2. Materials and Methods
Participants
The study’s sample consisted of one hundred (101) healthy adults, aged from 20 to 49 years old (40% male and 60% female). The mean age was 32.21 years with mean education of 14.98 years of schooling (
Table 1). Participants were recruited using the simple random sampling from the wider areas of North and South Greece; including towns, villages and islands which belonged to these areas. In more detail, participants were university students from the University of Western Macedonia, and Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, as well as other people from the aforementioned places of the country. Participants were enrolled in this study voluntarily, anonymously and after giving their written consent. Before their inclusion to the study, participants read the information sheet which stipulated that the research team could use their data for research purposes. In order to extract the Greek norms, all participants were divided into three age classes, a typical separation in normative data studies (age range 20–29; 30–39, and 40–49 years), two gender classes (men and women) and three educational classes (Secondary school final year and graduates: 10-12 schooling years, Diploma degree - University students - Bachelor degree: 13-16 years, and Master Degree - Doctorate studies: 16< years.
Study’s inclusion criteria were the following: age from 20 to 49 years and Greek as their native language. The exclusion criteria were the following; presence of previous addictive disorder, psychosis and major depression, concurrent history of neurologic disease known to affect cognitive functioning, auditory functioning no sufficient to understand normal conversational speech, and visual acuity non-normal or non-corrected to anticipate visual stimuli.
Description of the D-KEFS Tests
CWIT
CWIT
D-KEFS is the D-KEFS (Delis et al., 2001) [
42] version of the Stroop Color and the Word Test (SCWT; Stroop, 1935), and therefore, it constitutes its newer version. Stroop test includes three separate conditions; Color naming speed, colors and words reading printed in black ink, and verbal responses’ inhibition through colors naming written in discordant ink. A fourth trial added to the CWIT
D-KEFS is named Inhibition/Switching condition, which requires to switch between color naming and word reading of color words printed in incongruent colors. Furthermore, the D-KEFS version appears to use the Comalli variant [
60], in which color naming precedes word reading. To sum, CWIT conditions are the following; Color naming, Word reading, Inhibition, and Inhibition/Switching.
The CWIT
D-KEFS measures inhibition/switching skills as cognitive flexibility, whereas provide the ability to assess participants’ perseverance and tendency to act towards impulsive and unplanned responses in verbal modality. In more details, in the first condition the examinee is asked to name the colors he/she sees on the form (red, blue, green), hence, it measures naming speed in high frequency repeating stimuli, e.g color patches, as well as word- finding difficulty. The second condition requires reading the color words, printed in black ink, and therefore is measures the ability to read high-frequency repeated. The third condition involves color naming, where the word and the color in which it is printed are incompatible with each other (e.g., the word "green" is printed in red ink), and thus the examinee must name the color that it is printed and to inhibit his/her automatic tendency that prompts them to read the word. Therefore, the examinee is required to name colors when inhibiting the more automatic task of reading the words. Lower performance in condition 3 is associated to deficits in verbal inhibition over the deficiency in naming speed. However, unlike other versions of this test, in the CWIT
D-KEFS there is also a fourth condition (the Inhibit/Switching condition), in which the examinee is asked to alternate at irregular intervals between reading color-words and naming color-words printed with discordant color [
20]. Successful performance on this condition prerequisites naming speed, reading speed, cognitive flexibility and verbal inhibition. This provides an extra requirement not only to inhibit the tendency to read, but also to switch between different conditions through cognitive shifting [
22]. In case that the examinee performs well in conditions 1-3, but fails on condition 4 it implies deficits in cognitive flexibility, whereas problematic performance in both conditions 3 and 4 suggests impairments in verbal inhibition and cognitive flexibility. For each of the four conditions, the main score is based on the time required to complete the test. In addition, the number of uncorrected and self-corrected errors as well as the number of total errors in each condition is also measured. Finally, three primary contrast variables were also calculated; Inhibition versus Color naming, Inhibition/Switching versus combined naming plus reading and Inhibition/Switching versus Inhibition, by subtracting the completion time scaled score for one or more component tasks from the completion time scaled score for one of the higher-level tasks in order.
