Preprint
Article

Behavior Coding of Adolescent and Therapy Dog Interactions During a Social Stress Task

Altmetrics

Downloads

67

Views

48

Comments

0

A peer-reviewed article of this preprint also exists.

Submitted:

30 October 2024

Posted:

31 October 2024

You are already at the latest version

Alerts
Abstract
Youth mental health interventions incorporating trained therapy animals are increasingly popular, but more research is needed to understand the specific interactive behaviors between participants and therapy dogs. Understanding the role of these interactive behaviors is important for supporting both intervention efficacy and animal welfare and well-being. The goal of this study was to develop ethograms to assess interactive behaviors (including both affiliative and stress-related behaviors) of participants and therapy dogs during a social stress task, explore the relationship between human and dog behaviors, and assess how these behaviors may vary between experimental conditions with varying levels of physical contact with the therapy dog. Using video data from a previous experimental study (n = 50 human-therapy dog interactions, n = 25 control group), we successfully developed behavioral ethograms that could be used with a high degree of interrater reliability. Results indicated differences between experimental condition in dog and human behaviors based on whether participants were interacting with a live or a stuffed dog, and whether they were allowed to touch the dog. These findings suggest that physically interacting with a live dog may be an important feature of these interventions, with participants demonstrating increased positive behaviors such as laughing and smiling in these conditions. Dog behaviors also varied based on whether they were in the touching/petting condition of the study. Future research should focus on identifying specific patterns of interactive behaviors between dogs and humans that predict anxiolytic outcomes.
Keywords: 
Subject: Social Sciences  -   Psychology

1. Behavior Coding of Adolescent and Therapy Dog Interactions During a Social Stress Task

Therapeutic interventions and services incorporating trained therapy animals (often referred to as animal-assisted interventions or services; AAI/AAS) are popular as a strategy for supporting individuals experiencing anxiety. AAI/AAS typically involves partnering with trained therapy animals for the purpose of meeting therapeutic goals [1]. Research on AAI in mental health contexts has indicated that these interventions can be effective in supporting treatment outcomes [2,3,4,5] including anxiety reduction [6,7]. Interventions integrating animals also can promote motivation to engage in therapy [3]. Furthermore, there is evidence that contact with animals more generally in a variety of therapeutic and non-therapeutic settings (including both AAI and pets in the home) can buffer cardiovascular responses to stress [8,9].
However, despite the promise of therapeutic approaches integrating animals, it is still unclear exactly how interacting with animals can promote anxiety reduction, and the degree to which these individual types of interactions might impact intervention efficacy. For example, there is a need for research that identifies the specific mechanisms of action by which interacting with animals can produce therapeutic outcomes. By nature, interactions with therapy animals can vary widely based on the individual human and animal participants. Yet, there is little knowledge about how specific interactions may produce anxiolytic effects, which is critical to designing evidence-based interventions. For example, since participants can ideally control the degree of engagement with the therapy animal (choosing to ignore the animal, look at the animal, talk to the animal, and/or touch the animal), as well as animals having choice in engaging with the human participants (choosing to physically interact, leaving the interaction), it is possible that the degree to which youth and animals choose to engage in the interaction may impact any potential benefits.
One hypothesized mechanism for the anxiety-reducing effects of contact with therapy animals is that the interaction with the animal provides social support. In other contexts, social support has been found to reduce the effects of laboratory-based stressors [10]. Humans rely on social support to evaluate stressors [11], and perceptions of the strength of one’s social relationships contributes significantly to how stress is perceived and managed. When interacting with an animal, a relationship is formed that can become a part of an individual’s representation of social resources and social support network. Strong attachment or connection to a pet has been associated with adaptive coping to stress [12], but this mechanism has not been assessed as extensively in interventions where relationships with the therapy animal may be transitory. There is little empirical evidence demonstrating how specific interactions relate to immediate or global anxiolytic responses.
Another possible mechanism for how specific interactions can reduce anxiety is physical touch. Prior research on interpersonal touch between humans [13] shows that touch can impact heart rate [14] and reduce pain perception [15]. Initial research on the role of human-dog physical contact suggests this type of touch may be an important element of the interaction [16,17], and physical touching of a dog has been associated with lower cortisol in children during stressors [18]. Interestingly, one study found that contact with an animal was associated with lower anxiety than contact with a human friend [19], which further suggests that physical touch could be an important factor beyond social support for reducing anxiety. However, little is known about how much and what type of touch during these interactions is optimal, and what the temporal dynamics are for physical touch.
In addition, there has been relatively little research exploring how the behaviors of therapy animals can influence treatment efficacy within AAIs. Dogs have evolved with humans (for a review, see [20] and [21] and research has demonstrated how attuned they can be to human presence and emotion [22,23,24,25]. For adolescents experiencing acute anxiety in a therapeutic setting, a therapy dog proactively initiating interaction may be the prompt an anxious teen needs to engage with the dog and potentially reduce their anxiety. In addition, a therapy dog’s affiliative or engaging behaviors could create a feedback loop that would sustain the interaction longer and ultimately increase the efficacy of the intervention. Yet there has been little research examining the role and specific behaviors of the dog in a therapeutic setting and how those may relate to treatment efficacy. Research examining the specific responses of therapy dogs working with anxious clients is needed to understand if, and how, dogs may facilitate and improve therapeutic outcomes.
However, there also may be negative effects of therapy animal behavior on treatment efficacy and the ethical considerations of integrating animals into healthcare services. In recent years, there has been an increase in research focusing on dog stress in the context of therapeutic interventions, primarily measured via behavioral ethograms and salivary cortisol. While some studies have shown no substantive impact of participating in interventions on dog stress behaviors [26,27,28,29], others have found evidence of subtle stress behaviors [30]. The effects of stress behaviors, even subtle ones, on both the therapy animal and the client are not clear. Therapy dogs experiencing stress in a therapeutic setting may display stress-linked behaviors such as lip licking, avoiding the client, seeking comfort from their handler, gaze and touch avoidance, and, in more severe cases, trembling and hiding, among others [29,30]. A therapy dog who avoids gaze and contact with a participant may be ineffective at best, but such behaviors may, at worst, be interpreted by anxious youth as personally directed at them, further exacerbating their anxiety (in addition to the detrimental stress of the animal participants that these behaviors may reflect). Additional research is needed to examine specific stress-linked behaviors of animals displayed during AAIs, and adolescents’ reaction to them (in terms of both observable behavior and physiological responses), in order to understand the effect on the outcome of AAIs. Although therapy animal handlers are generally trained to identify stress in their animals, the degree to which even minor stress behaviors can impact the intervention is unknown. Understanding these behaviors in more detail is critical to supporting ethical AAI/AAS that respects both human and animal well-being.
As a first step towards understanding how specific human and dog behaviors may contribute to the efficacy of interventions incorporating dogs for anxiety, there is a need for developing behavioral coding tools that can be used in this specific setting. Previous ethogram work (e.g., [28]) has focused on dog behaviors; this study aims to extend this prior work by including human stress and affiliative behaviors and coding human and dog interaction in an integrative way. Using previously collected video data from an experimental protocol involving adolescents participating in a social stress task [31], the objective of this study is to 1) develop ethograms for assessing specific individual and interactive behaviors (including both affiliative and stress-related behaviors) of participants and therapy dogs during an anxiety-inducing situation, 2) assess if human and dog behaviors are correlated in this setting, and 3) explore how these behaviors may differ across conditions with regard to level of contact with the dog (no dog present, dog present but no physical contact with participant, dog present and participant allowed physical contact). This study will allow for a more nuanced understanding of how people and therapy dogs interact during a situation involving anxiety, which will set the stage for future research that can uncover how specific interactions may be best suited to producing anxiolytic effects and reducing the risk of stress in therapy dogs.

2. Materials and Methods

This study used existing data collected as part of a larger study [31] that tested the effects of interacting with a therapy dog for youth with social anxiety. The data used in this study include videos of experimental sessions where therapy dogs (along with their handlers) were interacting with adolescent participants. The experimental procedure is outlined below. The original experimental procedures were approved by the Tufts University Social Behavioral Educational Research Institutional Review Board (protocol # 1702004) and Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol # G2017-09). The original study was also registered on ClinicalTrials.gov (ID: NCT03249116). The video coding procedures for the analyses reported in this paper were additionally reviewed and approved by the Tufts University Social Behavioral Educational Research Institutional Review Board (protocol #1599). Preliminary results from an initial set of coding from this study were presented at a conference prior to publication [32].

2.1. Participants

Video data were collected during an experiment in which researchers enrolled 75 adolescents ages 13-17 (76% female; 77% pet owners) on a continuum of social anxiety. Individuals were screened for social anxiety using the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A; [33]). Parental consent and youth assent were obtained for the screening process. Based on existing research suggesting the assessment of social anxiety on a continuum [34], participants were stratified to select a sample of individuals who range across low (n = 18), mid (n = 22), and high social anxiety (n =35) using the cutoffs recommended by La Greca [33]. Exclusion criteria included fear of or allergy to dogs. Participants were randomized into one of three conditions: 1) social + physical interaction with a live, trained therapy dog (n = 25) 2) social interaction (no physical contact) with a live, trained therapy dog (n = 25); and 3) stuffed toy dog (active control condition; n = 25).

