1. Introduction
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia in the elderly, accounting for more than 60% of all cases [
1]. It is characterized by a slow onset and progressive cognitive decline that affect higher functions as language, personality, memory, visuospatial perception and knowledge [
2]. Consequently, this neurodegenerative disease is a serious problem that decreases the quality of life of patients and their relatives.
The stage at which a diagnosis of AD is made impacts the therapy advised, the counseling given to patients and family, and the approach to long-term care [
3]. Therefore, the issue in AD diagnosis today is to recognize the disease before the cognitive deficits have reached the threshold of dementia, that is to say, at its prodromal stage. Several terms have been used to describe this pre-dementia phase. The most popular one is Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), proposed by Petersen et al. in the late 90s [
4,
5].
In the last decade, there has been unprecedented growth of scientific knowledge about early diagnosis, especially regarding biomarkers and neuroimaging techniques. However, in clinical context, the diagnosis of AD is mainly based on neuropsychological testing. The best known cognitive markers of this pathology are: 1) deterioration of episodic memory, which does not improve even with cues and involves the presence of numerous intrusions and perseverations; 2) reduced social and occupational performance; 3) anosognosia; 4) anomie or difficulty in naming; 5) spatial disorientation in unfamiliar places; 6) deficits in visual processing speed and in selective and divided attention and; 7) communicative difficulties [6}. A marked deterioration in semantic memory is a consistent finding in patients, even in the early stages of the Alzheimer’s disease [
7,
8,
9]. One of the earliest and most notorious manifestations are naming problems, which are usually measured by verbal fluency (VF) tasks, in which the subject is asked to produce as many words as possible in a given time [
10,
11]. The two forms of verbal fluency commonly assessed are phonemic - related to the retrieval of words that begin with a certain letter or phoneme- and semantic -related to the ability to produce series of words that belong to a semantic category such as animals or fruits11. It also has to be said that these tasks are not only related to the activation of processes linked to lexical access, but also to executive processes that are also altered with age, especially in cognitive impairment processes [
12,
13].
Several studies have found semantic verbal fluency (SVF) to be very sensitive assessment of semantic impairment at a very early stage of AD [
8,
14,
15,
16,
17], but this is not a universal finding. On the one hand, Whels et al. [
18,
19] did not find statistic significant differences between patients with mild AD and healthy elderly control subjects in performing the SVF task, and Albert et al. [
20] reported normal performance on naming and category fluency in a group of patients categorized as 0.5 on the clinical dementia rating (CDR) scale [
21]. On the other hand, Goñi et al. [
22] found that people with Alzheimer's disease performed worse than the control group in the phonemic verbal fluency (PVF) task, so they deduced that it deteriorates before the SVF. In this sense, there seems to be an unresolved debate about the type of fluency that deteriorates earlier in AD. For this reason, the aims of this research are, on the one hand, to evaluate both types of VF in people with early stages of Alzheimer's disease in order to know which of them deteriorates earlier and, on the other hand, to investigate whether either of them predicts the evolution of MCI to AD.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Twenty-five MCI patients and their respective control subjects matched by age, sex and educational level took part in the study (see
Table 1). The experimental group was recruited at the Neurology Unit of the University Hospital of Cabueñes (Asturias, Spain) and the control group was enrolled in cultural centers, Day Centers for the Aged and in the University Program for the Elderly of the University of Oviedo.
Two years after the first evaluation, the MCI patients were followed up and eight of them had converted to probable AD (MCI-AD).
The educational level was separated into three groups: Low (from 0 to 4 years), Medium (from 5 to 10 years), and High (more than 10 years).
The inclusion criteria used in the University Hospital of Cabueñes for MCI patients were: 1) objective memory impairment on neuropsychological evaluation; 2) normal activities of daily living; 3) evidence of concomitant dementia; 4) clinical and indirect evidence of depression; 5) other psychiatric diseases, epilepsy, drug addiction, and 6) current or previous uncontrolled systemic diseases or recent traumatic injuries. Medical history, neurological examination and brain scans (TAC or RM) were also reviewed for all patients. For their part, control subjects had to meet two conditions: 1) to be over 65 years of age and 2) to have an MMSE score equal to or higher than 26. As exclusion criteria for both groups; 1) not presenting any psychiatric, neurological (except MCI in the case of the experimental group) or medical disease that could interfere with the performance of the tests in this study.
