4.1. Characteristics of Motions
All participants demonstrated success with the M1-S1 motion. Furthermore, preparatory poses performed before motion jumps in the first series were excluded from the evaluation because they were unique to that series. Therefore, M1-S1 differs from the first motion of the other series, and, owing to its lack of alignment with the characteristics of the motions within the scope of the evaluation, it was not considered in this study.
M2-S1 was the point of departure for the examination. During the second motion, the upper limb moved laterally from the center of the head to both shoulders on the same side. In a symmetrical human body, when moving the upper and lower limbs on the same side simultaneously, they tend to move in the same direction, based on the body’s midline [
31,
32,
33,
34]. Furthermore, Nakagawa et al. [
35,
36] demonstrated that movements on the same side of the upper and lower limbs in the horizontal plane are more stable and can be performed with greater accuracy than movements on the opposite side. Therefore, for the feet apart or together motion, Task 1 (feet apart motion) exhibited a high success rate owing to the alignment of the upper and lower limb movements in a single direction from the center outwards to both sides. This allows for a more natural and fluid motion. In contrast, Tasks 5 and 6 (feet together) required the lower limbs to move from both sides towards the center of the body in a direction different from that of the upper limbs. However, with regard to the “keeping the feet apart or together” motion, regardless of whether the feet are apart or together, the lower limbs do not move on the same or opposite side as the upper limbs. Consequently, it is postulated that this does not affect the directionality of motion. It is therefore surmised that the reason for Tasks 2–4 of the “keeping feet apart or together” motion also exhibited a high success rate, given that they were maintenance motions. These tasks entailed maintaining the same posture without being affected by directionality.
The following section examines the characteristics of M3-S1. In the third motion, the upper limbs are positioned on both shoulders without any movement. As observed by Meesen et al. [
37], when engaging in motor coordination exercises targeting the upper and lower limbs, there is a proclivity to move both the upper and lower limbs simultaneously. In other words, when the lower limbs are moved in a feet-apart or together motion, the upper limbs, which are not permitted to move, are likely to move in conjunction with the lower limbs. It can be posited that the high success rates observed for Tasks 1, 2, and 5 may be attributed to the nature of the motion involved, namely, keeping the feet apart or together. This is due to the fact that both the upper and lower limbs are kept stationary (with the exception of the jump), thereby reducing the potential for interference from other movements. Conversely, the low success rates observed for Tasks 3, 4, and 6 may be attributed to the nature of the motion involved, namely, keeping the feet apart. This is due to the fact that the aforementioned tasks involve a greater degree of movement, which may have resulted in a higher level of interference from other movements.
The following section examines the characteristics of M4-S1. In contrast to M2-S1 and M3-S1, no distinction was observed in the success rate between the tasks, irrespective of whether the motion involved the feet being apart or together, or a combination of the two. In the fourth motion, the upper limbs exhibited a pause in the middle of the jump, with the hands positioned on the shoulders on the opposite side. At the time of landing, the upper limbs were required to perform two motions: contralateral and lateral. It is essential that the motion is executed at a faster pace than that of the lower body. Thus, it is postulated that the rhythms of the upper and lower limbs are distinct. Kobayashi et al. [
38] indicated that when a rhythm mismatch occurs, motivation tends to decline if the rhythm and motion are not synchronized. In other words, the alteration in rhythm observed in the fourth motion may have evoked a sense of inadequacy in the participants, potentially leading to a disruption in the coordination between the upper and lower limbs. Therefore, the fourth motion presents a challenge owing to the upper limb motion regardless of the combination of lower limb motions. In other words, the fourth motion was inherently challenging. Nevertheless, it is postulated that the complexity of the lower limb motions does not influence the overall difficulty of the task, as each has a comparable level of difficulty.