Findings show an atypical pattern of performance, where most of the sample (57.1%) scored with faster completion time and/or fewer errors in the fourth condition than in the third, which may be attributed to two main factors. First, Lippa & Davis (2010) [
61] argue that faster performance could be explained because color naming typically takes slightly longer than word reading, and the fourth condition involves only half as much color naming as the third. Second, the occurrence of fewer errors may be attributable to specific learning characteristics, such as increased verbal learning ability or increased semantic verbal fluency, which interacts with the order of test’s administration, i.e., the inhibition condition always precedes the inhibition/switching condition [
61].
Finally, since Delis et al. (2001) [
42] focused on the interpersonal connection and comfort between the examiner and the examinee during the neuropsychological assessment, Barnett et al. (2022) [
62] studied the influence of them on CWIT
D-KEFS performance. Finding showed that lower interaction between the examiner and the examinee was associated with increased completion time of Inhibition condition, however, no differences were observed in Inhibition/Switching in relation to the interaction between them, which is perhaps explained by the fact that the third condition serves as a practical test for the suspension procedure in the Inhibition/Switching condition [
61,
62,
63].
TMT
The TMT
D-KEFS version provides a more detailed evaluation of neurocognitive performance by separating its conditions in control conditions (1, 2, 3, 5) which measure clinically relevant components of visuomotor sequencing skills as well as psychomotor speed, and on the other side, condition 4, which measures higher-order executive skills, for example flexibility, cognitive set-shifting [
64]. The main outcome measure is time-to-completion in seconds across all TMT
D-KEFS conditions. To conclude, TMT
D-KEFS provides conceptual control conditions that provide the opportunity for detailed evaluation of neurocognitive performance by separating psychomotor speed and higher-order executive functions.
In detail, the first condition is named visual search and requires visual scanning. The second condition includes numerical sequence and measures simple visual attention and visual scanning, visual- motor skills, and psychomotor speed. The third condition consists also a visual - motor task and verbal learning task which measures grammatical sequence. The fourth condition measures the number-letter alternation assessing divided attention, cognitive flexibility, set shifting by alternating two different sets of cognitive stimuli [
65]. The fifth condition assesses motor speed [
66]. Conditions 2 and 3 are often used to measure processing speed, while condition 4 is considered an index of cognitive function [
66]. However, according to a previous study [
16], the TMT
D-KEFS condition 4 was the only measure of executive functions which is clearly assumed as a functional ability index. To perform well in the switching tests, the examinee must have visual scanning and motor speed abilities to extract the score data, and the following test may describe problems in the above domains [
64]. Lezak et al. (2012) [
67] argue that TMT is assumed as one of the most highly sensitive tools to detect neurocognitive deficits, and therefore, has an inseparable role in executive functions’ evaluation.
The first four conditions are presented on a page A3, on which were scattered circles with numbers and letters as indicators. In condition 1 of visual scanning, the participant has to delete the number 3 located in random order on the reference sheet among other numbers and letters. In the number sequence condition, the examinee has to draw a single line connecting sequentially in ascending order numbers from 1 to 16, which are in random order on the reference sheet between letters. Correspondingly, in condition 3 of the letter sequence, the examinee has to connect letters in order of the alphabet, from A to P, which are randomly presented among other numbers. In the number-letter alternation condition 4, the participant has to draw a line, alternating the connections between numbers and letters sequentially. In more detail, starting from the number 1 the participant has to draw a line to A, then from A to the number 2, from number 2 to the letter B and so on until he/she reaches the letter P. In the motor speed condition 5, the A3 reference sheet consists of a single dotted line on which empty circles are scattered. The examinee has to draw a line following the dotted line and passing through the circles located along the course (
Table 2). Except for condition 4 which has a maximum time limit of 240 seconds, the remaining conditions have a limit of 150 seconds. In each case, the participant is asked to complete the tests as quickly as possible. If the participant would make an error, the condition was interrupted, saying that an error had been made, and examiner waited for the participant to find the error and continue the condition, without interrupting the stopwatch. Four out of the five basic test conditions can empirically show whether participants' poor performance in the switching conditions is due to higher-level deficits in cognitive flexibility or due to impairments in the underlying skills needed to perform the switching tests, such as motor speed, visual scanning, and sequence of numbers or letters [
48]. For example, poor performance in the alternation test may be due to deficits in visual search and motor control, whereas better performance in the letter-sequence condition may contribute to better performance in the number-letter alternation condition [
66].