Therapy Dogs

All dog/handler teams were members of Tufts Paws for People and evaluated and registered through the Pet Partners® therapy animal organization. Per the Pet Partners® guidelines, all dogs were on a 6-foot leash for the duration of the experimental sessions. All animal handlers had completed a handler training course and their animals passed a rigorous evaluation (with re-evaluation every 2 years) to meet training, safety, and health standards. The therapy dogs were bathed/groomed 24 hours prior to participating in the study, and the teams were covered by comprehensive liability insurance through Pet Partners®. The dogs were always accompanied by their handlers who were trained to recognize stress and discomfort signals in their dogs. Study procedures stipulated that if any dog was deemed by their handler or the research team as becoming stressed by the visit, the interaction would be discontinued, and an alternate team would be substituted for future participants; however, no instances of this occurred. In total, four dogs were used for this study, all <30 pounds in weight (n = 3 female; n = 1 male), ranging from 8 to 13 years.

2.2. Procedure

Experimental Procedure

The primary task all participants completed was the Trier Social Stress Task for Children (TSST-C), which involves six distinct phases: baseline, anticipation, preparation, speech, mental math, and recovery [35]. The TSST-C has been used extensively as a robust and reliable method for inducing social stress, and variants have been used in conjunction with animal interaction (e.g., [19]). The TSST-C was originally validated for use in 10–14 year-old youth [35], but the protocol used in our study was a specific adaptation validated for use in adolescents up to 17 years old [36]. The TSST-C involves a public speaking task and a mental arithmetic performance challenge. Participants in all three conditions listened to an experimenter speak for several minutes about a therapy dog and saw a photo of the dog, in order to further control for the novelty effects of an animal stimulus and to provide a baseline rest period (20 minutes). After a baseline period, participants were told about the stress task (anticipation). Then participants had a 5-minute preparation period, after which they were asked to speak on an academic topic for 5 minutes (e.g., discuss a historical figure, give a plot summary and interpretation of a book of their choice). Participants were then asked to complete a serial subtraction mental arithmetic task for 5 minutes, with difficulty level adjusted for age. Finally, there was a 30-minute recovery period.
In the control condition (1), participants listened to the verbal presentation but there was no animal present. Instead, there was a stuffed toy dog to serve as active control [18] and a person (to mirror the therapy dog handler). In both animal interaction conditions, one therapy dog and their handler accompanied the participant during all phases of the TSST-C. In the social interaction condition (2), participants were told that the dog will be present, and they were able to socially interact with it at any point during the experiment (i.e., talking), but they were not permitted to touch the dog. In the social + physical interaction condition (3), participants were told that the dog would be next to them and they were encouraged to interact socially and touch the dog during the experiment.
Animal handlers were instructed to provide consistently minimal verbal contact, to reduce the confounding effects of handler variability and distraction from the TSST-C tasks. To simulate a typical AAI/S environment, the handler remained in proximity to the dog to monitor their behavior but did not interfere with the TSST-C tasks in any way. All sessions were videotaped in their entirety using two cameras at consistent angles to capture both participant and dog behavior.

2.3. Video Coding Procedure

To code the videos for dog and human stress and affiliative behaviors, we used Noldus Observer XT 14 software with advanced analysis and multiple media modules to analyze video files from the experiment involving adolescents and therapy animals. All sessions were videotaped from two angles to ensure high quality images of both the participants and the therapy dogs. Researchers used an ethogram to code social, physical, and verbal contact between participants and therapy dogs as well as independent movements and behaviors. Each video was coded using standardized coding manuals to train raters, code videos, and assess inter-rater reliability regularly throughout the coding process.

2.4. Measures

2.4.1. Behavior Coding

We developed dog and human behavior ethograms for this study based on prior research on both dogs [26,30,37,38,39,40,41,42,43]) and humans [44,45,46,47], including past research in AAIs [26,30,48] and behavior coding within the TSST-C paradigm [49]. The ethograms were developed by reviewing prior literature and generating a list of behaviors associated with movement, stress/stress coping, and affiliative/contact seeking interactions, and specific operational definitions were created for each of these behaviors. Behaviors were coded as duration (in seconds) or count (frequency), depending on the nature of the behavior. The draft ethogram was reviewed by the research team and edited through an iterative process including testing with non-study videos. Next, the ethograms were tested on two study videos that were not used as part of the study analyses. The ethograms were further refined as part of this process, including adding and removing behaviors and creating more specific operational definitions for the behaviors until adequate inter-rater reliability could be reached. Behaviors with continued poor reliability after training were dropped from analyses.
Two coders rated the videos, and they achieved excellent interrater reliability for the final analytic behaviors (Intraclass correlation [ICCI] using a two-way random effects for consistency agreement model = .81 to 1.00, M = .95, for human behaviors in nine videos, ICC = .62 to 1.00, M = .93, for dog behaviors in 50 videos). One set of codes was used for analytic purposes.

2.4.2. Adolescent Behaviors

Adolescent behaviors were coded in several broad categories (see Table 1 for full ethogram). Behaviors included different types of vocalizations, facial movements, eye activity, head position, and body movement (including stress-linked behaviors such as fidgeting and nail biting, as well as body positioning relative to the therapy dog). Behaviors that were related to the experimental task were coded (such as math or speech task), as well as the participant’s location in the room, however, these behaviors were not included in the analyses and so are not included in Table 1.

2.4.3. Dog Behaviors

Similarly, dog behaviors were coded into categories related to locomotion (e.g., jumping, walking), body position (e.g., laying alert, sitting, location), tail position, body actions (including stress behaviors such as panting, whining), and dog behavior towards participant (e.g., head towards participant). See Table 2 for full ethogram.

2.5. Data Analysis

From the initial set of 26 human behaviors (Table 1) and 91 dog behaviors (Table 2) that were coded, one human and 36 dog behaviors were excluded from analysis as they were not exhibited by any participant/dog or were observed for less than one second (the latter behaviors tended to be coded inconsistently). One human and ten dog behaviors had poor interrater reliability (kappas < .60) or were not detectable due to audio quality or camera angles so were excluded. Finally, 17 dog behaviors were excluded because they were cued by the handler or participant or directed toward the handler. Table 2 and Table 3 identify excluded behaviors. These exclusions left a total of 24 human behaviors and 28 dog behaviors included in the analysis.
SPSS v29 was used for all analyses. P values < .05 were considered significant. Descriptive statistics (mean, median, minimum, maximum) were calculated for the total sample and within each group. Given the significant departures of the behavioral data from normality, non-parametric tests were used. Related-Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank Tests compared differences in frequencies or durations of some behaviors, such as when participants spoke to the dog versus the handler or experimenter. Independent Samples Kruskal-Wallis tests were calculated to detect significant differences in behavioral data by experimental group although Independent Samples Mann-Whitney U tests were used when just the two groups with a live dog were compared. Visual inspection revealed that the distributions of the behavioral data were not similar for all groups, so mean ranks of distributions were used for these statistical tests. For significant Kruskal-Wallis tests, post-hoc pairwise comparisons adjusted with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons were used to identify group differences. To aid in the interpretation of the statistical tests, means are reported with medians, although test results are based on mean ranks. Spearman’s Rho determined the correlation between human and dog behavioral data.
In order to reduce the large number of behaviors used in the analyses to a more manageable number, Principal Components Analyses (PCA) were separately conducted for human and dog behaviors. To determine the suitability of the data for PCA, correlations amongst the behavior variables were examined and any behaviors which did not correlate with at least one other behavior at +/-.30 or higher were excluded from PCA. Additionally, behaviors with an anti-matrix correlation < .50 were excluded. The overall Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measures for the final human and dog behavior PCAs were > .70, considered suitable for factor analysis [50] and both PCAs generated significant Barlett’s test of sphericity (p < .05). The scree plot along with eigenvalues were used to determine the number of components. Varimax orthogonal rotations were used to help with interpretation. Upon review, any behaviors which loaded on two or more components at +/-.40 or higher and were within +/-.10 loading on the second component were excluded and the PCA was re-run. The final models were deemed interpretable so the final component solutions were retained. Component scores were calculated by averaging the behavior variables with factor loadings of +/-.40 or higher. Cronbach’s alpha was calculated for each component, which identified several components with low internal reliability; upon review, it was determined that these components were made up of both count and duration behaviors, which differed in the range of values. Behavior variables in the final models were then converted to z scores and the PCAs were rerun. Component scores and Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were recalculated using the standardized behavior variables; Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were adequate for the standardized component scores, therefore, the component scores based on the standardized behavior variables were used for subsequent analyses using the component scores.

