1. Introduction
1.1. Sustainability Goals and Search for Alternative Protein Sources
It is no mystery: the Earth's resources are being depleted to accommodate the demanding and unsustainable food consumption patterns of an ever-growing population. According to some authors [
1] at present, none of the World's countries has the means to meet both the growing demand for food (especially protein sources) and to ensure the protection of natural environments.
Modern agricultural and livestock practices, which are necessary for food production on a global scale, are associated with environmental issues related to over-exploitation of land and excessive water and carbon footprints [
2]. Then, making projections to the future, experts argue that humans are already well outside what is called a “safe operating space” in relation to biophysical processes [
3,
4]. However, this space is not only important for ensuring sustainability goals, but also health goals for the living things that inhabit these environments, including humans themselves [
1]. The attempt to get close to these goals needs taking effective actions that require lower-impact and more sustainable diets, reducing consumption of animal products or using alternative protein sources [
5].
In the last decades, more and more methodologies (such as genetic engineering or in vitro meat production), or alternative foods have been developed, including insects. Insects have been a part of the human diet for thousands of years [
6]. According to recent estimates, more than 2100 species of insects are consumed in about 140 countries. The countries with higher consumption are found in Southeastern Asia, America, and Africa [
7]. In some central areas of the African continent entomophagy represents more than 50% of protein needs [
8].
In recent years entomophagy has attracted particular interest [
9], both because of the curiosity it generates [
10], but also because insect products typically have a good nutritional profile [
11] allowing at the same time to recycle organic matter [
12] which is used for their sustenance. In addition, the production process has a much lower environmental impact than other sources of animal protein, in terms of greenhouse emissions, land use, and water footprints [
13]. However, although these products may be promising alternatives in terms of nutrition and environmental impact, their consumption remains limited, particularly in Western countries, despite the fact that some ingredients of common products have long been insect products (see, for example, the use of cochineal as a coloring agent for some alcoholic and non-alcoholic beverages and drugs) [
14].
This skepticism is even more pronounced in the older segments of the population, who are more reluctant to try new foods [
15,
16,
17], but also to give up taste in favor of sustainability or health motives [
18,
19].
For this reason, a change in the attitudes of older segments of the population seems more unfeasible, while it could be important to investigate the opinions and reactions of the younger segments, who are more open to ethnic and new foods [
20,
21] and could represent the most likely consumers of IBF products.
1.2. Attitudes towards IBF in Western Countries
An important contribution of psychological research to entomophagy has been to investigate the relationship between psychological factors and attitudes towards IBF. Previous studies have highlighted the role of emotional/affective variables over the development of negative attitudes, the most significant being disgust [
22], food neophobia [
23,
24,
25], and risk-perception [
26]. The idea of eating insects is generally accompanied by thoughts of uncertainty and fear of unhygienic food with possible negative consequences such as disease transmitting [
27,
28]. In their qualitative/quantitative work, Junges and colleagues [
29] found that consumers with an unfavorable attitude toward insect-based foods were characterized by high food neophobia scores and suspiciousness toward these novel foods.
Previous evidence [
30] revealed that, when it comes to food-related decisions, people tend to trust those perceived as competent to make decisions for them, especially when they know little about a new technology [
31]. Moreover, it has been revealed [
32,
33] that the trust factor toward experts or groups is a predictor of good consumer disposition toward new technologies. For example, it has been shown that trust in science and scientists can play a role in forming attitudes, but research is limited to other sustainable proteins such as cultured meat [
34] or alternative production techniques, such as genetically modified organism (GMOs) [
32]. No previous studies explored the role of trust towards IBF consumption.
1.3. Explicit Attitudes and Automatic Associations: Insights to IBF
Attitudes toward a particular product are an important psychological factor to consider when investigating the intention to try that product. The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) [
35] has extensively demonstrated this link and has been successfully applied to understand entomophagy-related behaviors. Indeed, prior research suggested that attitudes predict consumers' intention to try [
36,
37] or buy IBF [
38], highlighting how negative attitudes can predict avoidant behaviors.
Attitudes are traditionally measured by the so-called direct, or explicit, measures in which participants are asked to make judgments about a topic [
39]. This method, however, only partially provides the person's conscious point of view, who may be fully or partially unaware of their attitudes or may have an ambivalent position or be influenced by perceived expectations of the research [
39,
40].
In fact, previous psychological research already proved that attitudes are not always known or readily available, providing extensive and solid evidence that we do not always use analytical, rational, and conscious strategies when making a decision. Several theoretical models postulating a dual process in decision-making have been formulated in the field of cognitive and social psychology. These, which vary in their theoretical framing and constructs are united by the idea that there are two ways in which people make decisions, a more reflective/rational one that requires more time to process information, and a more impulsive and quicker one. For example, Strack and Deutsch [
41] proposed a model that distinguishes between a reflective and an impulsive system. In the former, decision-making is mediated by attitudes, reasoning and intention, and it is a verbalizable, conscious process. In the second, behavior is enacted directly by automatic associations between categories and concepts, which are perceived as gut-feeling.
To study implicit, automatic associations, the so-called indirect measures are used, which generally consist in computerized tasks based on reaction times [
39]. The most widely used is the Implicit Association Test (IAT) [
42], which measures the strength of the associative link between positive and negative categories and attributes. The use of indirect measures improves the prediction of behavior [
43,
44,
45], and it is particularly effective in cases where there is ambivalence [
39], such as that of IBF. In fact, food choices are also not always guided by rational considerations and are often driven by impulsive tendencies. This may be the case with IBF, as well.
Previous entomophagy research has extensively used direct measures [
26,
46], while indirect measures [
28,
47], or both [
22], have been less frequent. However, in general, both techniques revealed unfavorable attitudes in Western countries. This seems primarily due to cultural beliefs that portray insects as dirty and unhealthy creatures and societies that consume them as primitive and uncivilized [
48].
The combined use of direct/explicit and indirect/implicit measures allows the assessment of both reflexive and more impulsive system-related processes, since they reflect the cognitive and the more spontaneous dimension of attitudes [
42]. However, these two systems are not mutually exclusive, and the way people rely on one or the other when choosing or not choosing to try IBF deserves further investigation.
1.4. Decision-Making Style, Attitudes, and Eating Behavior
It has been shown that individual differences, made from combinations of different psychological traits, could lead to different eating behaviors [
49]. For example, previous research warned that impulsivity in food-related decision-making could represent a risk factor for adopting habits that can affect our health in favor of very palatable and satisfying products, with negative consequences on diet quality [
49]. The pursue of immediate reward [
50], thus, could obscure the benefits of consuming alternative foods that have long-term health and environmental benefits. However, no previous studies explored the impact of rational vs. impulsive decision-making on IBF.
Moreover, previous research proposed that the adoption of reflexive vs. spontaneous strategies in decision-making could mediate the relationship between explicit attitudes, automatic associations, and the intention to buy [
39]. One factor that has been proposed to influence the adoption of different strategies is decision-making style. Decision-making styles are considered stable characteristics [
51], behavioral patterns that become recurrent [
52] through the consolidation and repetition of specific reactions when a decision needs to be made [
53]. In detail, Songa and Russo [
39] explored if the preference for intuition or deliberation could account for participants’ preference for one of two popular soft drinks. Their findings showed that, for participants with a higher preference for an intuitive decision-making style, there was a significant increase in purchase intention when the IAT score was higher, while for participants with a less intuitive decision-making style, choices were less influenced by implicit processes.