Ethics
Consent was obtained from the Ethics Committee of the University of Western Macedonia, to approve the processing of the participants' data. Demographic information, such as age, gender, and education, was collected, adhering to the law of the European Union since 28 May 2018, which allows the use of sensitive personal data for research purposes. Participants were told and consented to that, upon a written request, their data could be removed from the online database. The study was aligned with the principles outlined in the Helsinki Declaration (World Medical Association, 1997).
Procedure
Initially, a pilot study administered to a total of 10 people, most of them were university students of the Psychology Departments, to evaluate CWIT and TMTD-KEFS tests in order to avoid concomitant problems in data collection. Before their inclusion to the study, participants read the information sheet, which mentioned that the researchers could use their clinical data for research purposes. Additionally, they were told that they would be able to withdraw from the study whenever they wanted without facing any consequences or giving any explanation. Before the completion of the consent form, the participants were told that their records would be coded and anonymized for future research purposes. After signing consent to participate in the research, a short structured interview followed to collect demographic information, including the participant's gender, age, and education level. These data were accompanied by a code, which included the initial letters of the participant's name in combination with the number of the series of administration (e.g. PM54), to preserve anonymity, but also to facilitate the identification process of the participants in the statistical database. D-KEFS tests’ administration was conducted in a quiet environment at the University premises, mainly during morning hours, in order to perform better without external interference.
The neuropsychological assessment lasted about half an hour maximum and involved a face-to-face assessment. In specific, the instruction sheet of each condition was presented in front of the participants, before each test’s administration.
Statistical Analyses
At first, a Pearson correlation test was applied for continuous variables, to examine whether there was a correlation between demographic variables with the two D-KEFS tests. Despite that D-KEFS subtests raw scores are otherwise converted to scaled scores having a mean of 10 as well as a standard deviation of 3, in the current analyses we provide only raw scores to identify participants’ performance. Finally, inferential cut off scores were also calculated to select the score under which the possibility for an individual to belong to the normal population was below 10% and therefore would be assumed as low performance. The D-KEFS provide primary and optional variables; in order to provide both global as well as more detail evaluation about participants’ performance. In the current study, only primary measures were used, in agreement with similar studies in literature. The Total Achievement scores attributable to each measure are as follows: CWITD-KEFS (the four conditions measuring Time-to-Completion, Errors and three primary contrast measures; Inhibition versus Color naming, Inhibition/Switching versus combined naming plus reading and Inhibition/Switching versus Inhibition), and TMTD-KEFS (the five conditions measuring Time-to-Completion). It is worth mentioning that performance in CWITD-KEFS is measured not only by completion time on each of the four trials, and the total number of errors on each trial, but also for corrected and uncorrected errors on the inhibition and inhibition/switching trials. However, the present norms were extracted by completion of time in each condition.
An alpha value of .05 (two-tailed) was used. The statistical analyses were performed by the SPSS software v 27 (IBM Corp. Released 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp).
4. Discussion
To our knowledge, there are no norms outside the original American age-adjusted norms presented in the D-KEFS manual by Delis et al. (2001) [
42] available for clinicians and researchers in Greece. Despite some previous versions of Stroop and TMT validated in Greek adult population, this is the first study which measures the effects of demographic variables on CWIT
D-KEFS as well as TMT
D-KEFS performance and provide normative data for the Greek sample. Moreover, there is a lack in literature regarding neuropsychological tests’ normative data studies in the age range between early to middle adulthood, whereas no particular focus was given to the socio-demographic effect in examinees performance. This research gap is supposed to be covered by the current study.
Regarding CWIT
D-KEFS, it was created to improve the Stroop version by including an inhibition/switching trial, which was designed to be more difficult than the inhibition trial by means of completion time, as well as number of errors. Overall, the results of the current study suggest that the CWIT
D-KEFS can be regarded as a measure of performance in processing speed/executive functioning, however, till now there are no studies comparing the CWIT with the old Stroop test. The results of the current study showed that age was a predictive factor of CWIT
D-KEFS performance across its conditions, also in agreement with previous studies from Lippa and Davis (2010) [
61], Zhao et al. (2020) [
43] and Espenes et al. (2024) [
69]. In fact, the three age classes of the sample differed by means of their performance in CWIT
D-KEFS mainly regarding the last two conditions. Based on Pearson test, age classes differed statistically significantly in condition 3 (r = .489, n = 100, p < .001), 4 (r = .514, n = 100, p < .001) and 2 (r = .219, n = 100, p < .005). Additionally, regarding condition 2, it is worth mentioning that the average and upper percentile scores among the three age classes were identical with subtle differences, whereas those between 30-39 years old had a slightly better performance followed by those between 20-29, and those between 40-49 years old. However, this slightly lower performance of younger counterparts compared to adults who belong to the 30-39 age group does not necessarily mean lower executive functioning, because various factors, such as fatigue, low motivation or reduced attention could affect their performance [
70].