3. Results

3.1. Participant Behavior

A total of 24 behaviors were observed for at least one second or at least one time in the human participants. Table 3 presents descriptive statistics for the duration (s) or number of times a behavior was observed by experimental condition. Fidgeting was observed the most, followed by participants’ orienting their head toward the dog. Eight behaviors (bite lip, close eyes, cross arms, cry, general scratch, sit on hands, talks to dog, talks to self) were seen infrequently enough to result in median durations or counts of 0. The most common stress-linked behaviors displayed by the human participants were fidgeting, moving their mouth or tongue internally, and playing with their hair, all with median durations of over 30 seconds. Repositioning also was a frequently displayed behavior that is linked to stress (Med = 87 times). Other less commonly seen stress-linked behaviors included touching their face, grimacing or displaying facial tension, yawning, biting their nails, biting their lips, crying, and scratching. Participants displayed orientation or affiliative behaviors toward the dog including positioning their head toward the dog, touching the dog (even when directed not to touch the dog based on experimental condition), and leaning toward the dog. Talking to the dog was observed infrequently (Table 3). Participants talked to the experimenter or handler longer than they talked to the dog (talking to dog vs experimenter: z = -5.50, p < .001; talking to dog vs handler: z = -7.06, p < .001), a difference that was seen even when the two conditions with a live dog were pooled (talking to dog vs experimenter: z = -5.45, p < .001; talking to dog vs handler: z = -5.44, p < .001).
Participants in the different experimental conditions displayed significant differences in the duration or number of times they exhibited seven behaviors (Table 3, Figure 1). Participants in the control group exhibited a grimace or facial tension for significantly less time than those in the social or social + physical groups, H(2) = 8.37, p = .015. When orienting toward the dog, participants in the control group faced toward the stuffed dog significantly less than participants in the social or social + physical groups faced the live dog, H(2) = 45.76, p < .001. Participants in the control group laughed significantly less than did participants in the other two groups where live dogs were present, H(2) = 12.88, p = .002. Examining leaning toward the dog, the median duration for all groups differed significantly from all other groups, with participants in the social + physical group having a higher median duration, followed by participants in the social group, and then those in the control group, H(2) = 38.34, p < .001. Participants in the control group smiled significantly less than those in the social + physical group, H(2) = 9.12, p = .010. Finally, looking at interactions with the dog, participants in the control group talked to the dog significantly less than those in the other groups, H(2) = 25.11, p < .001. In terms of touching the dog, as expected due to the experimental conditions, participants in the social + physical group touched the therapy dog significantly longer than participants in the other groups, H(2) = 50.98, p < .001. Interestingly, even though participants in the social group were instructed not to touch the therapy dog, some were observed doing just that, albeit significantly less than participants in the social + physical group. Participants in the control group were not given instructions about touching the stuffed dog, and some of them did do so. There were no other significant differences for participant behavior by experimental group, p > .05.

3.2. Therapy Dog Behavior

A total of 28 behaviors were observed for at least one second or at least one time in the four therapy dogs. Table 4 presents descriptive statistics for the duration (s) or number of times a behavior was observed by experimental condition. Laying down and laying alert (head up while laying down) occurred for the greatest total duration and shifting position was the most frequently counted behavior. Twenty out of the 28 coded behaviors were seen infrequently enough to result in median durations or counts of 0, including common stress-linked behaviors such as panting, stretching, shaking off, or yawning (Table 4). The most commonly displayed behavior by the dogs related to the participant was positioning their head toward the participant, although the median duration for this behavior was just 10 seconds. All other orientation or affiliative behaviors toward the participant such as being near the participant, pawing at or putting paws on, or sniffing the participant had median durations or counts of 0.
Therapy dogs in the two experimental conditions with live dogs displayed significant differences in the duration or number of times they exhibited 12 behaviors (Table 4, Figure 2). Dogs in the social + physical condition exhibited longer durations or higher counts of the following behaviors: ears twitching (U = 419.5, p = .036), laying down (U = 446.0, p = .010), and shifting their head (U = 499.0, p < .001). Dogs in the social condition displayed longer durations or higher counts of the following behaviors: circling (U = 225.0, p = .019), being near the participant (U = 203.0, p = .031), scratching an object or the floor (U = 225.0, p = .005), sniffing the participant (U = 211.0, p = .046), sniffing an object or the floor (U = 169.0, p = .005), standing (U = 124.0, p < .001), stretching (U = 224.0, p = .026), wagging tail (U = 176.5, p = .005), and walking (U = 20.0, p < .001). There were no other significant differences for dog behavior by experimental group, p > .05.

3.3. Participant and Dog Behavior Correlations

Table S1 presents the results of the correlation analysis for all participants for 672 human-dog behavior pairings. Table S2 presents the results of the same analysis for just participants in the social + physical condition, where both participant and therapy dog were able to move freely. Table 5 summarizes the correlation results for only participants in the social + physical condition, presenting significant correlations of .30 or higher in grouped ranges based on Spearman’s rho coefficients (+/-.30-+/-.49, +/-.50+). There were some potentially stress-linked behaviors in humans and dogs that were correlated, such as human grimace and dog circling, human arms crossed and dog barking, human biting lip and dog jerking/twitching, human playing with hair and dog licking the floor or an object, and human scratching and dog grooming. Interestingly, there were several instances of potential deflection behaviors on the part of the dog, such as a correlation between participants yawning and the dog looking at the participant, and the human laughing (which can be nervous laughter) and the dog shaking off.

3.4. Participant Behavior Principal Components Analysis

A principal components analysis (PCA) was calculated using eight behaviors which met analysis requirements (correlation coefficients +/-.30+ with at least one other behavior variable, anti-image correlation coefficients >=.50, rotated factor loadings +/-.40+ on one component or, if loading on two component, >+/-.10 difference between loadings). Behavioral data were converted to z scores, given the range in values between duration and count data which had resulted in low internal reliability. All PCA results and subsequent component scores are based on the standardized variables. The overall Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure was 0.73, which Kaiser [50] classified as “middling”. Bartlett's test of sphericity was statistically significant (p < .001), indicating that the data were likely suitable for PCA.
PCA identified three components that had eigenvalues greater than one, explaining 34.4%, 21.8%, and 16.4% of the total variance in the rotated model, for a total of 72.6% (Table 6). Visual inspection of the scree plot supported the three-component solution, which was deemed interpretable. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranged from .91 (component 1) to .41 (component 3), with only component 1 achieving what is generally considered an acceptable value of .70 or higher [51,52].

3.5. Therapy Dog Behavior Principal Components Analysis

Using the same process as for participant behavior, a principal components analysis (PCA) was calculated using 10 standardized behavior variables which met analysis requirements. The overall Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure was 0.77, which Kaiser [50] classified as “middling”. Bartlett's test of sphericity was statistically significant (p < .001), indicating that the data were likely suitable for PCA.
PCA identified three components that had eigenvalues greater than one, explaining 34.5%, 24.0%, and 12.5% of the total variance in the rotated model, for a total of 71.0% (Table 7). Visual inspection of the scree plot supported the three-component solution, which was deemed interpretable. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranged from .86 (component 1) to .30 (component 3), with components 1 and 2 achieving acceptable reliability.

3.6. Participant and Therapy Dog Behavior Component Scores by Experimental Group

Table 8 presents descriptive statistics for participant and dog behavior component scores based on standardized behavior variables by experimental group. There was a significant difference in participant component 3 scores by group, with participants in the social + physical group exhibiting those behaviors (smiling and touching the dog) more frequently than participants in the social or control groups, H(2) = 29.93, p < .001. Therapy dogs in the social group had higher scores on dog component 1 (wagging, grooming, stretching, shifting position, and laying alert) than dogs in the social + physical group, exhibiting those behaviors more frequently, U = 188.0, p = .016.

3.7. Correlations between Participant and Therapy Dog Behavior Component Scores

For the total sample, there were two weak but significant Spearman’s rho correlation coefficients between participant and dog behavior component scores: human component 3 (smiling/touching the dog) and dog component 1 (wagging, grooming, stretching, shifting position, laying alert); and human component 3 and dog component 2 (pawing person, ear twitching, head towards participant). When examining correlations for just the social + physical group, where both participant and dog were free to move around, there was one moderate negative but significant correlation participant component 3 (smiling/touching the dog) and dog component 3 (being near to the participant and jerking/twitching) (Table 8).