Finally, an examination of M1-S2 is warranted. M1-S2 involves the movement of both hands from the same shoulder to the center of the head in an inward direction, which is the opposite of M2-S1. It is postulated that M1-S2 has an inverse relationship in terms of difficulty to M2-S1 in terms of the direction of the upper and lower limbs. Therefore, for Tasks 1 and 3 with the feet together, the lower limb motion involves closing the feet from a separate position towards the center of the body. As this is in the same direction as the upper limb (towards the inside), it has an opposite relationship to Task 1 of the second motion (the upper and lower limbs both move in an outward direction). This finding is consistent with those of the previous studies. As demonstrated by Semjen [
31], Carson et al. [
32], Byblow et al. [
33], Temprado et al.[
34], Nakagawa et al.[
35], and Nakagawa et al. [
36], the motion is executed with greater stability and accuracy, resulting in a high success rate. In contrast, Task 2 involved the motion of the feet apart. Although there was no significant difference in the success rate between Tasks 1, 3, and 4, the success rates remained below 90%. This discrepancy could be attributed to the fact that the lower limbs undergo lateral motion, whereas the upper limbs exhibit different directional movements. However, considering the aforementioned observations regarding the motion of keeping the feet apart or together, the success rate for Task 4, which involved keeping the feet together, was notably high, exceeding 90%. Conversely, the success rates for Tasks 5 and 6, which entailed keeping the feet apart, were markedly low at approximately 30%. In other words, the assumption that the direction of motion would remain unaffected due to its lack of correlation with the direction of the upper limbs, regardless of whether the feet were kept together or apart, was deemed erroneous.
In the case of the “keeping the feet apart” motion, the lower limbs are in fact kept apart, resulting in a conscious difference in the directionality of the upper limbs. It has been postulated that the lower limbs may be close together when they move inward. In contrast, keeping the feet together involves conscious effort to maintain the lower limbs in a fixed position, allowing the upper limbs to move inward without affecting the perception of ipsilateral movement. This explains the high success rate observed in this study. Muraoka et al. [
39] suggested that psychological factors might influence limb movement patterns. If this new proposal is indeed more accurate, then in –M2-S1, for Task 2 of the “keeping the feet apart” motion, the lower limbs were aware that they were in a separate position, and because they were in the same direction as the upper limbs, they could be treated as ipsilateral motions, which is why the success rate was high. In contrast, the success rates for Tasks 3 and 4 of –M2-S1 should have been low given that the upper and lower limbs were perceived as contralateral movements. However, the observed success rate was high and this was regarded as an exceptional case within the context of this study. Thus, it can be inferred that in –M2-S1 and M1-S2, there was also an awareness of the same-side or contralateral movements of the upper and lower limbs in the feet apart and together. If the upper and lower limbs consciously move to the same side, the success rate is high. Conversely, if the upper and lower limbs consciously moved to the opposite sides, the success rate was low.
In conclusion, it can be posited that the reasons affecting the difficulty of the four motions used in this experiment are similar to those affecting the difficulty of new motions. These reasons pertain to the ability to maintain consistency between the upper and lower limbs, either in motion or conscious judgment. Therefore, achieving consistency is the key to coordinating the upper and lower limbs.
4.2. The Difficulty and Characteristics of the Six Tasks
Regarding the difficulty and characteristics of the six tasks used in this study, irrespective of experience, the three evaluation indices for Tasks 1 and 2 exhibited higher values than those observed for the remaining four tasks. With the exception of the fourth motion, which was not considered as a factor influencing task difficulty owing to the aforementioned considerations, there were no motions in Task 1 that could be classified as difficult. In Task 2, only the first motion was identified as slightly challenging. Prior research on attention capacity has indicated that as conditions become more complex, greater attention is required, thereby making it more challenging to automate motor control [
40,
41,
42,
43]. In other words, as the difficulty of the motions increased, the success rate of the task decreased and the difficulty level increased. Thus, it can be inferred that Tasks 1 and 2 are relatively straightforward and require minimal attention, making them amenable to automation. By contrast, Task 2 is also relatively straightforward to automate, although one moderately challenging motion does exist, which leads to the conclusion that it can be distinguished from Task 1.