Kurniadi et al. (2021) [
20], found moderate correlation between education with CWIT
D-KEFS performance in condition 3 (r = .212, n = 101, p < .005) and condition 4 (r = .319, n = 100, p < .001), whereas Karr et al. (2018; 2019 [
71,
72]) found that performance in the D-KEFS was proportional to participants' level of education, which is in contrast to the results of the current study. Finally, regarding gender, no statistically significant correlation with performance was detected, which is confirmed by the study of Cutler et al. (2023) [
24], in which the effect of gender did not play a significant role in the performance of the sample across all test’s conditions. Furthermore, in the longitudinal study by Adólfsdóttir et al. (2017) [
18], a marginal correlation in performance was detected, as for the suspension condition they predicted that completion time increases per year by 0.61% for men and by 0.62% for women, while for the suspension condition
/shift, the rates were 0.72% for men and 0.70% for women.
The primary variable of the CWIT
D-KEFS is completion time, errors and the three contrast measures mentioned previously. Therefore norms -at this stage of our study- were calculate only for them, because they are assumed as primary scores. Norms for the optional scores, that include the Inhibition/Switching versus Color naming as well as Inhibition/Switching versus Word reading using their scaled scores, were not included in the current study. It is worth mentioning that in the first two conditions total errors were almost zero for all age groups without exception. Then, in the condition 3, a limited number of errors were observed, most of which were self-corrected. This finding is probably related to the increase in completion time, relative to the previous baseline conditions, as correcting existing errors requires the participant to delay. Finally, in condition 4, the number of errors was twice as high as in condition 3, while the mean of uncorrected errors and self-corrections appeared almost equal, regardless of age. In fact, this finding agrees with the study of Lippa & Davis (2010) [
61], where it was found that basically in people aged from 14 to 69 years, the average of errors was found greater in the condition 4 than in 3. However, it contrasts with the study of Barnett et al. (2022) [
62], because they argue that condition 3 is assumed to function as a practice test for condition 4 that requires inhibitory control and switching. In general, as the last two conditions are considered more complex, it was observed that the participants largely seemed to sacrifice more time in completing them, trying to avoid mistakes.
The inhibition/switching trial was designed to be the most difficult as regards completion time and the number of errors. Of particular interest is the study by Lippa & Davis (2010) [
61], which investigates the complexity of the fourth condition compared to the third in adult population with an average level of 14.8 years of schooling and a diagnosis of either neurological or psychiatric pathology. Among people between 14-69 years old the mean of errors in inhibition/switching condition is greater compared to the inhibition condition, whereas in those between 8-13 years old and 70-89 years old the errors’ mean score is lower or equal with the ones in inhibition condition [
61]. Moreover, longitudinal study of Adólfsdóttir et al. (2017) [
18] demonstrates age-related changes in performance on inhibition, and combined inhibition and switching in middle-aged and older adults, where these populations have lower performance which appears to persist, even after controlling for baseline measures of processing speed, gender and years of schooling. Finally, norms for the three contrast variables; Inhibition versus Color naming, Inhibition/Switching versus combined naming plus reading and Inhibition/Switching versus Inhibition were shown to be around 0 across all age groups, which is in line with the American norms.
A previous normative data study for the Stroop version called as Trenerry’s Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test (SNST) was conducted by Zalonis et al. (2009a) [
45] in Greek adult population between 18-84 years and education range 6-18 years of schooling. On the contrary to our results, their findings suggest that both age and education significantly contributed to SNST performance. However, no direct comparisons can be made between our findings with the study of Zalonis et al. (2009a) [
45], because they included a broader sample in terms of age and education, which profoundly influenced examinees’ performance. Additionally, between those with 20-29 years old in the sample of this study, no one had less than 13 years of education, something that seems to be representative of the educational level of young adults in Greece according to the official laws of the government. Nevertheless, according to their findings, age appeared to be the most predictive factor of SNST performance, compared to education. Their study calculated four variables: Color task; the time needed to read the 112 items, and in the Color–Word task; the number of errors; the number of self-corrections; and the interference score, were calculated by subtracting the number of errors from the total number of items completed in 120s. On the contrary, in our study we followed a totally different scoring method by measuring the completion time across the four conditions.