The NIA-AA criteria (2011), proposed by the National Institute of Aging (NIA) and the Alzheimer’s Association (AA), were used to identify those MCI patients who converted to probable AD [
23].
All participants signed the informed consent after being informed of the study characteristics by their regular neurologist or psychologist. It should also be mentioned that the research complied with the standards established by the National Health Council on research involving human subjects and the study was approved by the Clinical Research Ethical Committee of Asturias.
2.2. Material
Two screening tests were applied to describe the general cognitive functioning of the participants: the Spanish adaptation of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [
24] and the Spanish version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [
25]. The former is the most commonly used screening method in the assessment of the severity of dementia in both clinical and research field; but the latter is better detecting MCI among patients over 60 years of age than MMSE [
26].
After cognitive screening, verbal fluency was assessed. First, participants were asked to produce words in the animal category -to assess SVF. Then, participants were asked to generate words that start with the letter P but neither names of people nor names of cities were allowed -to assess PVF. On each trial, they were told to generate as many words as possible within a minute.
In the phonological verbal fluency task, the choice of the letter depends on the language of the participants. In this case, the letter “P” was selected since it is the most informative [
27].
2.3. Procedure
The subjects were evaluated in one single session in an individual room in order to avoid possible distractions that could bias the results.
The neuropsychological evaluations were conducted entirely by a psychologist paying attention to the behavior of the subjects under testing. All the evidences of unexpected behavioral response during testing has been registered and taken into account.
The questionnaires were administered in the following order: 1) MMSE; 2) MOCA and; 3) verbal fluency tasks. Subsequently, the results obtained were statistically analyzed using the statistical software package (IBM SPSS Statistics 21, Chicago, IL., USA).
3. Results
Descriptive statistics are provided in
Table 1. On the one hand, as expected, the control group obtained higher scores than the experimental group in both types of fluency. On the other hand, the means of the SVF were higher than those of the PVF in both groups (see
Table 2).
Individual test scores for Verbal Fluency Task were transformed into Z-scores based on larger sample mean and standard deviation (see
Table 2). Next, to determine whether it is SVF or PVF that deteriorates earlier in subjects with MCI, a Student’s t-test was performed (see
Table 3).
As can be seen in
Table 3, there are statistically significant differences between experimental and control group in PVF (t = -2.30 p = .026) but not in SVF performance (t = -1.56 p = .124). Hence, it seems that PVF deteriorates earlier.
Finally, to investigate if Verbal Fluency Tasks can help to predict the conversion from MCI to probable AD two years later, we analyze the differences between the eight patients who converted to AD (MCI-AD) from those seventeen who did not (MCI-nonAD) at the time in which they all maintained the original diagnosis (see
Table 5). Due to the small sample size and non-compliance with the normality criterion, Mann-Whitney U test was calculated. Descriptive statistics are provided in
Table 4.
It is worth mentioning the age difference between both groups. Although all subjects are diagnosed with Mild Cognitive Impairment, some developed symptoms at an early age while others at an older age. Nevertheless, the difference of almost 3 years between both groups is not statistically significant.
*p < 0.05.
After analyzing the data, no statistically significant differences were found between PVF and SVF in both groups. Nevertheless, if we compare the VF task performance between the eight patients with the diagnosis of probable AD –two years after the first evaluation- from those seventeen who maintained the original diagnosis of MCI, we find statistically significant differences between PVF and SVF in both groups (see
Table 6).
*p < 0.05.
Taking all these results together, it could be that either a larger sample size would allow statistically significant differences to be obtained, or that the deterioration of PVF could help predict the conversion of MCI to probable AD.
4. Discussion
Efforts at earlier detection of AD face significant challenges such as improving assessment of earliest symptoms. Memory impairment is typically the most common manifestation of early AD, but growing evidence suggests that other cognitive domains such language dysfunction begins several years before the onset of dementia, suggesting this could be a possible prognostic marker [
9,
28] that should be taken into account in neuropsychological assessments.