Subsequently, Tasks 3–6 were examined. A notable discrepancy was observed between Tasks 3, 4, 5, and 6 in terms of the success rate of at least one series. However, the group of inexperienced participants who successfully completed the four series in Tasks 5 and 6 were not included in this analysis. With regard to challenging motions (with the exception of the fourth motion), only the third proved to be problematic in Tasks 3 and 4. However, in Task 5, two motions (first and second) were identified as difficult, whereas in Task 6, all three motions (first, second, and third) were classified as challenging. Therefore, Tasks 3 and 4 require a degree of attention for motor control automation. Given the near-equivalent difficulty levels of Tasks 3 and 4, it can be inferred that the same evaluation effect can be obtained by performing either task similarly. Nevertheless, it is recommended that this task be performed as an alternative or a reserve task. Tasks 5 and 6 are relatively demanding in terms of the amount of attention required for motor control automation with two or more difficult motions. Task 6 was assumed to require the greatest degree of attention.
Furthermore, the difficulty of the tasks was influenced by the number of challenging motions, as evidenced by the six tasks included in this study and the observations made during the development of other tasks. In other words, it was also inferred that the evaluation method proposed in this study allows for the free combination and adjustment of the series composition and difficulty levels according to the characteristics and difficulty of the motions involved.
4.3. Validity of the Test
A comparison of the results obtained from the group of inexperienced participants with those from the group of experienced participants revealed that the latter demonstrated superior performance across all three evaluation indices. A substantial body of research has demonstrated that motor coordination can be enhanced through targeted training [
44,
45,
46,
47]. In other words, it can be stated that the group of experienced participants who received motor coordination training on a weekly basis exhibited a higher skill level than the group of inexperienced participants who did not receive such training. It can be reasonably predicted that those with higher scores will perform better. In other words, the results obtained in this study align with the anticipated outcomes. Accordingly, this study proposed a task that could reflect the strength and weakness of the motor coordination skills of the upper and lower limbs. Furthermore, it can reflect the skill level not only in terms of experience but also within the same group. This is considered a valid evaluation method.
Furthermore, among the experienced participants, even with the stringent evaluation index of “success for the four series”, there were participants who demonstrated success in all six tasks. However, this was not observed in the inexperienced participants. Notably, none of the participants in the inexperienced group succeeded in Tasks 5 and 6. With the evaluation index of “success in at least one series”, half the experienced group succeeded in all six tasks. In contrast, the inexperienced group demonstrated a lower success rate even when the criterion was relatively relaxed. With regard to the number of successful series, the group of inexperienced participants exhibited the highest level of success, with five out of the 24 series completed successfully. The group of experienced participants exhibited the highest success rate, with 12 completed by most participants. Moreover, in Tasks 1 and 2, the vast majority of participants in the experienced group achieved success, whereas less than 50% of those in the inexperienced group were able to do so. Wulf et al. [
29] highlighted that the difficulty of a task is relative, with participants with relatively high skill levels tending to find even difficult tasks to be relatively straightforward. In other words, for the inexperienced participants, Tasks 5 and 6 were particularly challenging, and it may be preferable to exclude them from the measurement process. In contrast, the experienced participants found Tasks 1 and 2 to be exceedingly simple, suggesting that they may be less suitable as test tasks and that more challenging options should be employed instead.
Furthermore, the test can be conducted with minimal effort, requiring approximately 10 minutes from the initial explanation to the conclusion of the assessment, with no specialized equipment. Considering these findings, the evaluation method proposed in this study is a useful and effective approach for accurately assessing upper- and lower-limb motor coordination skills. It offers a high degree of flexibility in terms of setting and can be easily implemented, making it a valuable addition to existing assessment tools.