However, although it has been shown that the adoption of different decision-making styles can lead to different strategies in the decision-making process and to specific and diverse effects in direct and indirect measures, to the best of these authors’ knowledge no previous study explored the role of decision-making style on attitudes towards IBF, neither its relationship with other possible explanatory variables of the attitudes towards IBF, such as food neophobia and trust in science.
1.5. The Present Study
The aim of the present study was to investigate the propension towards IBF in a sample of university students. Starting from the idea that decisions derive from two parallel and distinct paths leading to automatic associations and reasoned attitudes [
41], we developed a framework that involves three main outcomes:
- 1.
-
explicit attitudes, as measured by self-report scales with different semantic content. We included the following dimensions: “Bad” vs. “Good,” Risky” vs. “Safe,” Harmful” vs. “Healthy,” Disgusting” vs. “Tasty.” Based on previous literature we expected to find:
- -
Hp 1: negative attitudes, especially on the disgusting/tasty dimension.
- 2.
-
implicit attitudes, as measured by IAT. We predicted the presence of:
- -
Hp 2: automatic adverse reactions toward IBFs and favorable associations for traditional foods.
- 3.
-
Intention to taste, as measured by a specific item. We anticipated:
- -
Hp 3: an average low propensity to taste. However, considering the age of our sample, we also expected to find several participants curious and inclined to taste IBF.
Moreover, we argued that individual differences could influence people’s attitudes and intention to taste IBF. Thus, we pursued:
- 4.
the identification of psychological profiles that could determine specific and differential dispositions towards IBF. To achieve this, we used a person-centered approach [
54] which involves categorizing individuals based on their similarities, enabling researchers to examine individuals more comprehensively than traditional approaches focused on isolated individual components.
For this stage we included three main psychological traits. The first one is food neophobia, which has been vastly used in previous studies, as already discussed. The second is trust in science and scientist, which has been previously found as being relevant in forming attitudes toward other sustainable food [
34] or production technologies [
32], but not yet in entomophagy. The third is decision-making style, which can have a role in modulating explicit/implicit attitudes but was never explored in relation to edible insects. Given the novelty of the study, we chose the General Decision-Making Style Inventory (GDMS) [
53], which measures a broad spectrum of decision-making styles. Besides the deliberative vs. intuitive continuum, which are paralleled by the rational vs. spontaneous/intuitive styles [
55], GDMS also includes the dependent and the avoidant scale, which can capture other aspects of the approach toward IBFs, such as the role of external guidance and social support (for the dependent style) [
56], but also indecision and lack of environmental awareness (for the avoidant style) [
57].
For the profiling stage we expected to find:
- -
Hp 3: more positive attitudes and a higher willingness to try IBF in those participants low in food neophobia and higher in trust in science and scientists;
- -
Hp 4: more positive attitudes and a higher willingness to try IBF in those participants high in rational style. We believe that the analysis of pros and cons could more easily lead to favorable opinions and a higher intention to taste. We also predicted a worse disposition in those high in intuitive/spontaneous style, since it could be more related on emotional/instinctive components.
No specific hypotheses have been formulated for the other styles; hence, we adopted an exploratory approach. However:
- -
RQ: we expected to find different combinations of the profiling variables determining specific patterns in the outcomes.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Italian university students aged between 20 and 30 years were invited to participate in the study. Participants were recruited using a snowball sampling method. The online questionnaire was distributed via different social networks (i.e., WhatsApp, Telegram, Facebook, and LinkedIn).
A total of 175 responses were collected. Among the volunteers who completed the questionnaire, there were 93 women (53.1%), 77 men (44.0%), and five individuals (2.9%) who identified as non-binary or did not specify their gender. The participants ranged from 20 to 30 years, with a mean age of 21.85 and a standard deviation of 1.75. Regarding eating habits, 168 volunteers were omnivores or flexitarians (96.0%), while seven (4.0%) were vegetarians or vegans. Due to the low number of non-binary and vegetarian/vegan volunteers, they were not included in the statistical analyses to ensure a sufficient and homogenous sample. Moreover, seven participants produced less than 75% correct responses in the IAT and were therefore not included in the analyses. Therefore, the final sample included 158 volunteers, with 86 women (54%) and 72 men (46%) and a mean age of 21.8 years (SD = 1.56).
The sample size was calculated a priori by resorting to power analysis [
58] through G*Power Software Version 3.1.9.7 [
59]. Considering the exploratory nature of this pilot study, we computed the achieved power of the relevant statistical models, given
α, sample size, and effect size.
The study was conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ethical committee of the University of Milano-Bicocca. Each participant provided written informed consent.
2.2. Procedure
Data were collected between November 2023 and June 2024. Participants were recruited mainly online by sharing the questionnaire link via WhatsApp, Telegram, Facebook and LinkedIn.
The questionnaire was generated using Qualtrics software. It was accessible for completion online, via mobile phone, or computer. Participants were initially asked to provide informed consent before processing to complete the questionnaire. Information about the study’s aim, procedure, duration, and the researchers’ contact details were provided in this section. Following that, participants were asked socio-demographic questions regarding age and gender. Additionally, participants' psychological dispositions regarding decision-making style and trust in science and scientists were examined. This was accomplished using the General Decision-making Style Inventory [
60] and Trust in Science and Scientist Inventory ([
61]. Furthermore, participants were asked about their eating habits and food neophobia [
62]. All variables described up to this point were treated as predictors of attitudes and intention to eat IBF. The final part of the online questionnaire assessed the three outcome measures: explicit and automatic attitudes toward IBF and intention to taste IBF.
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Socio-Demographics Variables and Diet
Participants were asked to declare their age, gender, and eating habits.
Eating habits: Participants had to select from 9 multi-choice options, choosing the one that best represented their usual eating habits. The options included: “I regularly eat red meat, white meat, and fish,” representing an omnivorous dietary pattern; “I consciously reduce the meat consumption, but still eat it occasionally;” “I do not eat red meat, but I do eat white meat and fish,” “I do not eat meat, but I do eat fish,” “I eat organically grown, locally sourced foods, with a significant overlap with foods consumed in a vegetarian diet, but also some types of meat,” which all together indicated a flexitarian dietary pattern. Additionally, options such as “I do not eat meat nor fish, but I do eat eggs and dairy,” I do not eat meat, fish, and eggs, but I do eat dairy,” I do not eat meat, fish, and dairy, but I do eat egg” corresponded to a vegetarian diet. Finally,” I do not eat meat and fish, nor do I consume animal source products” represented veganism, taken and modified from De Backer & Hudders [
63].