4. Discussion

The goal of this study was to develop ethograms for assessing human and dog behaviors during a social stress task, and to evaluate if these behaviors were correlated and if they varied across experimental condition. We were able to successfully develop the ethograms with high interrater reliability and found that a number of the behaviors were correlated and varied across experimental condition, as expected. With additional research, this ethogram may be a useful tool for assessing participant and therapy dog interactive behaviors in an experimental setting.
The behavior coding results indicated some interesting differences between experimental conditions (social + physical involving physical contact with the therapy dog, social only with no touching of the therapy dog, and the control group with a stuffed dog). The participants spent more time facing towards the live therapy dog as well as more time smiling in the therapy dog conditions as compared to the control condition stuffed dog, which underscores the positive engagement that is associated with interacting with a live dog, as found in other prior research [48,53]. Similar to prior research [54], interacting with a dog may be associated with more positive affect, even during a stressful task. Somewhat unexpectedly, we found that participants grimaced and showed facial tension less frequently in control group. However, because this group also smiled less, it is possible that these facial behaviors are conflated, and this finding should be explored more in future research.
Participants in the condition with physical contact also spent more time leaning towards the dog, which could indicate that physical touch is an important component of how participants interact with the dogs, aligning with prior research underscoring the role of touch in AAI/S [16]. There were also some differences in behaviors between dogs in the condition where they were being touched vs. not touched. For example, dogs exhibited more ear twitching and head shifting in the social + physical condition, although these behaviors were negatively correlated with the amount they were being touched. Dogs in the “social only” condition were more frequently close to participants, sniffing participants, and wagging their tail. It is possible that these dogs were trying to engage with the participants in the absence of the adolescents patting or touching them. Some dogs could find it mildly stressful to not interact with a nearby human, especially therapy dogs trained for that exact type of interaction. For example, prior research has found touch synchrony between handlers and dogs [55], a concept that could be explored between participants and therapy dogs. Further research should explore in more detail whether there are dog-specific factors or preferences to amount of touch and whether some dogs move more during physical touch than others and if/how/when they solicit touching, especially as these variations could contribute to therapeutic outcomes.
In addition to assessing behaviors across condition, we also explored correlations between human and dog behaviors. We found some patterns of correlations between potentially stress-linked behaviors, such as participant grimacing and dog circling, participant’s arms crossed and dog barking, participant biting lip and dog jerking/twitching, participant playing with their hair and dog licking the floor or object, human scratching and dog grooming, and human yawning and dog facing away from the person. While some of these behaviors are not always linked to stress, it does suggest that there could be patterns where there are reciprocal stress behaviors. In particular, the co-occurrence of human scratching and dog grooming is an interesting finding that should be explored in future research to see if these patterns are replicated in other circumstances. Counterintuitively, we found few correlations between affiliative behaviors in the social + physical group, where both participant and dog could move freely and choose to interact or not. However, although they were allowed to interact or not, this interaction did take place in a very structured experimental setting where the participant was completing various tasks, and therefore there was not as much opportunity for spontaneous interaction as in more naturalistic settings. Together, these findings all suggest further research on synchronization of dog and human behavior and the conditions under which synchrony takes place, as found with familiar pet dogs [56].
Because the ethogram included a large range of behaviors, we employed PCA as a method of data reduction to attempt to create more interpretable factors that would be easier to use as a tool for assessment. We did find three components of behaviors for both humans and dogs. For human behaviors, one component included a group of “talking behaviors,” another component was related to fidgeting-type behaviors often associated with stress (biting nails, playing with hair, touching face), and another related to positive engagement (smiling and touching the dog). For dog behaviors, the conceptual nature of the components was a little bit less clear; the first component included wagging, grooming, stretching, shifting position, and laying alert, which seems to be a combination of friendly, relaxation, and mild stress behaviors. The second component included behaviors potentially responsive to the participant, such as pawing at the person, ear twitching, and head towards the participant. The third component was only two factors – being near to the participant and jerking/twitching, which could potentially capture dogs who were not enjoying proximity to the participant, though this is a tenuous interpretation and needs to be explored in more detail. In addition, the reliability of the third components for both human and dog behaviors was poor, though they each contained only two items which is challenging for establishing good reliability even with two items that are related to the same concept. Future research should assess if these components are correlated with outcomes such as self-reported anxiety or psychophysiological responses to stress in order to determine if they are useful as an assessment tool.

4.1. Limitations

Our study was limited by our relatively small sample of dogs and the lack of variability in many of the stress-linked behaviors. This sample of dogs was highly trained and experienced, and handlers were instructed to stop any interactions where stress behaviors occur (although it should be noted that this did not happen at all during this study). Many of the stress behaviors, especially the more obvious/problematic behaviors, were not observed at all in this sample. This limitation highlights a methodological conundrum for studying therapy animal behavior in research settings. It is not ethical to integrate therapy dogs into settings when they are not prepared or they are demonstrating stress behaviors, and as researchers we have a responsibility to ensure that therapy dogs participating in our research are not, to the best of our knowledge, experiencing stress. However, it then makes it challenging to validate measurement tools for assessing dog stress in these settings. Nonetheless, we feel that measuring subtle stress behaviors is still worthwhile in moving towards a more nuanced understanding of how dogs indicate preferences for engaging in therapeutic services.
Another limitation of this study is the specific experimental protocol. While this presented an excellent opportunity to validate behavior coding in a highly controlled and standardized setting, we recognize that these behaviors and patterns may not translate to all adolescent/dog interactions in therapeutic contexts. We encourage other researchers to use our ethogram if it is useful to them in other settings, which will enable expansion of validating the tool.

4.2. Conclusions and Future Directions

In conclusion, this study successfully developed ethograms for measuring therapy dog and adolescent behavior in the context of a stress task. We did not find evidence of any significant or severe stress behaviors in the therapy dogs. There were differences across experimental conditions in dog behaviors, suggesting that physical touch and freedom to interact physically with participants may be associated with patterns of dog behavior as they orient towards participants. Adolescents who were in the control group with no live dog laughed and smiled significantly less, suggesting that interactions with a live therapy dog may be associated with indicators of positive affect.
This study was successful in developing a behavioral coding tool that can be used to capture interactive behaviors between adolescents with anxiety and therapy dogs. Increasing the number of tools available to capture human and dog behaviors simultaneously will allow us to both better assess the efficacy of interventions involving therapy dogs as well as optimize welfare and well-being for our animal partners. Future research should work to assess the association between these behaviors and therapeutic outcomes such as anxiety and affect, and determine if the factors identified through the PCA are useful groupings for predicting outcomes. Similarly, we should assess if these behaviors are correlated with other physiological measures of dog stress such as cortisol or oxytocin. In addition to assessing these outcomes, another useful area of exploration is assessing nuance in patterns of behaviors. For example, are there some situations where more or less of a particular behavior or set of behaviors is more or less beneficial? Are there certain dogs with particular behavior types who have preferences for types of interactions with participants? Future research should examine a therapy dog’s possible synchrony as well as sensitivity to the participant’s affective state and stress levels which suggest a more active role for the therapy dog is possible, one that may be beneficial to AAI outcomes. There is an incredible amount of nuance in these behaviors, which means more sensitive and flexible measurement tools are needed, enabling further exploration of these complex questions.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at the website of this paper posted on Preprints.org, Table S1: Correlations Between Participant and Therapy Dog Behaviors, Total Sample (N = 75). Table S2: Correlations Between Participant and Therapy Dog Behaviors, Social + Physical Group (N = 25).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, SDG and MKM; Methodology, SDG and MKM; Formal Analysis, SDG, KD, EAR; Resources, MKM; Data Curation, SDG; Writing – Original Draft Preparation, SDG, MKM; Writing – Review & Editing, KD, EAR; Visualization, SDG, MKM; Supervision, SDG, MKM; Project Administration, MKM; Funding Acquisition, MKM.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development under award number R03HD103300. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The original experimental procedures were approved by the Tufts University Social Behavioral Educational Research Institutional Review Board (protocol # 1702004) and Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol # G2017-09). The video coding procedures for the analyses reported in this paper were additionally reviewed and approved by the Tufts University Social Behavioral Educational Research Institutional Review Board (protocol #1599).

Informed Consent Statement

The analyses conducted in this article were completed on existing video data. Informed consent from parents/guardians and assent from youth participants were obtained as part of the original experimental study.

Data Availability Statement

The data from the original experimental study are openly available in the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/w7k8p/?view_only=965f3d7390d7434895216fe8b88a2160. Video data files are not publicly available due to the identifying nature of the videos (i.e., participant faces). Video code data are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the therapy dog teams from Tufts Paws for People for participating in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