Language impairment initially affects verbal fluency and naming. Regarding fluency, although it is well known that patients with AD begin to produce a lower number of words in both semantic and phonemic verbal fluency compared with age-and education-matched elderly control subjects, controversy exits regarding the type of fluency measure that best discriminates patients with an early AD from healthy elderly. In this sense, our results show that the experimental group performed worse than the control group in both types of fluency. What is more, the SVF scores of the experimental group were slightly higher than that of the PVF and statistically significant differences were found.
These data are consistent with authors such as Goñi et al, Montañés, and Comesaña and Coni [
22,
27,
29]. They suggest that SVF is better preserved over time than PVF due to the executive impairment of AD patients. SVF tasks are considered to be easier to perform than PVF tasks because retrieval of words beginning with a certain letter involves searching a high subset number of categories and requires higher executive functioning performance.
Regarding assessing the progression of MCI to AD, although we found statistically significant differences between patients with MCI and AD in both type of fluency tasks, they were not found when comparing the performance of subjects with MCI and MCI who converted to AD two years later. Similarly, Vaughan et al. [
30] could not differentiate between progresses and non-progresses using individual fluency measures. Tracking progression from MCI to AD requires not only accurate diagnosis but also cognitive measures sensitive to change over time31. In this sense, Vaughan et al. [
30] proposed using discrepancy scores at baseline to discriminate between MCI who maintained stable and MCI who converted to AD after two years. Discrepancy scores were calculated for each participant by subtracting the letter fluency from the animal fluency score. They conclude that individuals with MCI with a phonemic advantage at initial assessment present a high index of suspicion for progression to AD. But the truth is that other studies of relative semantic-phonemic discrepancies in MCI have yielded discordant results. Therefore, future research is needed to determine the specificity of such findings to AD and their utility in serial assessments over time.
The current study is subject to certain limitations, in particular the small sample size that makes it difficult to generalize the results. Secondly, it is known that there is great variability in fluency performance depending on which fluency task is used, and our study focuses only on “letter p” and “animals” because these are frequently used in clinical practice. In addition to this, the use of combined categories is more reliable than use of single letters or categories, nevertheless, we believe that our results remain applicable clinically because “letter p” fluency is part of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment - a commonly administered cognitive screening test- and animal fluency can be easily added to any assessment protocol. Supporting our choice of category test, some investigators report that semantic fluency tasks that use large categories (e.g., animals) contain more disease-associated variance and are more sensitive for detecting AD [32]. Finally, it is also worth mentioning the strengths of the study. First, the diagnosis was undertaken by a specialized neurologist using standardized neuropsychological assessments and the latest published AD diagnostic criteria [
23]. Second and last, promote more research on a topic that continues to generate great controversy.
5. Conclusions
Despite the advances in the identification of AD-related biomarkers, neuropsychological assessment remains essential for diagnosis. Although memory dysfunction is the most common manifestation of early AD, some cases first present with executive, language or visuospatial disturbances. Our results support the fact that there are linguistic alterations already in early stages. Thus, in order to make an early diagnosis, verbal fluency should be taken into account, especially phonemic verbal fluency which is worse preserved over time than semantic verbal fluency. Additionally, further longitudinal studies are needed to clarify whether this may be clinically relevant in predicting progression of MCI to AD
Funding
This work was supported by the FC-15-GRUPIN14-021 project from the Asturias Regional Government and the CTQ2014-58826-R project from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO).
Informed Consent Statement
The research complied with the standards established by the National Health Council on research involving human subjects and the study was approved by the Clinical Research Ethical Committee of Asturias.
Acknowledgments
The author would like to thank Antonello Novelli, Fernando Cuetos and Carmen Martínez for their expertise and assistance throughout all aspects of the sudy.
Conflicts of Interest
Authors declare no Conflict of Interests for this article.
References
- De Leon MJ, Mosconi L, Blennow K et al. Imaging and CSF studies in the preclinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2007; 1097: 114-145. [CrossRef]
- Proaño J, Aguilar F. Enfermedad de Alzheimer. Clínica, diagnóstico y neuropatología. Plasticidad y Restauración Neurológica 2004; 3(1-2): 95-105.