2.3.2. Profiling Variables
General decision-making style: The General Decision-making Style Inventory (GDMS) is a 25-item self-administered scale developed by Scott and Bruce in 1995 [
53]. It is structured into five sub-scales, each representing a particular decision-making style: 1. Rational: characterized by a logical and structured approach to decision-making (e.g., “I double-check my information sources to be sure I have the right facts before making a decision”). 2. Intuitive: represented by a tendency to rely upon intuitions, feelings, and sensations (e.g., “When making a decision, I rely upon my instincts”). 3. Dependent: characterized by the need for assistance and support from others (e.g., “I often need the assistance of other people when making important decisions”). 4. Avoidant: represented by attempts to postpone and avoid decisions (e.g., “I avoid making important decisions until the pressure is on”). 5. Spontaneous: characterized by the tendency to make decisions impulsively (e.g., “I generally make snap decisions”). Participants express their level of agreement with each of the 25 statements on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Each sub-scale score was calculated as the sum of the pertinent five items. This study used the Italian-validated version of the scale [
60]. The scale showed a discrete internal consistency in the present sample (Cronbach’s alpha ranged from 0.65 for the Rational subscale to 0.84 for the Avoidant subscale).
Trust in Science and Scientists: We used the short version [
61] of the Trust in Science and Scientist Inventory (TSSI) [
64] to assess trust in science. The scale consists of 14 items (e.g., “Scientific theories are trustworthy”), which are rated on a 5-point Likert scale from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Half of the statements are worded in reverse to measure trust in science, so responses to these statements were reversed when calculating the score. The final score was calculated as the mean score across all 14 items. A higher score indicates a greater trust in science and scientists. Given that there is no Italian version of this scale, we created an ad hoc translation of the items to measure the trust in science of the participants in our study. Two bilingual authors independently translated the text from English into Italian, and a third researcher retranslated each version of the Italian text back into English to ensure accuracy. Subsequently, all the authors discussed and agreed upon the final Italian translation. Since we used an adapted version of the scale, we conducted a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) to test the scale's validity. The results showed that the one-factor solution sufficiently fit the data (Chi-squared = 136, df = 73, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.90; SRMR = 0.065; RMSEA = 0.072, 90% CI [0.05, 0.09]). Standardized estimates were all significant (p < 0.001) and their values were > 0.4. Additionally, the scale demonstrated adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.85).
Food Neophobia: The level of neophobia towards new foods was assessed using the Italian version of the Food Neophobia Scale (FNS) [
65], as described by Proserpio et al. in 2016 [
62]. This scale measures reluctance, fear, and refusal to try new or unfamiliar foods. Respondents indicate their level of agreement with ten statements (e.g., “If I do not know what is in a food, I will not try it”) about foods or food situations using a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). Half of the statements are worded in reverse to measure food neophobia (FN), so responses to these statements were reversed when calculating the score. The FNS score is calculated as the sum of the responses to the 10 items, ranging from 10 to 70. A higher score indicates a greater level of food neophobia. In the present sample, the scale showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87).
2.3.3. Outcome Variables
Explicit attitudes toward insect-based food: Explicit attitudes toward each category of IBF (i.e., grasshopper flour, cricket burger, larvae cookies, insect crackers) were measured by asking participants to think about it and evaluate it on a 7-point Likert scale using four pairs of adjectives within a semantic differential scale adapted from Maggino and Mola [
66]. The adjectives used were: “Bad” vs. “Good,” Risky” vs. “Safe,” Harmful” vs. “Healthy,” Disgusting” vs. “Tasty.” An example item was “What adjectives do you think are most suitable to describe grasshopper flour?” A mean score was calculated for each of the four couple of adjectives to explore specific attitudes toward IBF, and a synthetic index representing the mean explicit attitude was then calculated. This synthetic index was calculated as the mean score across all items. A higher score indicates a more positive attitude toward IBF. All the scores showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha ranged from 0.84 to 0.94).
Automatic attitudes toward insect-based food: Implicit Association Test (IAT): To identify automatic associations between IBF, traditional food, and positive or negative attributes, participants completed an adapted version of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) [
43,
67]. In this task, they were prompted to associate eight adjectives with either positive or negative valence, with eight words representing insect-based and traditional food. The stimuli for the two target food categories consisted of four pairs of words. These pairs included grasshopper flour vs. wheat flour, cricket burger vs. veal burger, larvae cookies vs. rye cookies, and insect crackers vs. cereal crackers. Meanwhile, the four pairs of negative and positive attributes were the adjectives also employed for the explicit attitudes measure. The underlying assumption was that individuals harboring numerous biases against IBF would find it easier (i.e., exhibit lower response times) to associate the IBF category with negative attributes than with positive attributes. Compared with other studies using a more common version of the task (flowers vs. insects), we opted to create a more focused task that contained real traditional and insect-based foods to ensure greater ecological value.
The strength of the automatic association between the food categories and the positive or negative attributes was quantified by the D-index, which is a score derived from the standardized mean difference between target-attribute pairings that are 'inconsistent with the hypothesis' and pairings that are 'consistent with the hypothesis' [
67]. The D-index value typically ranges from -1 to +1. A higher D-index (more positive) indicated a stronger association between pairings 'consistent with the hypothesis' (i.e., the association between the traditional food category and positive attributes). Conversely, a negative D-index suggested a stronger association between pairings 'inconsistent with the hypothesis' (i.e., the association between IBF category and positive attributes). A D-index equal to zero indicated the absence of a significant preference for either food category. Errors were managed by requesting participants to correct their responses.
Intention to taste insect-based food in the future: Four ad-hoc items were used to ask participants about their willingness to taste four different IBF (grasshopper flour, cricket burger, larvae cookies, insect crackers) in the future. The full text of the items is reported below:
- 1.
Grinding grasshoppers can produce flour for making bread, pizza, protein bars, or smoothies. Would you consider trying these grasshopper flour-based recipes in the future?
- 2.
In some restaurants, it is possible to taste burgers made with cricket flour. Would you like to try them in the future?
- 3.
There are cookies on the market produced using dried moth larvae. Would you like to try them in the future?
- 4.
It is already possible to buy crackers made from dried insects. Would you like to try them in the future?
The response options range from 1: “extremely unlikely” to 10: “extremely likely.” The responses were analyzed separately, and a synthetic index representing the mean intention to taste IBF was calculated. This synthetic index was calculated as the mean score across all four items. A higher score indicates a greater intention to taste IBF in the future. The synthetic index showed good internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = 0.96).
2.4. Data Analysis
To answer the research questions, the following analyses were conducted:
2.4.1. Preliminary Analyses
Preliminary analyses were performed on the dataset to verify data normality and the internal consistency of the psychological scales. Cronbach’s alpha was calculated to estimate the internal consistency of the psychological scales [
68]. Because an adapted version of the Trust in Science and Scientists scale was used, Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was performed. Hu and Bentler’s guidelines [
69] for several fit indices were employed to decide if the expected models were consistent with the data. A good model yields a nonsignificant chi-square statistic, a comparative fit index (CFI) higher than 0.90, and a weighted root-mean-square residual (SRMR) lower than 1.0. Values close to 0.06 for the RMSEA indicate a good fit; between 0.06 and 0.08, a moderate fit, and values larger than 0.10 indicate a poor fit.
2.4.2. Identification of Psychological Profiles
- 1.