Megan K. Mueller has received research grants from Mars Petcare UK that are unrelated to this study. The authors declare no other conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Jegatheesan, B., et al., The IAHAIO definitions for animal assisted intervention and guideliness for wellness of animals involved in AAI, in Handbook on Animal-Assisted Therapy, A.H. Fine, Editor. 2019, Elsevier. p. 499-504.
  2. Hoagwood, K.E., et al., Animal-Assisted Therapies for Youth with or at risk for Mental Health Problems: A Systematic Review. Appl Dev Sci, 2017. 21(1): p. 1-13. [CrossRef]
  3. Jones, M.G., S.M. Rice, and S.M. Cotton, Incorporating animal-assisted therapy in mental health treatments for adolescents: A systematic review of canine assisted psychotherapy. PLoS One, 2019. 14(1): p. e0210761. [CrossRef]
  4. Nimer, J. and B. Lundahl, Animal-Assisted Therapy: A Meta-Analysis. Anthrozoös, 2007. 20(3): p. 225-238. [CrossRef]
  5. Souter, M.A. and M.D. Miller, Do Animal-Assisted Activities Effectively Treat Depression? A Meta-Analysis. Anthrozoös, 2007. 20(2): p. 167-180. [CrossRef]
  6. Barker, S.B. and K.S. Dawson, The Effects of Animal-Assisted Therapy on Anxiety Ratings of Hospitalized Psychiatric Patients. Psychiatric Services, 1998. 49(6): p. 797-801. [CrossRef]
  7. Barker, S.B., et al., The Effect of an Animal-Assisted Intervention on Anxiety and Pain in Hospitalized Children. Anthrozoös, 2015. 28(1): p. 101-112. [CrossRef]
  8. Allen, K., B.E. Shykoff, and J.L. Izzo Jr, Pet ownership, but not ACE inhibitor therapy, blunts home blood pressure responses to mental stress. Hypertension, 2001. 38(4): p. 815-820. [CrossRef]
  9. Allen, K.M., et al., Presence of human friends and pet dogs as moderators of autonomic responses to stress in women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1991. 61(4): p. 582-589. [CrossRef]
  10. Heinrichs, M., et al., Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological psychiatry, 2003. 54(12): p. 1389-1398. [CrossRef]
  11. Coan, J.A. and D.A. Sbarra, Social Baseline Theory: The Social Regulation of Risk and Effort. Curr Opin Psychol, 2015. 1: p. 87-91. [CrossRef]
  12. Mueller, M.K. and K.S. Callina, Human–Animal Interaction as a Context for Thriving and Coping in Military-Connected Youth: The Role of Pets During Deployment. Applied Developmental Science, 2014. 18(4): p. 214-223. [CrossRef]
  13. Gallace, A. and C. Spence, The science of interpersonal touch: an overview. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 2010. 34(2): p. 246-259. [CrossRef]
  14. Fairhurst, M.T., L. Löken, and T. Grossmann, Physiological and behavioral responses reveal 9-month-old infants’ sensitivity to pleasant touch. Psychological science, 2014. 25(5): p. 1124-1131. [CrossRef]
  15. Mancini, F., et al., Pain relief by touch: a quantitative approach. Pain, 2014. 155(3): p. 635-642. [CrossRef]
  16. Binfet, J.-T., F.L.L. Green, and Z.A. Draper, The Importance of Client–Canine Contact in Canine-Assisted Interventions: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Anthrozoös, 2021: p. 1-22. [CrossRef]
  17. Vormbrock, J.K. and J.M. Grossberg, Cardiovascular effects of human-pet dog interactions. Journal of behavioral medicine, 1988. 11: p. 509-517. [CrossRef]
  18. Beetz, A., et al., Effects of social support by a dog on stress modulation in male children with insecure attachment. Frontiers in psychology, 2012. 3: p. 352. [CrossRef]
  19. Polheber, J.P. and R.L. Matchock, The presence of a dog attenuates cortisol and heart rate in the Trier Social Stress Test compared to human friends. J Behav Med, 2014. 37(5): p. 860-7. [CrossRef]
  20. Miklósi, Á., Dog behaviour, evolution, and cognition. 2014: oUp Oxford.
  21. Thalmann, O., et al., Complete mitochondrial genomes of ancient canids suggest a European origin of domestic dogs. Science, 2013. 342(6160): p. 871-874. [CrossRef]
  22. Albuquerque, N., et al., Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biology letters, 2016. 12(1): p. 20150883. [CrossRef]
  23. Kaminski, J. and M. Nitzschner, Do dogs get the point? A review of dog–human communication ability. Learning and Motivation, 2013. 44(4): p. 294-302. [CrossRef]
  24. Merola, I., E. Prato-Previde, and S. Marshall-Pescini, Dogs' social referencing towards owners and strangers. PloS one, 2012. 7(10): p. e47653. [CrossRef]
  25. Müller, C.A., et al., Dogs can discriminate emotional expressions of human faces. Current Biology, 2015. 25(5): p. 601-605. [CrossRef]
  26. Lee, C.Y., et al., Development of a pilot human-canine ethogram for an animal-assisted education programme in primary schools–A case study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2022. 255: p. 105725. [CrossRef]
  27. Glenk, L.M., et al., Salivary cortisol and behavior in therapy dogs during animal-assisted interventions: A pilot study. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 2014. 9(3): p. 98-106. [CrossRef]
  28. McCullough, A., et al., Physiological and behavioral effects of animal-assisted interventions on therapy dogs in pediatric oncology settings. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2018. 200: p. 86-95. [CrossRef]
  29. Corsetti, S., M. Ferrara, and E. Natoli, Evaluating Stress in Dogs Involved in Animal-Assisted Interventions. Animals (Basel), 2019. 9(10). [CrossRef]
  30. Sarrafchi, A., et al., Effect of human-dog interaction on therapy dog stress during an on-campus student stress buster event. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2022. 253: p. 105659. [CrossRef]
  31. Mueller, M.K., et al., Null effects of therapy dog interaction on adolescent anxiety during a laboratory-based social evaluative stressor. Anxiety Stress Coping, 2021. 34(4): p. 365-380. [CrossRef]
  32. Dowling-Guyer, S., et al., Behavioral coding of adolescent and therapy dog interactions during a social stress task, in International Society for Anthrozoology. 2024: Gloucester, England. [CrossRef]
  33. La Greca, A.M., The Social Anxiety Scales for Children and Adolescents. The Behavior Therapist, 1999.
  34. McNeil, D.W., Terminology and evolution of constructs in social anxiety and social phobia. 2001.
  35. Buske-Kirschbaum, A., et al., Attenuated free cortisol response to psychosocial stress in children with atopic dermatitis. Psychosomatic medicine, 1997. 59(4): p. 419-426. [CrossRef]
  36. Stroud, L.R., et al., Stress response and the adolescent transition: performance versus peer rejection stressors. Dev Psychopathol, 2009. 21(1): p. 47-68. [CrossRef]
  37. Beerda, B., et al., Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs. Applied animal behaviour science, 1998. 58(3-4): p. 365-381. [CrossRef]
  38. Epstein, J., et al., Addressing stress in dogs in shelters through a novel visual and auditory enrichment device. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2021. 236: p. 105215. [CrossRef]
  39. Kateřina, K., et al., Evaluation of cortisol levels and behavior in dogs during animal-assisted interventions in clinical practice. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2024. 277: p. 106321. [CrossRef]
  40. Mariti, C., et al., Effects of petting before a brief separation from the owner on dog behavior and physiology: A pilot study. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 2018. 27: p. 41-46. [CrossRef]
  41. Protopopova, A., et al., In-kennel behavior predicts length of stay in shelter dogs. PloS one, 2014. 9(12): p. e114319. [CrossRef]
  42. Rehn, T. and L.J. Keeling, The effect of time left alone at home on dog welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2011. 129(2-4): p. 129-135. [CrossRef]
  43. Schipper, L.L., et al., The effect of feeding enrichment toys on the behaviour of kennelled dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 2008. 114(1-2): p. 182-195. [CrossRef]
  44. Blöte, A.W., et al., The Speech Performance Observation Scale for Youth (SPOSY): Assessing social performance characteristics related to social anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 2015. 6(2): p. 168-184. [CrossRef]
  45. Rith-Najarian, L.R., et al., The biopsychosocial model of stress in adolescence: self-awareness of performance versus stress reactivity. Stress, 2014. 17(2): p. 193-203. [CrossRef]
  46. Lanteigne, D., J. Glozman, and T. Hollenstein, Self-conscious affect coding manual. Unpublished Coding Manual, 2010.
  47. Fydrich, T., et al., Behavioral assessment of social performance: A rating system for social phobia. Behaviour research and therapy, 1998. 36(10): p. 995-1010. [CrossRef]