As regards the effects of demographics on TMT
D-KEFS , it is worth mentioning that most studies used the traditional version of the test, so despite that useful comparisons can be made, this controversy could be a limitation [
52,
73]. More specifically, previous studies which used the traditional version of the test in different populations show that TMT performance appeared to be related to age and years of schooling [
49,
50,
52,
54,
74], which is also confirmed by the findings of the current study. Except from the study of Fine et al. (2011) [
48], the remaining studies referred to the traditional version of the test, showed that age and level of education significantly related with TMT execution time [
49,
50,
54,
74]. A possible explanation could be that typically, motor speed gradually declines with age [
53], which is also in agreement with the study of Cavaco et al. (2013) [
52], who found that the performance becomes better as the level of education increases. Finally, Fine et al. (2011) [
48] showed that the effect of higher educational level is stronger mainly in the conditions of letter sequence and number-letter switching, which is also confirmed by the results of the current study. Although many studies found that overall age has a greater impact on the overall test performance than education [
53], in our study we found that across the three age classes, those with 16+ years of schooling did better than their peers with a lower level of education, indicating the improvement in motor speed in those with a higher level of education, which is also confirmed by some previous studies [
54].
Regarding gender, no differences were found by means of TMT
D-KEFS performance in the current study. According to literature review, previous studies observed differences between men and women. In specific, Cavaco et al. (2013) [
52] found differences in the number sequencing condition, while study by Cangoz et al. (2009) [
73] reported a relationship between gender and TMT conditions. However, the study of Cangoz et al. (2009) [
73] was conducted in people aged over 50, so no clear comparisons can be made. Heterogeneity of results by means of gender may be attributed to uncontrolled or unmeasured factors, such as sample’s characteristics, differences in men and women by means of educational level and/or cultural differences [
75].
Finally, according to the findings mentioned above, it seems that age and educational level may be predictive factors of TMT
D-KEFS, because process speed declines with age [
52,
54] and increases with years of schooling [
76]. These results are supported by theories about cognitive reserve which stress the protective role of education, among other factors, in cognitive decline, even in fluid intelligence aspects [
77,
78].
To compare the results of the current study with the first normative data study in Greek adult population by Zalonis et al (2009) [
45], it was found that in their study they followed a totally different age clustering, because they divided their sample in 12 different groups; (16–19, 20–29, 20–40, 25–45, 30–50, 35–55, 40–60, 45–65, 50–70, 55–75, 60–80, 65–85) and three different from ours educational levels according to the number of years of schooling (<9, 10–12, 13<). At this point it must be noted that due to the absence of participants in the age group of 20-29 with educational level less than 10-12 years of schooling, the following category was not included in the normative tables. However, it is worth noting that the normative data scores in number sequence and switching conditions, which are common between the two TMT versions, in the age class who had 10-12 years of schooling was almost the same. Finally, given that age classes between the two studies are very different, comparing them is vague and insufficient.
Comparing Greek norms of CWIT with the American ones stratified also by age, it was found that our mean scores belonged to the same range in terms of scaled scores which is equivalent to the mean American score of 10 or 1 SD above, which is acceptable according to what was previously mentioned. Hence, despite that Greek adults had approximately slightly lower time of completion in some conditions, compared to Americans, this evidence can be attributed to higher educational levels across all age classes. Moving to the TMT
D-KEFS, Greek norms were equivalent to Americans, however, in some conditions Greek adults scored 1 SD higher compared to the American sample, which can be also explained by increased years of schooling. Finally, it can be assumed that CWIT and TMT norms for Greek adult population are equivalent to the original norms calculated by Delis et al. (2001) [
42], hence they can be used by health professionals and researchers.
Table 1.
Demographic distribution of the sample.
Table 1.
Demographic distribution of the sample.
|
Age groups |
Total |
Years of education |
Age group 20-29 |
Age group 30-39 |
Age group 40-49 |
|
10-12 |
|
15 |
15 |
30 |
13-16 |
19 |
11 |
10 |
40 |
16+ |
11 |
11 |
10 |
31 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Table 2.