Cluster analyses were performed on the continuous scores of the psychological traits (FNS, TSSI, GDMS), following the recommendations of Bergman and colleagues [
70]. First, all variables were standardized. Additionally, a residue analysis was conducted (average squared Euclidean distance—ASED—less than 0.5). Ten multivariate outliers were identified (6.3% of the sample) and removed from the subsequent analyses. A two-step clustering procedure was used, which combined Ward’s hierarchical and nonhierarchical k-means methods. In the hierarchical method, different solutions were explored based on the magnitude of the change in the explained error sum of squares percentage (%EESS) value between adjacent cluster solutions. Subsequently, each solution was employed as the initial cluster center for a nonhierarchical k-means clustering procedure.
- 2.
Descriptive statistics were calculated on all profiling variables.
- 3.
Differences in age and gender distribution were investigated. For gender analysis, χ2 test was run on cluster and gender variables. For age analysis, a univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with age as dependent variable, and cluster as independent variable.
2.4.3. Differences between Clusters on Outcome Variables
- 1.
Descriptive statistics were calculated on the outcome variables.
- 2.
Three separate univariate ANOVAs were conducted using mean explicit attitudes, automatic attitudes, and the intention to taste IBF as dependent variables. The independent variable in each analysis was cluster. Post-hoc Tukey tests were used for comparisons when variances were equal, while the Games-Howell method was used when variances were unequal. Before conducting the analyses, the normal distribution of the variables was confirmed through assessments of skewness and kurtosis, and the homogeneity of variances was evaluated using Levene’s test.
All statistical tests were two-tailed, and a p ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 29 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA) and Jamovi (Version 2.2.5, The Jamovi project, 2021, retrieved from
https://www.jamovi.org). The statistical package ROPstat [
71] was used for typological analyses.
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analyses
The normal distribution of the data was tested by calculating skewness and kurtosis indices; the recommended range of ±2 and ±7 was considered for normality, respectively [
72]. All variables were normally distributed. All scales had moderate to good fit.
3.2. Identification of Psychological Profiles
3.2.1. Cluster Analyses
After analyzing the scree-type plot displaying the change in %EESS by cluster solutions and considering the magnitude of the change in the %EESS values, we decided to retain the solution involving four clusters (%EESS = 35.7). Given that the study is a pilot with a relatively small sample size, we concluded the process by extracting four clusters, that ensured a numerosity of at least 30 cases per cluster (at least 20% of cases per cluster). Participants were well distributed in the four clusters (see
Table 1).
Figure 1 presents the cluster solution. The y-axis represents Z-scores. Because the clusters were defined using Z-scores for the total sample, each cluster’s mean z-scores indicate the distance between the cluster means and the total sample’s standardized mean. In other words, a Z-score between −0.5 and +0.5 denoted an average value (i.e., the “average participant” psychological traits). A Z-score over +0.5 denoted values above the sample mean.
Cluster 1, “the gut-feeling,” was characterized by a more intuitive/spontaneous and less rational decision-making style than the average sample. We chose the cat for avatar as it is an extremely sensitive and instinctive animal.
Cluster 2, “the suspicious,” was characterized by high food neophobia and low trust in science and scientists. We chose the llama for avatar because it spits when it feels threatened.
Cluster 3, “the vicarious,” was characterized by a more dependent/avoidant and less intuitive decision-making style, with low food neophobia as well. We chose the chameleon for avatar to represent the vicarious profile's tendency to mimic what others do, to go along with the environment.
Cluster 4, “the mind,” was characterized by high trust in science and scientists and a decision-making style that is more rational than avoidant/spontaneous. We chose the bat for avatar since it is technological (it possesses a biosonar).
3.2.2. Descriptive Statistics
Descriptive data are reported in
Table 1 with cluster number, cluster size, mean age, and gender frequency, along with data related to all the psychological scales.
3.2.3. Gender Differences
The clusters did not differ in terms of age (F 3,75.2 = 2.07; p = 0.11) or gender (χ2 3, n = 148) = 6.88; p = 0.076). However, although the analysis was not statistically significant, it is possible to appreciate from a qualitative point of view that the frequency of men was higher in Cluster 4 (“the mind”), while the frequency of women was higher in all the other cluster, especially Cluster 2 (“the suspicious”).
3.3. Differences between Clusters on Outcome Variables
3.3.1. Explicit Attitudes towards IBF
In general, participants showed a more negative attitude toward IBF when rated along the disgusting vs. tasty scale (M = 3.35; SD = 1.37), followed by the bad vs. good scale (M = 3.63; SD = 1.42), and the harmful vs. healthy scale (M = 4.23; SD = 1.6). The most positive attitude was observed on the risky vs. safe scale (M = 4.42; SD = 1.69).
The mean explicit attitude (M = 3.91; SD = 1.27) was used as dependent variable in the first ANOVA, with cluster as independent variable. Assumption checks suggested that the group variances were homogeneous (Levene's test = 1.01; p = 0.39). Results (see
Figure 2) highlighted a significant effect of Group (Fisher’s F = 5.5; p < 0.005; η2p = 0.1; achieved power = 0.94). Cluster 4 declared the more favorable attitude toward IBF (M = 4.51; SD = 1.51), followed by Cluster 3 (M = 4.15; SD = 1.21) and Cluster 1 (M = 3.67; SD = 1.1). Cluster 2 declared the most negative attitude toward IBF (M = 3.46; SD = 1.06). Post-hoc comparisons showed that the difference between Cluster 1 and Cluster 4 was statistically significant (p < 0.05), as well as the difference between Cluster 2 and Cluster 4 (p < 0.005).
3.3.2. Automatic Attitudes towards IBF
The mean D-score for the whole sample was 0.61 (SD = 0.38) and had a normal distribution (skewness = -0.20; kurtosis = -0.38,
Figure 3). As the procedure section outlines, a higher D-score indicates a stronger association between positive attributes and traditional food. In contrast, negative D-scores indicates a stronger association between positive attributes and IBF. Therefore, the results suggested that the participants displayed a more favorable automatic attitude toward traditional food than IBF.
The second ANOVA considered the automatic attitude toward IBF as the dependent variable and the division into clusters as the independent variable. Assumption checks suggested that the group variances were homogeneous (Levene's test = 0.09; p = 0.97). Results (see
Figure 4) did not evidence a significant Group difference (p=0.086). However, from a descriptive perspective, Cluster 4 exhibited the more favorable automatic attitude toward IBF (M = 0.47; SD = 0.37), followed by Cluster 2 (M = 0.63; SD = 0.36) and Cluster 1 (M = 0.65; SD = 0.39). Cluster 3 displayed the most negative automatic attitude toward IBF (M = 0.68; SD = 0.37).
3.3.3. Intention to Taste IBF
Descriptive statistics suggested that, in general, participants had little intention of trying cookies made with moth larvae (M = 4.85; SD = 3.17), followed by insect cracker (M = 5.77; M = 2.97) and cricket burger (M = 6.34; SD = 2.85). They were more inclined to taste products made from grasshopper flour (M = 6.74; SD = 2.75).