  48. Beetz, A., et al., The Effect of a Real Dog, Toy Dog and Friendly Person on Insecurely Attached Children During a Stressful Task: An Exploratory Study. Anthrozoös, 2011. 24(4): p. 349-368. [CrossRef]
  49. Thomassin, K., J. Raftery-Helmer, and J. Hersh, A review of behavioral observation coding approaches for the trier social stress test for children. Frontiers in Psychology, 2018. 9: p. 2610. [CrossRef]
  50. Kaiser, H.F., An index of factorial simplicity. psychometrika, 1974. 39(1): p. 31-36. [CrossRef]
  51. DeVellis, R.F. and C.T. Thorpe, Scale development: Theory and applications. 2021: Sage publications.
  52. Kline, R.B., Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 2023: Guilford publications.
  53. Gee, N.R., et al., Preschoolers' Adherence to Instructions as a Function of Presence of a Dog and Motor Skills Task. Anthrozoös, 2009. 22(3): p. 267-276. [CrossRef]
  54. Janssens, M., et al., The Pet-Effect in Daily Life: An Experience Sampling Study on Emotional Wellbeing in Pet Owners. Anthrozoös, 2020. 33(4): p. 579-588. [CrossRef]
  55. Pirrone, F., et al., Measuring social synchrony and stress in the handler-dog dyad during animal-assisted activities: A pilot study. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 2017. 21: p. 45-52. [CrossRef]
  56. Wanser, S.H., M. MacDonald, and M.A.R. Udell, Dog-human behavioral synchronization: family dogs synchronize their behavior with child family members. Anim Cogn, 2021. 24(4): p. 747-752. [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Human behaviors by experimental condition.
Figure 1. Human behaviors by experimental condition.
Preprints 138060 g001
Figure 2. Dog behaviors by experimental condition.
Figure 2. Dog behaviors by experimental condition.
Preprints 138060 g002
Table 1. Human Behavior Ethogram.
Table 1. Human Behavior Ethogram.
Behavior Description of Behavior Type (Duration (s)/
Count)
Vocalizations
Audible sounds2 Stutters, Ums, Repetitive place holder noises C
Laugh Participant produces a laughing sound C
Talk to experimenter Participant engages in conversation, asks questions to experimenter, or responds to questions from experimenter D
Talk to handler Participant engages in conversation or asks questions to handler D
Talk to self Participant engages in self-oriented conversation D
Facial Movement
Bite lip Lips are between teeth, outwards or inwards D
Grimace/facial tension Furrowed brow, squinted eyes, tight facial muscles, gritted teeth, clenched jaw, pursing lips D
Internal tongue movement Tongue movement inside the mouth, open or closed mouth, biting lip if teeth aren't visible, lip licking, biting the tongue D
Yawn Participant yawns C
Smile Mouth is in an up-turned position C
Eye Activity
Cry Tears are visible, face and mouth may quiver D
Eyes closed Eyes are closed for 2 seconds or longer D
Movement & Activity
Cross arms Both arms are crossed in front of body D
Bite nails Biting nails, finger in mouth, finger tips on lips D
General fidget Foot bouncing, body swaying side to side, pronounced finger movement (entire finger moves, not just tip); Finger/hand tapping, playing with clothing/wrist band. Movement must be 2 seconds or longer; stop code if there is a 2 second pause between instances, otherwise the code can continue running D
General scratch Itching or scratching body some place other than face D
Play with hair Twirling, running fingers through, twisting hair D
Reposition Includes body movements, arm shifts, big positional movements or arm place changes, arm/elbow moves or moves off arm rest, turning hand over (usually seen when arm is at rest on arm rest or lap) C
Sit on Hands Hands are under the thighs or bottom D
Stretch Shoulder shrug, double or single arm stretches, leg stretches, neck stretch (either side) D
Touch face Touching, stroking, scratching the face, rubbing hands on the chin, hand/hands holding the face, handing covering the mouth C
Location in Room
On chair3 Subject is sitting on the chair D
On couch3 Subject is sitting on the couch D
Dog Orientation & Interaction
Head towards dog Head is turned in direction of dog D
Lean away from dog1 Body tilts away from dog D
Lean towards dog Body tilts towards dog, within arm’s length D
Talk to dog Must be audible to coder and clear that the participant is speaking to dog vs speaking in general D
Touch dog Participant initiates contact with hand on dog excluding accidental touching D
Notes: 1Behaviors not exhibited so excluded from analysis. 2Excluded behaviors due to poor kappa (< .60) or poor visibility/audible on recordings. 3Behaviors deemed irrelevant to analyses so excluded.
Table 2. Dog Behavior Ethogram.
Table 2. Dog Behavior Ethogram.
Behavior Description of Behavior Type Duration(s)/Count
Locomotion
Paws on handler1 Dog jumps up and puts forelimbs onto the handler D
Paws on inanimate object3 Dog jumps up and puts forelimbs onto the object D
Paws on participant Dog jumps up and puts forelimbs onto the participant D
Circle Turning in one or more circles prior to sitting or lying down D
Jump2 Front two feet or all four paws off of the floor, either in the air or onto a wall or vertical surface; usually repetitive C
Jump into/out of handler's lap1 Dog jumps off of the floor into the lap of the individual or off of lap to another surface C
Jump into/out of participant's lap2 Dog jumps off of the floor into the lap of the individual or off of lap to another surface C
Jump on/off couch/chair1 Dog jumps on to or off of couch/chair C
Lifted into/off chair/couch/bed1 Handler lifts dog into the dog bed (may be on floor or chair) C
Pace2 Repeated locomotion around something or in fixed route (usually three times or greater) D
Repetitive circling2 Turning in multiple short circles while standing in the same location, not associated with circling prior to sitting or lying down or tail chasing D
Run2 Dog exhibits rapid locomotion from one location in the room to another with the movement forward movement of all four paws D
Walk Forward movement of all four paws resulting in shift of the whole body to a new position in the room D
Body Position
Crouch2 Rapid and pronounced lowering of the posture D
Cued behavior1 Cued behavior initiated by handler’s verbal cue or hand signal C
Lay alert Dog is lying down with stomach in contact with surface but head is raised and/or not down, eyes open if visible D
Lay down Lying down, body in a ventral or lateral position, stomach in contact with a surface, head lowered or down D
Lean away from participant2 Dog leans away from the participant of the study, even if still in contact with participant, increasing distance without changing position D
Lean on handler1 Dog has physical contact with the handler, bearing weight against handler or leaning into hand D
Lean on object2 Dog has physical contact with an object, bearing weight against object D
Lean on participant2 Dog has physical contact with the participant, bearing weight against participant or leaning into hand D
Rolling on back2 Lying on ground with belly up lifting hind leg, or rolling onto back exposing ventral side, including half roll (fine tune - at least two legs in the air) D
Shift within position Whole body or individual body part movement usually when in lying down or sitting including readjustment from one position back to a similar form of that position under 2 seconds (Not including head movements) C
Sit Dog is sitting with front legs extended and hind legs flexed, stomach not in contact with surface D
Stand Dog is standing on all four paws, not moving D
Location in Room
In bed on floor3 Dog is on bed/blanket/mat on the floor for 2 seconds or longer D
In handler's lap1 Dog is in handler's lap for 2 seconds or longer D
In participant’s lap1 Dog is in participant's lap for 2 seconds or longer D
Near handler1 Close to dog handler, some part of dog is within 1.5-2ft of the handler D
Near participant Close to research participant, some part of dog is within 1.5-2ft of the participant D
On chair/couch1 On chair or couch near participant or handler (may or may not be on bed) for 2 seconds or longer D
Tail Position
Chase tail2 Focuses on tail and repeatedly follows, trying to bite or lick tail, generally circling to follow tail D
Tail tucked2 Tail held still and tightly between hind legs, may be curled under genital area or ventral side D
Tail up2 Tail is erect, perpendicular to the body D
Wag tail Repetitive side-to-side movements of the tail; tail generally moves perpendicular to the dog's body D
Body Actions
Tremble/cower2 Shaking of the body or shivering; visible shaking while dog is still or cowering; all or part of the body shaking D
