cross-sectional design of our study limits our ability to establish causation, and future research employing longitudinal approaches could provide a more nuanced understanding of the phenomenon. Specifically, research should better address the independent and interactive relationships among attributional styles, life events, coping and depressive symptomatology [
59,
60].
In summary, our study investigated attributional styles and depressive symptoms in a sample of Italian children using the CASQ-R; our findings contribute to the understanding of attributional styles in the Italian context. The identified clusters shed light on distinct patterns associated with depressive symptoms, offering potential insights for targeted interventions. Future research should employ longitudinal designs, diverse measurement methods, and broader participant inclusion to strengthen causal inferences and enhance generalizability. Additionally, evaluating the effectiveness of interventions targeting attributional styles could further inform preventive strategies for depressive symptoms in children.