We used the mean intention to taste IBF (M = 5.92; SD = 2.77) as dependent variable for the third ANOVA, with cluster as independent variable. Assumption checks suggested that the group variances were homogeneous (Levene’s test = 0.97; p = 0.41). Results (see
Figure 5) highlighted a significant effect of Group (Fisher’s F = 9.87; p < 0.001; η2p = 0.18; achieved power = 0.998). Cluster 3 declared the highest intention to taste IBF (M = 7.15; SD = 2.28), followed by Cluster 4 (M = 7.1; SD = 2.5), and Cluster 1 (M = 5.63; SD = 2.68). Cluster 2 declared the lowest intention to taste IBF (M = 4.26; SD = 2.6). Post-hoc comparisons showed that the difference between Cluster 2 and Cluster 4 was statistically significant (p < 0.001), as well as the difference between Cluster 2 vs. Cluster 3 (p < 0.001).
4. Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to investigate the role of different psychological profiles in relation to a novel and rather controversial behavior in Western countries: eating insect products. The interest was particularly in exploring the effect on both reflexive and impulsive processes that may be associated with the enactment of the actual behavior, namely, explicit attitudes and automatic associations. Then, we profiled participants into separate clusters and compared them in terms of the different outcomes.
The results identified some interesting data. First, considering the whole sample, unfavorable attitudes toward IBF emerged, consistent with hypotheses and previous literature. This trend was manifested in all three outcomes. For explicit attitudes, the lowest scores were given along the disgusting vs. tasty scale, with a mean of 3.35, while the least problematic scores were given along the risky vs. safe scale, with a mean of 4.42. As discussed in the introduction, both disgust [
22] and risk perception [
26] are important resistance factors in IBF consumption, but disgust is probably the biggest obstacle [
28].
The negative attitude also emerged at the automatic level, as can be appreciated from the d-index. This, 0.61, indicates the presence of significantly more favorable automatic associations toward traditional food.
About the intention to taste, the participants in our sample expressed on average an intention of 5.92 out of 10 to taste IBF. This seems a bit more favorable than what Roma and colleagues found [
16] on another Italian sample. In fact, the researchers found how only 22.9% of their sample was willing to taste IBF. This can be explained by taking into consideration the samples’ age. In fact, previous work already underlined that age is negatively related with the acceptance of insects as food [
15,
16,
17]. Our sample was composed only by young adults, while Roma and colleagues’ sample ranged from 18 to 81 years old. Their results showed that the consumers who strongly rejected IBF in any form or preparation were the oldest [
16].
Moreover, the product considered least tempting in our sample was moth larvae cookies, while grasshopper flour was the one considered most appealing. In the work by Roma and colleagues [
16] the percentage of participants willing to taste was significantly higher (16.8%) when it came to products where insects were not visible, compared to the case where insects were visible (2.3%). In a large study involving 13 countries worldwide, Castro and Chambers [
73] asked participants what were the main psychological or sensory reasons in choosing whether or not to taste certain products. Participants from all nations cited appearance as the main reason, making explicit that it is important that you do not see bits of insects in the food. A preparation made from insect flour, therefore, seems to be the one that generates the least adverse reactions. In our case probably the word “larvae” accentuated the feeling of disgust, since larvae have been found as being even more problematic that adult insects [
74].
But what individual differences tell us about outcome variables? We identified 4 main profiles by performing a cluster analysis, including factors that might contribute to the decision-making process in different ways. These comprised both well-known factors in the literature, such as food neophobia, and variables new to the topic area, such as trust in science and decision-making style. The 4 main profiles were: “the gut-feeling”, “the suspicious,” “the vicarious,” and “the mind.” After identifying the psychological profiles, we explored the differences on the main outcomes, namely explicit and automatic attitudes, and intention to taste IBF.
The “gut-feeling” profile is mainly characterized by the decision-making style with a combination of spontaneous and intuitive, and not at all rational. We are, therefore, faced with people who make decisions solely based on their feelings and emotions, in a very quick way. The cluster is characterized by rather negative attitudes, both implicit and explicit, and a modest intention to taste. Not surprisingly, avoidance of analytical reasoning of pros and cons, and trust in one's own instincts, can lead to avoidance behavior from IBF. In fact, several previous contributions have highlighted the crucial role of emotional aspects, and particularly of disgust, toward IBF. For example, in the previously cited work by La Barbera and colleagues [
28], food neophobia and the emotion of disgust were found to negatively and independently affect the intention to eat IBF. The explanatory power of disgust was even greater. This important finding underscores how, although the two constructs may be similar, they are not overlapping and thus may contribute specifically and differentially on the outcomes. The modest declared intention to taste may be more determined by the tendency to try and not back down, but it does not seem very promising since it is not supported by favorable attitudes. For the “gut-feeling” profile, we can speculate that the emotion of disgust might be one of the determinants of aversion to IBF, and that a more intuitive/impulsive system for decision-making could be adopted. Although not statistically significant, this profile was the one with the highest percentage of women. This finding is also reflected in the literature, as it has been found that women are generally more reluctant to accept IBF and report higher FN and disgust scores [
17,
75,
76].
“The suspicious” profile finds support in previous literature, as it is characterized by high food neophobia, negative attitudes, and low intention to taste IBF. This profile is similar to one of the two profiles identified by Junges and colleagues [
29] in their qualitative/quantitative work. The segments identified were “consumers with a favorable attitude toward insect-based foods” and “consumers with an unfavorable attitude toward insect-based foods.” The main characteristic of people belonging to the second segment, in addition to negative attitudes and low intention to eat IBF, were high food neophobia scores and suspiciousness toward these novel foods. The negative relationship between food neophobia and willingness toward IBF has already been widely confirmed in the literature [
23,
24,
25]. In Verbeke's work [
17] it was found that the increase of just one unit in food neophobia scores led to an 84% decrease in the likelihood of being ready to adopt a diet that includes IBF. A very interesting perspective is offered by the work of Jaeger and colleagues [
77] which showed that people with higher FN scores rated the emotional impact of food more negatively and with greater arousal. “The suspicious” profile, moreover, is characterized by very low trust in science. This result, which has no previous findings in the entomophagy literature, is in line with our hypotheses as it was, instead, identified with other sustainable foods. In their work investigating openness to try cultured meat Lewish and Riefler [
34] found that distrust of scientists was negatively related to behavioral intention. Similar findings emerged on the acceptance of genetically modified foods [
32].
“The vicarious” profile is characterized by a fair overt disposition toward IBF in terms of both explicit attitudes and intention to taste. However, this good disposition is not matched by automatic attitudes and the scores are comparable to those of the two less favorable profiles. How can this discrepancy be explained? The cluster is characterized by the concurrence of two decision-making styles: avoidant and dependent. The avoidant style is prone to postpone any decision and correlates negatively with rationality in decision-making [
56]. The dependent needs confirmation and seeks external references to make decisions, such as advice from trusted people, but also from what authorities suggest. More interestingly, both the avoidant and dependent profiles are positively associated with indecisiveness, as opposed to the rational style [
56]. At the same time, they present low food neophobia. This aspect is very important since it indicates how low food neophobia is not sufficient to develop totally favorable dispositions, as already argued, nor to explain this ambivalence in cluster 3. It is therefore possible to hypothesize that the indecisiveness that characterizes both these styles may have led people to respond relatively positively to explicit questions, either because they did not have to think too much (avoidant) or because of social desirability (dependent), but still manifest a low propensity to a more automatic level. In this case, it is possible to hypothesize a conflict between the two systems.