- Dubois B, Picard G, Sarazin M. Early detection of Alzheimer's disease: new diagnostic criteria. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 2009; 11(2): 135-139. [CrossRef]
- Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Archives in Neurology 1999; 56(3): 303-308. [CrossRef]
- Valls-Pedret C, Molinuevo JL, Rami L. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: the prodromal and preclinical phase. Revista de Neurología 2010; 51(8): 471-480. [CrossRef]
- Jiménez MA, Arrieta E, Arriola E, Calle B, Colmenarejo JC, Cuadrado M. Cuidados Continuados en Atención Primaria a Personas con Enfermedad de Alzheimer. Informe Alzheimer Atención Primaria 2003.
- Chertkow, H.; Bub, D. Semantic memory loss in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Brain 1990, 113, 397–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hodges JR, Patterson K. Is semantic memory consistently impaired early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease? Neuroanatomical and diagnostic implications. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33(4): 441–459. [CrossRef]
- Dudas RB, Clague F, Thompson SA, Graham KS, Hodges, J.R. Episodic and semantic memory in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 2005. [CrossRef]
- Ivanova O, García Meilán JJ, Martínez-Nicolás I, Llorente T. La habilidad léxico-semántica en la Enfermedad de Alzheimer: Un estudio de la fluidez verbal con categorías semánticas. Revista Signos 2020; 53(102), 319-342. [CrossRef]
- Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 2012.
- Carballo, G.; García-Retamero, R.; Imedio, A.; García-Hernández, A. Diagnosis of cognitive impairment onset in older adults based on limitations in language skills/Diagnóstico del inicio del deterioro cognitivo en mayores a partir de limitaciones en las capacidades lingüísticas. Estudios de Psicología 2005, 36, 316–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luo L, Luk G, Bialystok E. Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition 2010; 114, 29–41. [CrossRef]
- Monsch AU, Bondi MW, Butters N, Salmon DP, Katzman R, Thal LJ. Comparisons of Verbal Fluency Tasks in the Detection of Dementia of the Alzheimer Type. Archives of Neurology 1992; 49(12): 1253-1258. [CrossRef]
- Gómez G, White DA. Using verbal fluency to detect very mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 2006; 21(8), 771-775. [CrossRef]
- Clark LJ, Gatz M, Zheng L, Chen Y-L, McCleary C, Mack WJ. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical and prevalent AD. American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease & Other Dementias 2009; 24(6), 461-468. [CrossRef]
- Henry JD, Crawford JR, Phillips LH. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2004; 42(9), 1212-1222. [CrossRef]
- Welsh K, Butters N, Hughes J, Mohs R, Heyman A. Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Archives of Neurology 1991; 48 (3): 278-281. [CrossRef]
- Welsh KA, Butters N, Hughes JP, Mohs RC, Heyman A. Detection and staging of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease. Use of the neuropsychological measures developed for the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease. Archives of Neurology 1992; 49(5): 448-452. [CrossRef]
- Albert MS, Moss MB, Tanzi R, Jones K. Preclinical prediction of AD using neuropsychological tests. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 2001; 7(5): 631-639. [CrossRef]
- Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal Of Psychiatry 1982; 140: 566-572.
- Goñi A, López JJ, Granados D, González A. Edad, escolarización y tareas de Fluencia Verbal para el screening de pacientes con Enfermedad de Alzheimer. Anales de Psicología/Annals of Psychology 2015; 31(3), 773-781. [CrossRef]
- McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, Klunk WE, Koroshetz WJ, Manly JJ, Mayeux R, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rossor MN, Scheltens P, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Weintraub S, Phelps CH. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the national institute on aging and the Alzheimer’s association work-group. Alzheimer’s Dementia 2011; 7(3), 263-269. [CrossRef]
- Folstein MF, Gary F, Folstein SE, Hugh PR. MMSE Examen Cognoscitivo Mini-Mental. Publicaciones de psicología aplicada; 1975.
- Lozano M, Hernández M, Turró O, Pericot I, López-Pousa S, Vilalta J. Validación del Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCa): test de cribado para el deterioro cognitivo leve. Datos preliminares. Alzheimer. Realidades e Investigación en Demencia 2009; 43, 4-11.