Ratio of letters between the English and Greek alphabets for conditions 1-4 of TMT.
Table 2.
Ratio of letters between the English and Greek alphabets for conditions 1-4 of TMT.
Numbers |
|
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
10 |
11 |
12 |
13 |
14 |
15 |
16 |
Letters |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
English |
A |
B |
C |
D |
E |
F |
G |
H |
I |
J |
K |
L |
M |
N |
O |
P |
Greek |
A |
Β |
Γ |
Δ |
Ε |
Ζ |
H |
Θ |
Ι |
Κ |
Λ |
Μ |
Ν |
Ξ |
O |
Π |
Table 3.
Norms for the Trail Making Test (TMT) in conditions 1 to 5 in adults aged 20-29 years old.
Table 3.
Norms for the Trail Making Test (TMT) in conditions 1 to 5 in adults aged 20-29 years old.
|
ΤΜΤ1 |
ΤΜΤ2 |
ΤΜΤ3 |
ΤΜΤ4 |
ΤΜΤ5 |
TMT1 |
TMT2 |
TMT3 |
TMT4 |
TMT5 |
%ile |
13-16 years of education (Education-Class 2) |
16< years of education (Education-Class 3) |
95 |
12.25 |
17.60 |
18.30 |
42.95 |
16.85 |
17.00 |
21.00 |
25.00 |
50.00 |
20.00 |
90 |
13.00 |
19.30 |
20.60 |
46.60 |
21.30 |
17.00 |
21.00 |
25.00 |
50.00 |
20.00 |
75 |
15.00 |
20.25 |
25.25 |
57.00 |
28.25 |
17.75 |
22.50 |
25.75 |
51.50 |
20.75 |
50 |
19.50 |
25.00 |
29.50 |
68.50 |
36.00 |
21.00 |
27.00 |
27.50 |
59.50 |
25.00 |
25 |
24.00 |
29.00 |
34.75 |
73.75 |
45.50 |
22.25 |
28.00 |
33.25 |
72.00 |
29.25 |
10 |
29.80 |
34.10 |
39.70 |
93.40 |
49.70 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
- |
M. |
20.47 |
25.31 |
30.34 |
67.94 |
36.00 |
20.33 |
25.67 |
28.83 |
61.17 |
25.00 |
S.D. |
6.77 |
5.49 |
8.06 |
15.89 |
11.70 |
2.42 |
2.94 |
3.86 |
10.02 |
4.05 |
Table 4.
Norms for the Trail Making Test (TMT) in conditions 1 to 5 in adults aged 30-39 years old.
Table 4.
Norms for the Trail Making Test (TMT) in conditions 1 to 5 in adults aged 30-39 years old.
|
TM1 |
TM2 |
TM3 |
TM4 |
TM5 |
TM1 |
TM2 |
TM3 |
TM4 |
TM5 |
TM1 |
TM2 |
TM3 |
TM4 |
TM5 |
%ile |
10-12 years of education (Education-Class 1) |
13-16 years of education (Education-Class 2) |
16< (Education-Class 3) |
95 |
20.00 |
19.00 |
27.00 |
65.00 |
31.00 |
16.00 |
15.00 |
19.00 |
53.00 |
20.00 |
15.00 |
14.00 |
18.00 |
45.00 |
16.00 |
90 |
20.00 |
19.00 |
27.00 |
65.00 |
31.00 |
16.00 |
15.70 |
19.60 |
54.80 |
25.40 |
15.80 |
15.60 |
18.40 |
47.80 |
17.20 |
75 |
20.00 |
19.00 |
27.00 |
65.00 |
31.00 |
17.00 |
22.00 |
23.00 |
61.00 |
35.00 |
17.00 |
19.00 |
23.00 |
55.50 |
24.50 |
50 |
23.00 |
27.00 |
30.00 |
103.00 |
40.00 |
19.00 |
27.00 |
33.00 |
69.00 |
39.00 |
20.00 |
23.00 |
26.00 |
60.00 |
33.00 |
25 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
- |
23.00 |
40.00 |
51.00 |
80.00 |
45.00 |
25.50 |
29.50 |
31.50 |
74.00 |
45.00 |
10 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
- |
27.20 |
46.60 |
63.60 |
140.0 |
64.00 |
27.60 |
32.80 |
38.60 |
87.60 |
54.40 |
Mean |
23.00 |
27.00 |
29.33 |
91.67 |
43.33 |
20.00 |
29.53 |
37.67 |
79.20 |
40.80 |
21.38 |
24.15 |
27.00 |
64.77 |
34.54 |
S.D. |
2.12 |
8.00 |
2.08 |
23.18 |
14.29 |
3.91 |
11.09 |
15.71 |
29.15 |
11.74 |
4.53 |
5.95 |
6.50 |
13.31 |
13.54 |
Table 5.