Finally, “the mind” profile, characterized by a rational decision-making style and high trust in science, has more positive attitudes than the other profiles toward IBF and a higher intention to taste them. These characteristics are partially reflected in the literature. In a previous study, Vernau and colleagues [
45] investigated the intention of an Italian and a Danish sample to include IBF in their diet by performing market segmentation based on their scores on the Food Related Lifestyle Scale. Although they used different tools than those employed in the present work, the researchers identified a “rational food consumer” profile, corresponding to an informed person who gathers information about the products they buy and considers multiple factors at once when shopping. Again, this profile was the one who declared a more favorable intention. Indeed, the main characteristics of the rational decision maker [
56] involve a logical evaluation of possible alternatives and a meticulous search for information, as also confirmed by eye-tracking data on product labels [
78]. In addition, a positive correlation between rational style and cognitive engagement was previously revealed [
56]. A more positive propensity toward IBF is not only explicitly stated by “the mind” cluster, but also emerges from the reaction times of the IAT, albeit not statistically significant. This profile is the only one with a higher percentage of men respondents. This result has a basis in previous literature, since men have been found to be more accepting of IBF than women [
76,
79]. An Italian research [
80] demonstrated that men were 2.55 times more likely to be open to insect consumption. However, the analysis of gender differences in previous research produced mixed results [
81,
82] and then would deserve a more thorough exploration in future studies.
The absence of a significant effect in d indices may still be a meaningful finding: as differences emerge about what participants overtly state, the absence of differences in automatic reactions may indicate that while there are more favorable profiles to tasting and consuming IBF (such as “the mind” and “the vicarious”), there is still much resistance on an implicit level that warrants future investigation. It is possible to hypothesize that, while from a cognitive point of view it is possible to identify good and convincing arguments that dispose people well to IBF, favorable impulsive reactions to this novelty require more time and familiarity. Indeed, it has been hypothesized that automatic attitudes change more slowly over time than explicit ones [
83]. The unfavorable position towards IBF, then, is very pronounced not only because they are foods produced through the processing of animals that in Western culture are considered gross, but also because they are products that people have never or rarely encountered.
The present work is not without limitations. First, the selection of psychological factors for profiling can certainly not be considered exhaustive, as many other aspects may contribute to modulate intention-to-taste behavior. Some possible explanations, such as those related to disgust for the “gut-feeling” profile or social desirability for “the vicarious” profile, have not been measured with specific instruments, but can only be hypothesized and explored in future research.
Moreover, since this was a pilot study, some results of interest did not reach statistical significance due to the small sample and the large number of variables involved. Starting from these initial findings, the future goal is to test our hypotheses in a larger and more representative sample of the general population.
This work has several strengths and innovations. First, it represents the first example in which decision-making style is integrated into the entomophagy literature. Second, the person-centered statistical approach enabled us to comprehend how particular combinations of psychological dispositions lead to different outcomes. This approach has the potential to offer deeper insights into how individuals form their attitudes towards IBF and, as a result, their behaviors in real-life scenarios.
Also, the simultaneous measurement of explicit and automatic attitudes may give way to future conversations on how these aspects interact with each other at different levels.
Finally, in light of our results, it will be possible to understand more deeply people's openness to these products as an alternative to more traditional, less sustainable, animal-based protein sources. Moreover, our findings may support the development of strategies to provide adequate information and support decision-making, taking into account of different psychological profiles.
5. Conclusions
The purpose of this research was to investigate how attitudes and intention to taste IBF could vary according to certain psychological traits. Specifically, the role of more reflexive/cognitive and more associative/automatic processes on the propensity toward IBFs was examined.
A person-centered approach was adopted, with which it was possible to identify different profiles based on decision-making style, food neophobia, and trust in science. These profiles showed unique and specific characteristics in outcome variables showing the role that these psychological factors play in food-related choices. These findings help to expand current literature on processes and psychological factors affecting lifestyle decision-making. In addition, they provide new insights to the field of entomophagy research by describing people's reactions to new sustainable food proposals.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, M.E.V., P.S., A.L. and P.A.L.; methodology, M.E.V., P.S., R.A. and M.D.; software, R.A.; validation, M.E.V. and R.A.; formal analysis, M.E.V. and R.A.; investigation, M.E.V. and R.A.; resources, P.S.; data curation, M.E.V. and R.A.; writing—original draft preparation, M.E.V. and R.A.; writing—review and editing, M.E.V., R.A., A.L., P.A.L., M.D. and P.S.; visualization, M.E.V.; supervision, P.S.; project administration, M.E.V. and R.A.; funding acquisition, P.S. and M.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was funded by the National Recovery and Resilience Plan (NRRP), Mission 4 Component 2 Investment 1.3 - Call for tender No. 341 of 15 March 2022 of Italian Ministry of University and Research funded by the European Union – NextGenerationEU; Award Number: Project code PE00000003, Concession Decree No. 1550 of 11 October 2022 adopted by the Italian Ministry of University and Research, CUP D93C22000890001, Project title “ON Foods - Research and innovation network on food and nutrition Sustainability, Safety and Security – Working ON Foods.”
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the University of Milano-Bicocca (Protocol no. RM-2024-787, date of approval March 7, 2024).
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.
Acknowledgments
Avatars figures for the psychological profiles have been generated with CGDream, retrieved from
https://cgdream.ai/
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- O’Neill, D.W.; Fanning, A.L.; Lamb, W.F.; Steinberger, J.K. A Good Life for All within Planetary Boundaries. Nat Sustain 2018, 1, 88–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- FAO Greenhouse Gas Emissions from Agrifood Systems 2022.
- Michel, P.; Begho, T. Paying for Sustainable Food Choices: The Role of Environmental Considerations in Consumer Valuation of Insect-Based Foods. Food Quality and Preference 2023, 106, 104816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- ROCKSTRÖM, J.; STEFFEN, W.; NOONE, K.; OWENS, S. “A Safe Operating Space for Humanity” (2009). In The Future of Nature; Robin, L., Sörlin, S., Warde, P., Eds.; Documents of Global Change; Yale University Press, 2013; pp. 491–505. ISBN 978-0-300-18461-7.
- van Huis, A.; Oonincx, D.G.A.B. The Environmental Sustainability of Insects as Food and Feed. A Review. Agron. Sustain. Dev. 2017, 37, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bodenheimer, F.S. Insects as Human Food. In Insects as Human Food: A Chapter of the Ecology of Man; Bodenheimer, F.S., Ed.; Springer Netherlands: Dordrecht, 1951; pp. 7–38. ISBN 978-94-017-6159-8. [Google Scholar]
- FAO Looking at Edible Insects from a Food Safety Perspective; FAO ;, 2021. ISBN 978-92-5-134196-4.
- Maffei, G.; Tacchini, G. Un insetto nel piatto [A bug on the plate]; Red Edizioni, 2016. ISBN 978-88-573-0701-5.