Jerk/twitch Sudden, quick movement with body/limb/skin/tail (usually the tip of the tail moves, movement not from the base of the tail), possibly involuntary movement D
Lift paw Forepaw is lifted into a position of approximately 45 degrees; dog is standing/sitting and lifts one paw D
Paw at person Touches person with paw, and moves paw in the area around the receiver of the action C
Piloerect2 Raising of hair on back anywhere from neck to tail D
Roll over2 Dog rolls completely over, from abdomen being ventral to dorsal to ventral again D
Scratch at object/floor Dog scrapes front claws at object or spot on floor D
Shake off Shaking of whole body, more overall movement than shake head D
Stretch Dog is extending/stretching a part of or whole body D
Head Actions
Ears back2 Ears are back, for 0.5 second or more D
Ears down Ears are tucked down in a non-neutral position, for 0.5 second or more D
Ears up3 Ears are up and alert, for 0.5 second or more D
Ears twitch Sudden, quick non-directional movement of one or both ears C
Eyes closed2 Eyes closed for 2 seconds or more D
Jerk head3 A quick movement of the head, less than 2 seconds, that does not involve other body parts, and is not only ears C
Look away2 Dog was looking at participant/handler and then looked away, deliberate action D
Nudge participant2 Dog pushes part of participant with nose muzzle or head D
Quick look3 Looking somewhere else (not at a person) for less than 2 seconds C
Repeatedly moving head2 Changing head position continuously for 2 seconds or more without directly looking at a person/place D
Shake head Shaking head as if wet, but only head, not a full body shake C
Shift head A movement of the head that does not involve other body parts, is not only ears, is more purposeful than a jerk, does not include body movement C
Side eye2 Dog is looking out of the corner of eye D
Sneeze2 Sneezing C
Sniff handler1 Dog smells handler, determined by active nose twitching and rapid inhalation D
Sniff object/floor Dog smells object, determined by active nose twitching and rapid inhalation D
Sniff participant Dog smells participant, determined by active nose twitching and rapid inhalation D
Tilt head2 Entire head quickly oriented laterally and held stationary for at least 1 second; head tilted to the side, gesture may be done in response to a stimulus or due to dog looking at an object/person C
Head Actions (Gaze)
Head towards back of room3 Dog is focused on the back of the room, by staring/gazing at back of the room for 2 seconds or longer D
Head towards experimenter3 Dog is focused on experimenter, by gazing/staring at experimenter D
Head towards floor3 Dog is focused on floor, by gazing/staring at floor for 2 seconds or longer D
Head towards front of room3 Dog is focused on the front of the room, by staring/gazing at front of the room for 2 seconds or longer D
Head towards handler1 Dog is focused on handler, by gazing/staring at handler D
Head towards participant Dog is focused on participant, by gazing/staring at participant D
Mouth Actions
Bite object2 Contact by teeth to skin or clothing with intention to threaten or harm D
Bite participant2 Contact by teeth to skin or clothing with intention to threaten or harm D
Drool/salivation2 Dog has saliva hanging from the mouth or emanating from the mouth D
Lick handler1 Tongue extends to touch handler before retracting into mouth D
Lick lip/nose2 Tongue licking lips or nose, slowly or in rapid succession; dog is snout licking, tongue visible C
Lick object Tongue extends to touch object before retracting into mouth D
Lick participant2 Tongue extends to touch participant before retracting into mouth D
Lift lip2 Vertical retraction of lips, pulling upper lips back to show full teeth, usually paired with growling C
Mouth open2 Mouth is open, not panting or vocalizing D
Mouthing2 Teeth contact skin of participant with no break D
Open/close mouth rapidly3 Sequential opening and closing of mouth, within a timeframe of 2 seconds or less; not a lip lick or pant behavior C
Pant An increased frequency of inhalation and exhalation combined with opening of the mouth; tongue exposed with audible/or observable breathing; try to watch the abdomen D
Snap at person2 Teeth snap in air towards person and do not touch skin C
Yawn Opens mouth widely and inhales C
Vocalization
Bark Short vocalization of low frequency, sometimes cyclical; rough sound often repeated in quick succession C
Howl2 Long, drawn out voice vocalization of high amplitude D
Whine2 Soft, high-pitched vocalizations; a cyclic vocalization; sustained whimper D
Yelp2 High-pitched vocalization usually occurring in response to a painful or unpleasant stimulus C
Maintenance
Groom Dog bites/nibbles, licks, sniffs, scratches self or performs other non-specific maintenance behaviors D
Dog Handler Actions
Handler gives command/cue1 Handler issues a command or cue, verbally and/or visually, for the dog to behave in a certain way or perform a behavior C
Handler gives dog treat1 Handler gives dog a treat, may be part of a command/cue but could also be without an associated behavior C
Handler speaks to dog1 Must be audible to coder and clear that the participant is speaking to dog vs speaking in general D
Handler touches dog1 Handler initiates contact with hand on dog excluding accidental touching D
Notes: 1Excluded behaviors as directed or influenced by handler or participant. 2Behaviors not exhibited or observed < 1 s so excluded from analysis. 3Excluded behaviors due to poor kappa (< .60) or poor visibility/audible on recordings.
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics of Observed Human Participant Behavior by Experimental Group.
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics of Observed Human Participant Behavior by Experimental Group.
Behavior Total Social + physical Social Control
Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N
Bite Lip (d) 15.3 0.0 0.0 787.2 75 11.0 0.0 0.0 263.0 25 34.7 0.0 0.0 787.2 25 0.2 0.0 0.0 5.6 25
Bite Nails (d) 37.9 1.3 0.0 466.4 75 30.9 3.8 0.0 340.5 25 28.3 1.2 0.0 392.3 25 54.4 0.0 0.0 466.4 25
Close Eyes (d) 1.0 0.0 0.0 22.9 75 0.7 0.0 0.0 9.1 25 0.5 0.0 0.0 10.2 25 1.8 0.0 0.0 22.9 25
Cross Arms (d) 20.4 0.0 0.0 770.2 75 48.4 0.0 0.0 770.2 25 7.4 0.0 0.0 96.0 25 5.5 0.0 0.0 111.6 25
Cry (d) 2.1 0.0 0.0 149.1 75 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 25 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 25 6.4 0.0 0.0 149.1 25
General Fidget (d) 759.4 706.4 0.0 2555.4 75 789.2 724.9 0.0 2555.4 25 859.7 760.9 0.0 1757.4 25 629.2 520.9 15.3 1700.6 25
General Scratch (d) 5.7 0.0 0.0 101.2 75 6.7 3.1 0.0 35.5 25 3.6 0.0 0.0 32.1 25 6.6 0.0 0.0 101.2 25
Grimace/Facial Tension (d)* 25.6 5.3 0.0 548.9 75 43.3 11.0a 0.0 548.9 25 25.8 7.9aa 0.0 233.0 25 7.7 1.7b 0.0 61.4 25
Head Toward Dog (d)* 221.8 135.6 0.0 3085.0 75 350.4 343.3a 0.0 1176.3 25 302.5 169.9a 0.0 3085.0 25 12.6 10.2b 2.2 29.9 25
Internal Tongue Movement (d) 138.5 53.5 0.0 1441.7 75 154.2 68.9 0.0 867.7 25 144.6 44.0 0.0 656.2 25 116.7 43.9 0.4 1441.7 25
Laugh (c)* 9.9 5.0 0.0 57.0 75 13.8 10.0a 0.0 57.0 25 11.8 8.0a 0.0 47.0 25 4.2 1.0b 0.0 24.0 25
Lean Towards Dog (d)* 45.6 3.2 0.0 1097.5 75 82.8 15.1b 0.0 1097.5 25 54.1 10.6a 0.0 329.9 25 0.0 0.0c 0.0 0.0 25
Play with Hair (d) 80.2 33.9 0.0 801.6 75 109.7 38.8 0.0 801.6 25 55.9 36.0 0.0 378.0 25 75.0 26.4 0.0 697.1 25
Reposition (c) 89.9 87.0 0.0 214.0 75 97.1 95.0 0.0 212.0 25 96.0 100.0 0.0 214.0 25 76.5 67.0 23.0 162.0 25
Sit on Hands (d) 4.3 0.0 0.0 196.2 75 9.5 0.0 0.0 196.2 25 3.4 0.0 0.0 56.1 25 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 25
Smile (c)* 22.5 19.0 0.0 69.0 75 26.2 22.0a 0.0 62.0 25 25.5 22.0a,b 0.0 69.0 25 15.9 17.0b 1.0 33.0 25
Stretch (c) 3.3 2.0 0.0 40.0 75 4.2 2.0 0.0 40.0 25 2.3 1.0 0.0 14.0 25 3.4 2.0 0.0 21.0 25
Talk to Dog (d)* 2.2 0.0 0.0 41.0 75 3.0 1.4a 0.0 25.6 25 3.7 1.1a 0.0 41.0 25 0.0 0.0b 0.0 0.0 25
Talk to Experimenter (d) 13.8 8.8 0.0 154.3 75 9.4 7.0 0.0 23.9 25 16.0 7.6 0.0 154.3 25 16.1 10.3 2.5 51.3 25
Talk to Handler (d) 14.2 2.2 0.0 117.5 75 26.0 17.9 0.0 117.5 25 16.5 4.2 0.0 103.9 25 0.1 0.0 0.0 2.0 25
Talk to Self (d) 0.5 0.0 0.0 37.9 75 0.1 0.0 0.0 2.8 25 1.5 0.0 0.0 37.9 25 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 25
Touch Dog (d)* 407.8 20.6 0.0 2856.8 75 1153.3 998.7b 47.4 2856.8 25 32.6 9.6a 0.0 268.3 25 37.5 0.0a 0.0 380.9 25
Touch Face (c) 27.5 26.0 0.0 93.0 75 28.6 29.0 0.0 65.0 25 29.8 21.0 0.0 93.0 25 24.2 18.0 1.0 63.0 25