- Ciesielska N, Sokolowski R, Mazur E, Podhorecka M, Polak-Szablea A, Kędziora-Kornatowska K. Is the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) test better suited than the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) detection among people aged over 60? Meta-analysis. Psychiatria Polska 2016; 50(5), 1039-1052. [CrossRef]
- Montañés, P. Fluidez verbal en pacientes con enfermedad de Alzheimer: un análisis transversal y longitudinal. Revista de la Asociación Colombiana de Gerontología y Geriatría 2005; 19, 816-827.
- Verma M, Howard RJ. Semantic memory and language dysfunction in early Alzheimer's disease: a review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 2012; 27(12), 1209-1217. [CrossRef]
- Comesaña A, Coni AG. Análisis de tareas de fluidez verbal en enfermos de Alzheimer y adultos sanos. Temas en Psicología 2013; 21(1), 269-281. [CrossRef]
- Vaughan RM, Coen RF, Kenny RA, Lawlor BA. Semantic and Phonemic Verbal Fluency Discrepancy in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Potential Predictor of Progression to Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 2018; 66(4), 755-759. [CrossRef]
Table 1.
Demographic data.
Table 1.
Demographic data.
Variables |
Control Group |
Experimental Group |
N Age (Mean ± SD) Education (L/M/H) Gender (M/F) |
25 73.68 ± 5.12 7/12/6 15/10 |
25 74.60 ± 5.12 7/12/6 15/10 |
MMSE (Mean ± SD) |
28.96 ± 1.17 |
26.13 ± 2.50 |
MOCA (Mean ± SD) |
26.44 ± 2.40 |
20.10 ± 3.95 |
Table 2.
Mean and SD of PVF and SVF in the control groups and experimental groups.
Table 2.
Mean and SD of PVF and SVF in the control groups and experimental groups.
Variables |
Control Group |
Experimental Group |
PVF (Mean ± SD) |
14.36 ± 3.47 |
12.08 ± 3.53 |
SVF (Mean ± SD) |
16.76 ± 4.30 |
14.88 ± 4.20 |
PVF (Mean ± SD) |
14.36 ± 3.47 |
12.08 ± 3.53 |
Table 3.
Comparison of the PVF and SVF performance of the MCI and control group (Z-scores).
Table 3.
Comparison of the PVF and SVF performance of the MCI and control group (Z-scores).
VF |
Group |
N |
Mean |
SD |
t |
p |
ZPVF |
MCI MCI-C |
25 25 |
-0.18 0.38 |
± 0.88 ± 0.86 |
-2.30 |
.026* |
ZSVF |
MCI MCI-C |
25 25 |
-0.34 0.35 |
± 0.87 ± 0.89 |
-1.56 |
.124 |
Table 4.
Demographic data and Z-scores of PVF and SVF.
Table 4.
Demographic data and Z-scores of PVF and SVF.
Variables |
MCI-AD |
MCI-nonAD |
N Age (Mean ± SD) Education (L/M/H) Gender (M/F) |
8 72.38 ± 4.34 2/4/4 4/4 |
17 75.65 ± 5.23 5/8/4 11/6 |
MMSE MoCA |
24.75 ± 1.83 17.25 ± 2.60 |
26.94 ± 2.51 21.41 ± 3.81 |
Table 5.
Comparison of the PVF and SVF performance of the MCI-AD and MCI-nonAD group (Z-scores).
Table 5.
Comparison of the PVF and SVF performance of the MCI-AD and MCI-nonAD group (Z-scores).
VF |
Group |
Mean ± SD |
U |
p |
ZPVF |
MCI-nonAD MCI-AD |
-0.01 ± 0.72 -0.55 ± 1.12 |
38.50 |
.086 |
ZSVF |
MCI-nonAD MCI-AD |
-0.07 ± 0.87 -0.27 ± 0.89 |
56.50 |
.511 |
Table 6.
Comparison of the PVF and SVF performance of the MCI-nonAD and AD group (Z-scores).
Table 6.
Comparison of the PVF and SVF performance of the MCI-nonAD and AD group (Z-scores).
VF |
Group |
Mean ± SD |
U |
p |
ZPVF |
MCI-nonAD AD |
-0.01 ± 0.72 -1.26 ± 0.84 |
17.50 |
.002* |
ZSVF |
MCI-nonAD AD |
-0.07 ± 0.87 -1.38 ± 0.53 |
8.50 |
.000* |
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).