- Berggren, Å.; Jansson, A.; Low, M. Approaching Ecological Sustainability in the Emerging Insects-as-Food Industry. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 2019, 34, 132–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stone, H.; FitzGibbon, L.; Millan, E.; Murayama, K. Curious to Eat Insects? Curiosity as a Key Predictor of Willingness to Try Novel Food. Appetite 2022, 168, 105790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rumpold, B.A.; Schlüter, O.K. Nutritional Composition and Safety Aspects of Edible Insects. Molecular Nutrition & Food Research 2013, 57, 802–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nakagaki, B.J.; Defoliart, G.R. Comparison of Diets for Mass-Rearing Acheta Domesticus (Orthoptera: Gryllidae) as a Novelty Food, and Comparison of Food Conversion Efficiency with Values Reported for Livestock. Journal of Economic Entomology 1991, 84, 891–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guiné, R.P.F.; Correia, P.; Coelho, C.; Costa, C.A. The Role of Edible Insects to Mitigate Challenges for Sustainability. Open Agriculture 2021, 6, 24–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Müller-Maatsch, J.; Gras, C. The “Carmine Problem” and Potential Alternatives. In Handbook on Natural Pigments in Food and Beverages; Carle, R., Schweiggert, R.M., Eds.; Woodhead Publishing Series in Food Science, Technology and Nutrition; Woodhead Publishing, 2016; pp. 385–428. ISBN 978-0-08-100371-8.
- Faccio, E.; Guiotto Nai Fovino, L. Food Neophobia or Distrust of Novelties? Exploring Consumers’ Attitudes toward GMOs, Insects and Cultured Meat. Applied Sciences 2019, 9, 4440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roma, R.; Ottomano Palmisano, G.; De Boni, A. Insects as Novel Food: A Consumer Attitude Analysis through the Dominance-Based Rough Set Approach. Foods 2020, 9, 387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verbeke, W. Profiling Consumers Who Are Ready to Adopt Insects as a Meat Substitute in a Western Society. Food Quality and Preference 2015, 39, 147–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bigliardi, B.; Galati, F. Innovation Trends in the Food Industry: The Case of Functional Foods. Trends in Food Science & Technology 2013, 31, 118–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verbeke, W. Consumer Acceptance of Functional Foods: Socio-Demographic, Cognitive and Attitudinal Determinants. Food Quality and Preference 2005, 16, 45–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mascarello, G.; Pinto, A.; Rizzoli, V.; Tiozzo, B.; Crovato, S.; Ravarotto, L. Ethnic Food Consumption in Italy: The Role of Food Neophobia and Openness to Different Cultures. Foods 2020, 9, 112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegrist, M.; Hartmann, C.; Keller, C. Antecedents of Food Neophobia and Its Association with Eating Behavior and Food Choices. Food Quality and Preference 2013, 30, 293–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pozharliev, R.; De Angelis, M.; Rossi, D.; Bagozzi, R.; Amatulli, C. I Might Try It: Marketing Actions to Reduce Consumer Disgust toward Insect-Based Food. Journal of Retailing 2023, 99, 149–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mancini, S.; Sogari, G.; Menozzi, D.; Nuvoloni, R.; Torracca, B.; Moruzzo, R.; Paci, G. Factors Predicting the Intention of Eating an Insect-Based Product. Foods 2019, 8, 270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sogari, G.; Menozzi, D.; Mora, C. The Food Neophobia Scale and Young Adults’ Intention to Eat Insect Products. International Journal of Consumer Studies 2019, 43, 68–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vartiainen, O.; Elorinne, A.-L.; Niva, M.; Väisänen, P. Finnish Consumers’ Intentions to Consume Insect-Based Foods. Journal of Insects as Food and Feed 2020, 6, 261–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- La Barbera, F.; Amato, M.; Fasanelli, R.; Verneau, F. Perceived Risk of Insect-Based Foods: An Assessment of the Entomophagy Attitude Questionnaire Predictive Validity. Insects 2021, 12, 403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, M.A.; Shin, J.T.; Kim, Y.W. An Exploration and Investigation of Edible Insect Consumption: The Impacts of Image and Description on Risk Perceptions and Purchase Intent. Psychology & Marketing 2016, 33, 94–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- La Barbera, F.; Verneau, F.; Amato, M.; Grunert, K. Understanding Westerners’ Disgust for the Eating of Insects: The Role of Food Neophobia and Implicit Associations. Food Quality and Preference 2018, 64, 120–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Junges, J.R.; do Canto, N.R.; de Barcellos, M.D. Not as Bad as I Thought: Consumers’ Positive Attitudes Toward Innovative Insect-Based Foods. Front. Nutr. 2021, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P.; Fischhoff, B.; Lichtenstein, S. Facts and Fears: Understanding Perceived Risk. In; Plenum, 1980.
- Siegrist, M.; Cvetkovich, G. Perception of Hazards: The Role of Social Trust and Knowledge. Risk Analysis 2000, 20, 713–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Deng, H.; Hu, R. A Crisis of Consumers’ Trust in Scientists and Its Influence on Consumer Attitude toward Genetically Modified Foods. British Food Journal 2019, 121, 2454–2476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lusk, J.L.; Roosen, J.; Bieberstein, A. Consumer Acceptance of New Food Technologies: Causes and Roots of Controversies. Annual Review of Resource Economics 2014, 6, 381–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewisch, L.; Riefler, P. Behavioural Intentions towards Cultured Meat: The Role of Personal Values, Domain-Specific Innovativeness and Distrust in Scientists. British Food Journal 2023, 125, 1769–1781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menozzi, D.; Sogari, G.; Veneziani, M.; Simoni, E.; Mora, C. Eating Novel Foods: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour to Predict the Consumption of an Insect-Based Product. Food Quality and Preference 2017, 59, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thu Thu Aung, M.; Dürr, J.; Klink-Lehmann, J.; Borgemeister, C. Predicting Consumers’ Intention towards Entomophagy Using an Extended Theory of Planned Behavior: Evidence from Myanmar. Int J Trop Insect Sci 2023, 43, 1189–1206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bae, Y.; Choi, J. Consumer Acceptance of Edible Insect Foods: An Application of the Extended Theory of Planned Behavior. Nutrition Research and Practice 2021, 15, 122–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Songa, G.; Russo, V. IAT, Consumer Behaviour and the Moderating Role of Decision-Making Style: An Empirical Study on Food Products. Food Quality and Preference 2018, 64, 205–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, L.L.; Lindsay, D.S.; Toth, J.P. Unconscious Influences Revealed: Attention, Awareness, and Control. American Psychologist 1992, 47, 802–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strack, F.; Deutsch, R. Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2004, 8, 220–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greenwald, A.G.; Banaji, M.R. Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes. Psychological Review 1995, 102, 4–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenwald, A.G.; McGhee, D.E.; Schwartz, J.L.K. Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1998, 74, 1464–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maison, D.; Greenwald, A.G.; Bruin, R.H. Predictive Validity of the Implicit Association Test in Studies of Brands, Consumer Attitudes, and Behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology 2004, 14, 405–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verneau, F.; La Barbera, F.; Amato, M.; Riverso, R.; Grunert, K.G. Assessing the Role of Food Related Lifestyle in Predicting Intention towards Edible Insects. Insects 2020, 11, 660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kröger, T.; Dupont, J.; Büsing, L.; Fiebelkorn, F. Acceptance of Insect-Based Food Products in Western Societies: A Systematic Review. Front. Nutr. 2022, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verneau, F.; La Barbera, F.; Kolle, S.; Amato, M.; Del Giudice, T.; Grunert, K. The Effect of Communication and Implicit Associations on Consuming Insects: An Experiment in Denmark and Italy. Appetite 2016, 106, 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shelomi, M. The Meat of Affliction: Insects and the Future of Food as Seen in Expo 2015. Trends in Food Science & Technology 2016, 56, 175–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bénard, M.; Bellisle, F.