Yawn (c) 3.6 2.0 0.0 16.0 75 4.8 4.0 0.0 16.0 25 2.1 2.0 0.0 10.0 25 4.0 1.0 0.0 16.0 25
Notes. Medians with different superscript numbers significant differently at p < .05. *p < .05.
Table 4. Descriptive Statistics of Observed Therapy Dog Behavior by Experimental Group.
Table 4. Descriptive Statistics of Observed Therapy Dog Behavior by Experimental Group.
Behavior Total Social+ Physical Social
Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N
Bark (c) 0.1 0.0 0.0 2.0 75 0.1 0.0 0.0 2.0 25 0.1 0.0 0.0 2.0 25
Circle (d)* 0.9 0.0 0.0 17.3 75 0.5 0.0 0.0 7.6 25 2.3 0.0 0.0 17.3 25
Ears Down (d) 0.1 0.0 0.0 4.5 75 0.2 0.0 0.0 4.5 25 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 25
Ears Twitch (c)* 2.7 1.0 0.0 30.0 75 5.5 3.0 0.0 30.0 25 2.6 2.0 0.0 14.0 25
Groom (d) 11.9 0.0 0.0 195.8 75 17.4 0.0 0.0 195.8 25 18.4 3.5 0.0 119.6 25
Head toward Participant (d) 24.8 10.2 0.0 210.1 75 44.1 27.2 7.4 210.1 25 30.3 17.7 0.0 104.9 25
Jerk/Twitch (c) 1.7 0.0 0.0 20.0 75 2.3 0.0 0.0 20.0 25 2.7 0.0 0.0 18.0 25
Lay Alert (d) 371.0 188.9 0.0 2989.6 75 327.7 273.4 72.9 968.7 25 785.2 352.7 64.6 2989.6 25
Lay Down (d)* 1565.7 1963.0 0.0 3032.0 75 2609.4 2807.4 1180.1 3032.0 25 2087.6 2455.1 0.0 2968.5 25
Lick Object (d) 1.4 0.0 0.0 96.2 75 0.2 0.0 0.0 4.2 25 3.9 0.0 0.0 96.2 25
Lift Paw (d) 2.5 0.0 0.0 99.3 75 2.3 0.0 0.0 49.4 25 5.2 0.0 0.0 99.3 25
Near Participant (d)* 126.5 0.0 0.0 2871.7 75 360.2 0.0 0.0 2871.7 25 19.2 14.8 0.0 98.0 25
Pant (d) 27.4 0.0 0.0 1182.7 75 80.3 0.0 0.0 1182.7 25 2.0 0.0 0.0 27.5 25
Paw at Person (c) 0.6 0.0 0.0 10.0 75 0.7 0.0 0.0 10.0 25 1.0 0.0 0.0 10.0 25
Paws on Participant (d) 0.8 0.0 0.0 22.1 75 0.2 0.0 0.0 4.2 25 2.2 0.0 0.0 22.1 25
Scratch at Object/Floor (d)* 1.8 0.0 0.0 43.2 75 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 25 5.5 0.0 0.0 43.2 25
Shake Head (c) 0.3 0.0 0.0 4.0 75 0.7 0.0 0.0 4.0 25 0.4 0.0 0.0 3.0 25
Shake Off (d) 0.7 0.0 0.0 14.0 75 1.3 0.0 0.0 14.0 25 1.0 0.0 0.0 4.4 25
Shift Head (c)* 7.8 3.0 0.0 81.0 75 17.4 14.0 0.0 81.0 25 6.1 5.0 0.0 31.0 25
Shift Position (c) 36.1 25.0 0.0 212.0 75 55.8 40.0 19.0 212.0 25 52.6 41.0 1.0 121.0 25
Sit (d) 91.2 2.2 0.0 1198.5 75 167.8 8.7 0.0 1198.5 25 105.7 35.2 0.0 542.2 25
Sniff Object/Floor (d)* 9.6 0.0 0.0 96.6 75 8.6 2.4 0.0 96.6 25 20.1 11.3 0.0 80.9 25
Sniff Participant (d)* 3.5 0.0 0.0 46.1 75 4.5 1.0 0.0 46.1 25 5.9 3.6 0.0 28.5 25
Stand (d)* 66.9 2.9 0.0 965.0 75 74.5 2.9 0.0 947.4 25 126.2 64.8 11.3 965.0 25
Stretch (d)* 1.7 0.0 0.0 23.8 75 1.0 0.0 0.0 10.3 25 4.1 0.0 0.0 23.8 25
Wag Tail (d)* 14.1 0.0 0.0 201.4 75 14.3 0.0 0.0 201.4 25 28.0 15.2 0.0 124.8 25
Walk (d)* 10.7 0.0 0.0 101.6 75 1.4 0.0 0.0 16.0 25 30.7 26.4 1.8 101.6 25
Yawn (c) 1.3 0.0 0.0 11.0 75 2.5 2.0 0.0 11.0 25 1.4 1.0 0.0 6.0 25
Notes. Medians with different superscript numbers significant differently at p < .05. *p < .05.
Table 5. Significant Correlations between Participant and Therapy Dog Behavior, Social + Physical Group Only (n = 25).
Table 5. Significant Correlations between Participant and Therapy Dog Behavior, Social + Physical Group Only (n = 25).
Spearman’s rho Coefficient Ranges for Human-Dog Behavior Pairings
+/-.30 -- +/-49 +/-.50 -- +/-.79
Human cross arms-Dog bark
Human cross arms-Dog lay alert
Human cross arms -Dog stand
Human cross arms-Dog wag tail
Human cross arms -Dog lay down (neg)
Human grimace-Dog circle
Human grimace-Dog near participant (neg)
Human internal tongue movement-Dog shake head (neg)
Human bite lip-Dog jerk/twitch
Human general scratch-Dog lay alert
Human general scratch-Dog shift position
Human general scratch-Dog sniff floor/object
Human general scratch-Dog wag tail
Human play with hair-Dog lick floor/object
Human yawn-Dog head to participant (neg)
Human yawn-Dog near participant (neg)
Human smile-Dog near participant (neg)
Human laugh-Dog shake off (neg)
Human laugh-Dog shift within position (neg)
Human laugh-Dog sniff participant (neg)
Human laugh-Dog stand (neg)
Human talk to experimenter-Dog lay alert
Human talk to experimenter-Dog lay down (neg)
Human talk to experimenter-Dog shake head (neg)
Human talk to dog-Dog lay alert
Human touch dog-Dog shift head
Human touch dog-Dog ears twitch (neg)
Human touch dog-Dog paw at person (neg)
Human cross arms-Dog stretch
Human internal tongue movement-Dog sniff object/floor
Human internal tongue movement-Dog near participant (neg)
Human general scratch-Dog groom
Human yawn-Dog walk (neg)
Human laugh-Dog near participant (neg)
Human laugh-Dog walk (neg)
Human touch dog-Dog ear twitch (neg)
Human touch dog-Dog near participant (neg)
Human touch dog-Dog stand (neg)
Notes. Groupings by Spearman’s rho coefficients for human-dog behavior analysis; significant correlations (p < .05) only. A total of 720 correlations were examined, Tables S1 and S2 present results for all participants and just those in the social + physical condition.
Table 6. Rotated Component Matrix for PCA of Observed Participant Standardized Behaviors (N = 75).
Table 6. Rotated Component Matrix for PCA of Observed Participant Standardized Behaviors (N = 75).
Standardized Behavior Component
1 2 3
Talk to Self (d) .93 .07 -.01
Talk to Experimenter (d) .89 .23 -.06
Talk to Dog (d) .87 -.01 .25
Bite Nails (d) -.12 .85 .10
Play with Hair (d) .19 .77 -.11
Touch Face (c) .46 .61 .09
Touch Dog (d) -.10 -.02 .82
Smile (c) .24 .06 .75
Eigenvalue 3.1 1.5 1.3
% of variance 34.4% 21.8% 16.4%
Cronbach’s alpha .91 .65 .41
Notes. Varimax rotation of standardized behavior data.
Table 7. Rotated Component Matrix for PCA of Observed Therapy Dog Standardized Behaviors (N = 50).
Table 7. Rotated Component Matrix for PCA of Observed Therapy Dog Standardized Behaviors (N = 50).
Standardized Behavior Component
1 2 3
Wag (d) .90 .05 .07
Groom (d) .89 -.03 .02
Stretch (d) .86 -.12 .10
Shift Position (c) .73 .53 .22
Lay Alert (d) .54 .20 -.09
Paw at Person (c) -.03 .85 -.12
Ear Twitch (c) .11 .79 .09
Head toward Participant (d) .04 .77 .31
Near Participant (d) -.13 .31 .83
Jerk/Twitch (c) .49 -.16 .62
Eigenvalue 3.7 2.3 1.0
% of variance 34.5% 24.0% 12.5%
Cronbach’s alpha .86 .76 30
Notes. Varimax rotation of standardized behavior data.
Table 8. Descriptive Statistics of Standardized Participant Behavior Components by Experimental Group.
Table 8. Descriptive Statistics of Standardized Participant Behavior Components by Experimental Group.
Behavior Component Total Social + physical Social Control
Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N Mean Med Min Max N
Participant
 Component 1 0.00 -0.57 -1.20 22.22 75 -0.19 -0.45 -1.20 4.38 25 0.58 -0.56 -1.20 22.22 25 -0.39 -0.68 -1.07 1.39 25
 Component 2 0.00 -0.68 -2.30 9.32 75 0.17 -0.58 -2.30 7.57 25 -0.15 -0.88 -2.30 5.31 25 -0.02 -1.00 -2.21 9.32 25
 Component 3* 0.00 -0.42 -2.20 6.31 75 1.30 1.091 -0.86 6.31 25 -0.30 -0.602 -2.20 2.87 25 -1.00 -0.902 -2.16 0.21 25
Therapy Dog
 Component 1* 0.00 -0.28 -0.53 3.64 75 0.08 -0.261 -0.40 3.64 25 0.45 0.312 -0.46 2.51 25 -- -- -- -- --
 Component 2 0.00 -0.23 -0.50 4.26 75 0.39 -0.101 -0.35 4.26 25 0.12 -0.032 -0.50 2.12 25 -- -- -- -- --
 Component 3 0.00 -0.32 -0.33 4.14 75 0.32 -0.21 -0.33 4.14 25 0.01 -0.29 -0.33 1.83 25 -- -- -- -- --
Notes. Medians with different superscript numbers significantly different at p < .05. *p < .05.
Table 9. Participant and Therapy Dog Behavior Component Correlations for Total Sample (N = 75) and Social + Physical Group (n = 25).
Table 9. Participant and Therapy Dog Behavior Component Correlations for Total Sample (N = 75) and Social + Physical Group (n = 25).
Behavior Components Participant Therapy Dog
Component 1 Component 2 Component 3 Component 1 Component 2 Component 3
All Groups
Participants Component 1 1.00 .34* .35* .14 .15 .01
Component 2 1.00 .20 .03 .07 .10
Component 3 1.000 .40* .31* .14
Therapy Dogs Component 1 1.00 .70* .68*
Component 2 1.00 .64*
Component 3 1.00
Social + Physical Group
Participants Component 1 1.00 .01 .49* .27 .19 -.05
Component 2 1.00 -.05 -.07 .12 .12
Component 3 1.000 -.08 -.37 -.61*
Therapy Dogs Component 1 1.00 .53* .38
Component 2 1.00 .34
Component 3 1.00
Notes. Spearman’s rho coefficients for standardized behavior data. *p < .05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Copyright: This open access article is published under a Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 license, which permit the free download, distribution, and reuse, provided that the author and preprint are cited in any reuse.
Prerpints.org logo

Preprints.org is a free preprint server supported by MDPI in Basel, Switzerland.

Subscribe

© 2024 MDPI (Basel, Switzerland) unless otherwise stated