Norms for the Trail Making Test (TMT) in conditions 1 to 5 in adults aged 40-49 years old.
Table 5.
Norms for the Trail Making Test (TMT) in conditions 1 to 5 in adults aged 40-49 years old.
|
TM1 |
TM2 |
TM3 |
TM4 |
TM5 |
TM1 |
TM2 |
TM3 |
TM4 |
TM5 |
TM1 |
TM2 |
TM3 |
TM4 |
TM5 |
%ile |
10-12 years of education (Education-Class 1) |
13-16 years of education (Education-Class 2) |
16< (Education-Class 3) |
95 |
15.00 |
18.00 |
20.00 |
40.00 |
27.00 |
14.00 |
22.00 |
23.00 |
51.00 |
14.00 |
18.00 |
23.00 |
27.00 |
69.00 |
24.00 |
90 |
15.00 |
18.00 |
20.00 |
40.00 |
27.00 |
14.40 |
22.40 |
25.00 |
55.80 |
15.20 |
18.00 |
23.00 |
27.00 |
69.00 |
24.00 |
75 |
20.00 |
24.00 |
26.50 |
84.00 |
33.50 |
17.50 |
24.00 |
28.50 |
70.00 |
29.50 |
19.00 |
24.00 |
29.50 |
75.75 |
31.50 |
50 |
23.00 |
30.00 |
42.00 |
107.0 |
45.00 |
19.00 |
29.00 |
33.00 |
73.00 |
40.00 |
20.50 |
27.50 |
34.50 |
87.00 |
38.50 |
25 |
27.50 |
39.00 |
44.00 |
138.0 |
71.00 |
23.50 |
34.00 |
40.00 |
84.50 |
48.50 |
24.25 |
32.25 |
48.25 |
97.50 |
42.25 |
10 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
- |
28.80 |
39.20 |
53.00 |
133.8 |
69.00 |
- |
- |
- |
- |
- |
Mean |
23.78 |
31.78 |
36.22 |
106.6 |
50.89 |
20.38 |
29.69 |
35.23 |
80.23 |
40.08 |
21.63 |
27.88 |
38.25 |
86.63 |
37.75 |
S.D. |
6.07 |
10.15 |
10.37 |
36.25 |
20.15 |
4.646 |
5.83 |
9.38 |
26.84 |
16.36 |
3.70 |
4.19 |
10.75 |
11.55 |
8.36 |
Table 6.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) in conditions 1 to 4 in adults aged 20-49 years old.
Table 6.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) in conditions 1 to 4 in adults aged 20-49 years old.