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Julia, C.; Andreeva, V.A.; Etilé, F.; Reach, G.; Dechelotte, P.; Tavolacci, M.-P.; Hercberg, S.; et al. Impulsivity Is Associated with Food Intake, Snacking, and Eating Disorders in a General Population. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2019, 109, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- French, S.A.; Epstein, L.H.; Jeffery, R.W.; Blundell, J.E.; Wardle, J. Eating Behavior Dimensions. Associations with Energy Intake and Body Weight. A Review. Appetite 2012, 59, 541–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bavoľár, J.; Bačíková-Slešková, M. Do Decision-Making Styles Help Explain Health-Risk Behavior among University Students in Addition to Personality Factors? SP 2018, 60, 71–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Driver, M.J.; Rowe, A.J. Decision-Making Styles: A New Approach to Management Decision Making. Behavioral problems in organizations 1979, 141–182. [Google Scholar]
- Scott, S.G.; Bruce, R.A. Decision-Making Style: The Development and Assessment of a New Measure. Educational and Psychological Measurement 1995, 55, 818–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magnusson, D. The Logic and Implications of a Person-Oriented Approach. In Methods and models for studying the individual; Sage Publications, Inc: Thousand Oaks, CA, US, 1998; pp. 33–64. ISBN 978-0-7619-1451-8. [Google Scholar]
- Cook, C.; Gonzales, H. Australian Individual Decision Styles, Intuitive and Rational Decision Making in Business. International Proceedings of Economics Development and Research 2016, 86. [Google Scholar]
- Curşeu, P.L.; Schruijer, S.G.L. Decision Styles and Rationality: An Analysis of the Predictive Validity of the General Decision-Making Style Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement 2012, 72, 1053–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atta, M.; Malik, N.I.; Makhdoom, I.F.; Fiaz, N.; Scholar, M.P. Decision-Making Styles Predicting Decisional-Procrastination among College Principals.
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Academic Press, 2013. ISBN 978-1-4832-7648-9.
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A Flexible Statistical Power Analysis Program for the Social, Behavioral, and Biomedical Sciences. Behavior Research Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gambetti, E.; Fabbri, M.; Bensi, L.; Tonetti, L. A Contribution to the Italian Validation of the General Decision-Making Style Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences 2008, 44, 842–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plohl, N.; Musil, B. Modeling Compliance with COVID-19 Prevention Guidelines: The Critical Role of Trust in Science. Psychology, Health & Medicine 2021, 26, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proserpio, C.; Laureati, M.; Bertoli, S.; Battezzati, A.; Pagliarini, E. Determinants of Obesity in Italian Adults: The Role of Taste Sensitivity, Food Liking, and Food Neophobia. Chemical Senses 2016, 41, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Backer, C.J.S.; Hudders, L. Meat Morals: Relationship between Meat Consumption Consumer Attitudes towards Human and Animal Welfare and Moral Behavior. Meat Science 2015, 99, 68–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nadelson, L.; Jorcyk, C.; Yang, D.; Jarratt Smith, M.; Matson, S.; Cornell, K.; Husting, V. I Just Don’t Trust Them: The Development and Validation of an Assessment Instrument to Measure Trust in Science and Scientists. School Science and Mathematics 2014, 114, 76–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pliner, P.; Hobden, K. Development of a Scale to Measure the Trait of Food Neophobia in Humans. Appetite 1992, 19, 105–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maggino, F.; Mola, T. Il differenziale semantico per la misura degli atteggiamenti: costruzione, applicazione e analisi. 2007.
- Greenwald, A.G.; Nosek, B.A.; Banaji, M.R. Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: I. An Improved Scoring Algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2003, 85, 197–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cronbach, L.J. Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests. Psychometrika 1951, 16, 297–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hu, L.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergman, L.R.; Nurmi, J.-E.; Eye, A.A. von I-States-as-Objects-Analysis (ISOA): Extensions of an Approach to Studying Short-Term Developmental Processes by Analyzing Typical Patterns. International Journal of Behavioral Development 2012, 36, 237–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vargha, A.; Torma, B.; Bergman, L.R. ROPstat: A General Statistical Package Useful for Conducting Person-Oriented Analyses. Journal for Person-Oriented Research 2015, 1, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, S.G.; Finch, J.F.; Curran, P.J. Structural Equation Models with Nonnormal Variables: Problems and Remedies. In Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications; Sage Publications, Inc: Thousand Oaks, CA, US, 1995; pp. 56–75. ISBN 978-0-8039-5317-8. [Google Scholar]
- Castro, M.; Chambers, E. Consumer Avoidance of Insect Containing Foods: Primary Emotions, Perceptions and Sensory Characteristics Driving Consumers Considerations. Foods 2019, 8, 351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tuccillo, F.; Marino, M.G.; Torri, L. Italian Consumers’ Attitudes towards Entomophagy: Influence of Human Factors and Properties of Insects and Insect-Based Food. Food Res Int 2020, 137, 109619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Egolf, A.; Siegrist, M.; Hartmann, C. How People’s Food Disgust Sensitivity Shapes Their Eating and Food Behaviour. Appetite 2018, 127, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruby, M.B.; Rozin, P. Disgust, Sushi Consumption, and Other Predictors of Acceptance of Insects as Food by Americans and Indians. Food Quality and Preference 2019, 74, 155–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaeger, S.R.; Chheang, S.L.; Roigard, C.M.; Cardello, A.V. Individual Differences in Food Neophobia and Private Body Consciousness Influence Product-Elicited Emotional Valence and Arousal. Food Quality and Preference 2022, 99, 104566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ares, G.; Mawad, F.; Giménez, A.; Maiche, A. Influence of Rational and Intuitive Thinking Styles on Food Choice: Preliminary Evidence from an Eye-Tracking Study with Yogurt Labels. Food Quality and Preference 2014, 31, 28–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lammers, P.; Ullmann, L.M.; Fiebelkorn, F. Acceptance of Insects as Food in Germany: Is It about Sensation Seeking, Sustainability Consciousness, or Food Disgust? Food Quality and Preference 2019, 77, 78–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cicatiello, C.; De Rosa, B.; Franco, S.; Lacetera, N. Consumer Approach to Insects as Food: Barriers and Potential for Consumption in Italy. British Food Journal 2016, 118, 2271–2286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szakály, Z.; Kovács, B.; Soós, M.; Kiss, M.; Balsa-Budai, N. Adaptation and Validation of the Food Neophobia Scale: The Case of Hungary. Foods 2021, 10, 1766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tuorila, H.; Lähteenmäki, L.; Pohjalainen, L.; Lotti, L. Food Neophobia among the Finns and Related Responses to Familiar and Unfamiliar Foods. Food Quality and Preference 2001, 12, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, T.D.; Lindsey, S.; Schooler, T.Y. A Model of Dual Attitudes. Psychological Review 2000, 107, 101–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
|
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).