|
Age group 20-29 |
Age group 30-39 |
Age group 40-49 |
%ile |
CWIT1 |
CWIT2 |
CWIT3 |
CWIT4 |
CWIT1 |
CWIT2 |
CWIT3 |
CWIT4 |
CWIT1 |
CWIT2 |
CWIT3 |
CWIT4 |
95 |
21.00 |
16.05 |
33.05 |
39.05 |
16.10 |
16.10 |
31.65 |
44.00 |
20.55 |
16.55 |
37.75 |
47.75 |
90 |
21.20 |
17.00 |
35.10 |
43.00 |
20.00 |
17.10 |
37.30 |
48.10 |
21.20 |
17.10 |
42.30 |
52.00 |
75 |
24.00 |
18.00 |
37.00 |
47.00 |
24.00 |
19.00 |
44.75 |
52.75 |
24.75 |
19.00 |
49.75 |
56.75 |
50 |
25.00 |
20.00 |
41.00 |
51.50 |
25.00 |
20.00 |
49.50 |
60.00 |
25.00 |
21.00 |
56.00 |
64.00 |
25 |
29.75 |
22.00 |
50.00 |
57.00 |
29.00 |
21.25 |
57.75 |
68.25 |
30.00 |
23.25 |
62.00 |
70.00 |
10 |
30.90 |
23.90 |
59.80 |
63.60 |
30.90 |
22.90 |
60.90 |
80.00 |
31.90 |
26.00 |
65.00 |
80.00 |
Μ. |
26.28 |
20.15 |
44.25 |
52 |
25.70 |
20.13 |
50.27 |
60.77 |
26.43 |
21.40 |
55.43 |
64.47 |
S.D |
3.78 |
2.66 |
9.44 |
8 |
4.24 |
2 |
9 |
10.45 |
3.67 |
3.26 |
8.44 |
9.89 |
Table 7.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT1 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
Table 7.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT1 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
|
Age group 20-29 |
Age group 30-39 |
Age group 40-49 |
%ile |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
95 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
90 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.90 |
00.90 |
00.00 |
00.90 |
00.90 |
75 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
50 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
25 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
10 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.90 |
00.90 |
00.00 |
00.90 |
00.90 |
Μ |
00.00 |
00.05 |
00.05 |
00.00 |
00.10 |
00.10 |
00.00 |
00.10 |
00.10 |
S.D |
00.00 |
00.22 |
00.22 |
00.00 |
00.30 |
00.30 |
00.00 |
00.30 |
00.30 |
Table 8.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT2 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
Table 8.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT2 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
|
Age group 20-29 |
Age group 30-39 |
Age group 40-49 |
%ile |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
95 |
00.00 |
00.45 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
90 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
75 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
50 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
25 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
10 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
Μ |
00.03 |
00.00 |
00.03 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.03 |
00.03 |
S.D |
00.15 |
00.00 |
00.15 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.00 |
00.18 |
00.18 |
Table 9.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT3 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
Table 9.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT3 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
|
Age group 20-29 |
Age group 30-39 |
Age group 40-49 |
%ile |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
95 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
90 |
0.90 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
75 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
50 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
0.00 |
1.25 |
2.25 |
25 |
0.00 |
2.90 |
3.00 |
0.00 |
2.00 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
3.00 |
3.00 |
10 |
1.00 |
3.00 |
3.95 |
0.45 |
2.00 |
0.00 |
2.45 |
3.00 |
3.45 |
M |
0.10 |
0.73 |
0.83 |
0.03 |
0.73 |
0.77 |
0.23 |
0.93 |
1.17 |
S.D |
0.30 |
1.08 |
1.23 |
0.18 |
0.69 |
0.67 |
0.67 |
1.04 |
1.28 |
Table 10.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT4 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
Table 10.
Norms for the Color-Word Interference (CWIT) for the CWIT4 in Errors (Total score, Corrected and Non Corrected) in adults aged 20-49 years old.
|
Age group 20-29 |
Age group 30-39 |
Age group 40-49 |
%ile |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
Uncorr |
Corrected |
Total |
95 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
90 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.10 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
75 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
50 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
2.00 |
0.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
1.00 |
2.00 |
25 |
1.00 |
2.00 |
3.00 |
1.00 |
2.00 |
3.00 |
2.00 |
2.00 |
4.00 |
10 |
2.90 |
3.00 |
4.00 |
1.00 |
3.00 |
3.90 |
3.00 |
3.00 |
5.90 |
M |
0.78 |
1.40 |
2.15 |
0.40 |
1.13 |
1.53 |
1.27 |
1.27 |
2.53 |
S.D |
1.09 |
1.00 |
1.47 |
0.56 |
1.13 |
1.38 |
1.28 |
1.11 |
1.97 |
Table 11.
Norms for the Composite Scaled-Score Equivalents of Scaled Scores by All age Groups.
Table 11.
Norms for the Composite Scaled-Score Equivalents of Scaled Scores by All age Groups.
%ile |
Inhibition vs. Color Naming |
Inhibition/ Switching vs Combined Naming + Reading |
Inhibition/Switching vs. Inhibition |
95 |
3.00 |
2.00 |
2.00 |
90 |
3.00 |
1.00 |
2.00 |
75 |
2.00 |
1.00 |
0.75 |
50 |
0.00 |
-1.00 |
-1.00 |
25 |
-2.00 |
-2.00 |
-2.00 |
10 |
-3.00 |
-4.00 |
-3.00 |
M |
-0.21 |
-0.98 |
-0.56 |
S.D |
2.32 |
2.09 |
1.88 |